Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T05:22:46.223Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Infant language predicts fathers’ vocabulary in infant-directed speech

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 April 2019

Jean QUIGLEY*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland
Elizabeth NIXON
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland
*
*Corresponding author: School of Psychology, Áras an Phiarsaigh, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin 2, Ireland. E-mail: quigleyj@tcd.ie

Abstract

Research on sources of individual difference in parental Infant-Directed Speech (IDS) is limited and there is a particular lack of research on fathers’ compared to mothers’ speech. This study examined the predictive relations between infant characteristics and variability in paternal lexical diversity (LD) in dyadic free play with two-year-olds (M = 24.1 months, SD = 1.39, 35 girls). Ten minutes of interaction for sixty-four father–infant dyads were transcribed and multiple regression analyses were performed to examine the effects of a set of distal and proximal sources of infant influence on paternal LD. Fathers’ LD was predicted only by infant language, both standardised language scores and dynamic language measures, and was not predicted by infant age, gender, executive function, or temperament. Findings are discussed in the light of the complex interplay of factors contributing to variability in IDS and the infant's linguistic environment.

Type
Brief Research Reports
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albert, R. R., Schwade, J. A., & Goldstein, M. H. (2018). The social functions of babbling: acoustic and contextual characteristics that facilitate maternal responsiveness. Developmental Science, 21(5), e12641. doi:10.1111/desc.12641CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayoub, M., Briley, D. A., Grotzinger, A., Patterson, M. W., Engelhardt, L. E., Tackett, J. L., Paige Harden, K. P., & Tucker-Drob, M. (2018). Genetic and environmental associations between child personality and parenting. Social Psychological & Personality Science, 111. doi:10.1177/1948550618784890Google ScholarPubMed
Barachetti, C., & Lavelli, M. (2010). Preschoolers’ communicative functions during shared book reading with mothers and fathers. Early Education & Development, 21, 595613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayley, N. (2005). Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development 3rd Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bergelson, E., Amatuni, A., Dailey, S., & Koorathota, S. (2019). Day by day, hour by hour: naturalistic language input to infants. Developmental Science, 22(1), e12715. doi:10.1111/desc.12715CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bingham, G. E., Kwon, K. A., & Jeon, H. J. (2013). Examining relations among mothers’, fathers’ and children's language use in a dyadic and triadic context. Early Child Development & Care, 183, 394414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cabrera, M., Fitzgerald, H., Bradley, R. H., & Roggman, L. A. (2014). The ecology of father–child relationships: an expanded model. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 6, 336–54.Google Scholar
Che, E. S, Brooks, P. J., Alarcon, M. F., Yannaco, F. D., & Donnelly, S. (2018) Assessing the impact of conversational overlap in content on child language growth. Journal of Child Language, 45, 7296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cristia, A. (2013). Input to language: the phonetics and perception of infant-directed speech. Language and Linguistics Compass, 7, 157–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dale, P. S., Tosto, M. G., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., & Plomin, R. (2015) Why does parental language input style predict language development? A twin study of gene–environment correlation. Journal of Communication Disorders, 57, 106–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusaroli, R., Weed, E., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. (2019). Hearing me hearing you: reciprocal effects between child and parent language in autism and typical development. Cognition, 183, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, A., Farago, T., Galambos, A., & Topal, J. (2017). Differential effects of speech situations on mothers’ and fathers’ infant-directed and dog-directed speech: an acoustic analysis. Scientific Reports, 7, 13739. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-13883-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gioia, G. A., Espy, K. A., & Isquith, P. K. (2003). Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Preschool Version. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Gros-Louis, J., Goldstein, M. H., West, M. J., & King, A. P. (2006) Mothers provide differential feedback to infants’ prelinguistic sounds. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 112–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hadley, P. A., Rispoli, M., & Holt, J. K. (2017). Input subject diversity accelerates the growth of tense and agreement: indirect benefits from a parent-implemented intervention. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60, 2619–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74, 1368–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26, 5588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, N., Hadley, P. A., & Rispoli, M. (2017). Diversity matters: parent input predicts toddler verb production. Journal of Child Language, 44, 6386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, G., & Rowland, C. F. (2017). Diversity not quantity in caregiver speech: using computational modeling to isolate the effects of the quantity and the diversity of the input on vocabulary growth. Cognitive Psychology, 98, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., Donnelly, S., & Christiansen, M. H. (2018). Individual differences in language acquisition and processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 154–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, S., & Kochanska, G. (2012). Child temperament moderates effects of parent–child mutuality on self-regulation: a relationship-based path for emotionally negative infants. Child Development, 83, 1275–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuchirko, Y. (2017). On differences and deficits: a critique of the theoretical and methodological underpinnings of the word gap. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, doi:10.1177/1468798417747029Google Scholar
Kucirkova, N., Dale, P. S., & Sylva, K. (2018). Parents reading with their 10-month-old babies: key predictors for high-quality reading styles. Early Child Development and Care, 188, 195207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kwon, K., Bingham, G. E., Lewsader, G., Jeon, H., & Elicker, J. (2013). Structured task versus free play: the influence of social context on parenting quality, toddlers’ engagement with parents and play behaviors, and parent–toddler language use. Child & Youth Care Forum, 42, 207–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovas, G. S. (2011). Gender and patterns of language development in mother–toddler and father–toddler dyads. First Language, 31, 83108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leech, K. A., Salo, V. C., Rowe, M. L., & Cabrera, N. J. (2013). Father input and child vocabulary development: the importance of Wh-questions and clarification requests. Seminars in Speech & Lanuage, 34, 249–59.Google ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: tools for analyzing talk – 3rd Edition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Mahr, T., & Edwards, J. (2018). Using language input and lexical processing to predict vocabulary size. Developmental Science, 21(6), e12685. doi:10.1111/desc.12685CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matte-Gagné, C., & Bernier, A. (2011). Prospective relations between maternal autonomy support and child executive functioning: investigating the mediating role of child language ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 611–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montag, J. L., Jones, M. N., & Smith, L. B. (2018). Quantity and diversity: stimulating early word learning environments. Cognitive Science, 42(S2), 375412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, R., Rowe, M., & Bernstein Ratner, N. (2016). Input and uptake at 7 months predicts toddler vocabulary: the role of child-directed speech and infant processing skills in language development. Journal of Child Language, 43, 1158–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language development. Annual Review of Linguistics, 3, 285308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76, 763–82.Google ScholarPubMed
Pancosfar, N., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2006). Mother and father language input to young children: contributions to later language development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27, 571–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pancosfar, N., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2010). Fathers’ early contributions to children's language development in families from low-income rural communities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 450–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: the Early Childhood Behaviour Questionnaire. Infant Behaviour & Development, 29, 386401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quigley, J., Nixon, E., & Lawson, S. (in press) Exploring the association of infant receptive language and pitch variability in fathers’ infant directed speech. Journal of Child Language.Google Scholar
Ramirez-Esparza, N., Garcia-Sieira, A., & Kuhl, P. (2014). Look who's talking: speech style and social context in language input to infants are linked to concurrent and future speech development. Developmental Science, 17(6), 880–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, C. R., Nulty, K. L., Betancourt, M. A., & DeThorne, L. S. (2015). Causal effects on child language development: a review of studies in communication sciences and disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders, 57, 315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83, 1762–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryckebusch, C., & Marcos, H. (2004). Speech acts, social context and parent–toddler play between the ages of 1;5 and 2;3. Journal of Pragmatics, 36, 883–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salley, B. J., & Dixon, W. E. (2007). Temperamental and joint attentional predictors of language development. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 53, 131–54.Google ScholarPubMed
Salo, V., Rowe, M., Leech, K., & Cabrera, N. (2016). Low-income fathers’ speech to toddlers during book reading versus toy play. Journal of Child Language, 43, 1385–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwab, J. F., & Lew-Williams, C. (2016a). Language learning, socioeconomic status, and child-directed speech. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive science, 7(4), 264–75.Google Scholar
Schwab, J. F., & Lew-Williams, C. (2016b). Repetition across successive sentences facilitates young children's word learning. Developmental Psychology, 52, 879–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwab, J. F., Rowe, N., Cabrera, N., & Lew-Williams, C. (2018). Fathers’ repetition of words is coupled with children's vocabularies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 437–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smolak, L., & Weinraub, M. (1983). Maternal speech: Strategy or response? Journal of Child Language 10, 369–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinelli, M., Fasolo, M., Shah, P., Genovese, G., & Aureli, T. (2018). The influence of early temperament on language development: the moderating role of maternal input. Frontiers in Psychology, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01527CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Baumwell, L. B., & Cabrera, N. J. (2013). Fathers’ role in children's language development. In Cabrera, N. J. & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of father involvement: multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 135–50). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Baumwell, L. B., & Cristofaro, T. (2012). Parent–child conversations during play. First Language, 32, 413–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Briggs, R. D., McClowry, S. G., & Snow, D. L. (2009). Maternal control and sensitivity, child gender, and maternal education in relation to children's behavioral outcomes in African American families. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, 321–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Custode, S., Kuchirko, Y., Escobar, K., & Lo, T. (2018). Routine language: speech directed to infants during home activities. Child Development, doi:10.1111/cdev.13089Google ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y., Luo, R., Escobar, K., & Bornstein, M. H. (2017). Power in methods: language to infants in structured and naturalistic contexts. Developmental Science, 20(6), e12456. doi:10.1111/desc.12456CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S, Kuchirko, Y., & Suh, D. D. (2018). Taking center stage: infants’ active role in language learning. In Saylor, M. M. & Ganea, P. A. (Eds.), Active learning from infancy to childhood (pp. 3953). Cham: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Conti-Ramsden, G., & Ewert, B. (1990). Young children's conversations with their mothers and fathers: differences in breakdown and repair. Journal of Child Language, 17, 115–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Topping, K., Dekhinet, R., & Zeedyk, S. (2013). Parent–infant interaction and children's language development. Educational Psychology, 33, 391426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vallotton, C., & Ayoub, C. (2011). Use your words: the role of language in the development of toddlers’ self-regulation. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 169–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2008a). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 4th Edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2008b). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 4th Edition: technical and interpretive manual. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24, 2143–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed