Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T14:44:43.274Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Multilingual toddlers’ vocabulary development in two languages: Comparing bilinguals and trilinguals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 February 2021

Stephanie L. CÔTÉ
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Canada Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music, Concordia University, Canada
Ana Maria GONZALEZ-BARRERO*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Canada Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music, Concordia University, Canada
Krista BYERS-HEINLEIN
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Canada Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music, Concordia University, Canada
*
Address for correspondence: Ana Maria Gonzalez-Barrero, Concordia University, Department of Psychology, 7141 Sherbrooke St. West Montreal, Québec, Canada, H4B 1R6 E-mail: anamaria.gonzalezbarrero@mail.concordia.ca

Abstract

Many children grow up hearing multiple languages, learning words in each. How does the number of languages being learned affect multilinguals’ vocabulary development? In a pre-registered study, we compared productive vocabularies of bilingual (n = 170) and trilingual (n = 20) toddlers aged 17–33 months growing up in a bilingual community where both French and English are spoken. We hypothesized that because trilinguals have reduced input in French and English due to time spent hearing their third language, they would have smaller French–English vocabulary sizes than bilinguals. Trilinguals produced on average 2/3 of the number of words in these languages that bilinguals did: however, this difference was not statistically robust due to large levels of variability. Follow-up analyses did, however, indicate a relationship between input quantity and vocabulary size. Our results indicate that similar factors contribute to vocabulary development across toddlers regardless of the number of languages being acquired.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albin, A., & Gershkoff-Stowe, L. (2016). Rapid word learning in trilingual children: One spurt or three? In Stinger, D. et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 13th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (pp. 1430). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Andersson, I., Gauding, J., Graca, A., Holm, K., Öhlin, L., Marklund, U., & Ericsson, A. (2011). Productive vocabulary size development in children aged 18–24 months – Gender differences. Proceedings from Fonetik 2011: Speech, Music and Hearing: Quarterly Progress and Status Report, 51, 109112.Google Scholar
Arnaus Gil, L., Müller, N., Sette, N., & Hüppop, M. (2020). Active bi-and trilingualism and its influencing factors. International Multilingual Research Journal, 1-22. https://doi.org/10.1080/19313152.2020.1753964Google Scholar
Barnes, J., & Garcia, I. (2013). Vocabulary growth and composition in monolingual and bilingual Basque infants and toddlers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17(3), 357374. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006912438992CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Reilly, J., & Hartung, J. (1994). Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 21(1), 85123. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008680CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bee, H. L., Van Egeren, L. F., Pytkowicz Streissguth, A., Nyman, B. A., & Leckie, M. S. (1969). Social class differences in maternal teaching strategies and speech patterns. Developmental Psychology, 1(6, Pt.1), 726734. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0028257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bleses, D., Makransky, G., Dale, P. S., Højen, A., & Ari, B. A. (2016). Early productive vocabulary predicts academic achievement 10 years later. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(6), 14611476. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716416000060CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). Evidence of early language discrimination abilities in infants from bilingual environments. Infancy, 2(1), 2949. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0201_3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byers-Heinlein, K. (2013). Parental language mixing: Its measurement and the relation of mixed input to young bilingual children's vocabulary size. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(1), 3248. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Schott, E., Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., Dubé, D., Jardak, A., Laoun-Rubenstein, A., Mastroberardino, M., Morin-Lessard, E., Iliaei, S. P., Salama-Siroishka, N., & Tamayo, M. P. (2019). MAPLE: A multilingual approach to parent language estimates. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/r2q3uGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2009). Monolingual, bilingual, trilingual: Infants’ language experience influences the development of a word-learning heuristic. Developmental Science, 12(5), 815823. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00902.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattani, A., Abbot-Smith, K., Farag, R., Krott, A., Arreckx, F., Dennis, I., & Floccia, C. (2014). How much exposure to English is necessary for a bilingual toddler to perform like a monolingual peer in language tests?: Language exposure and bilingual screening. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49(6), 649671. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12082CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chevalier, S. (2015). Trilingual language acquisition: Contextual factors influencing active trilingualism in early childhood. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collier, V. P., & Thomas, W. P. (2004). The astounding success of dual language immersion for all. NABE Journal and Research, 2(1), 120.Google Scholar
Core, C., Hoff, E., Rumiche, R., & Señor, M. (2013). Total and conceptual vocabulary in Spanish-English bilinguals from 22 to 30 months: Implications for assessment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 56(5), 16371649. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/11-0044)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, P. S. (1991). The validity of a parent report measure of vocabulary and syntax at 24 months. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 34(3), 565571. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3403.565CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
David, A., & Wei, L. (2008). Individual differences in the lexical development of French–English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11(5), 598618. https://doi.org/10.2167/beb478.0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A. (1995). Bilingual language acquisition. In Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, B. (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 219250). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2004). Trilingual input and children's language use in trilingual families in Flanders. In Hoffmann, C. & Ytsma, J. (Eds.), Trilingualism in the individual, family and society (pp. 118138). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2007). Parental language input patterns and children's bilingual use. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(3), 411424. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A., Bornstein, M. H., & Putnick, D. L. (2014). A bilingual–monolingual comparison of young children's vocabulary size: Evidence from comprehension and production. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(6), 11891211. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000744CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234248. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floccia, C., Sambrook, T. D., Delle Luche, C., Kwok, R., Goslin, J., White, L., Cattani, A., Sullivan, E., Abbot-Smith, K., Krott, A., Mills, D., Rowland, C., Gervain, J., & Plunkett, K. (2018). Vocabulary of 2-year-olds learning English and an additional language: Norms and effects of linguistic distance. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 83(1), 729. https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12348CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, M. C., Braginsky, M., Marchman, V. A., & Yurovsky, D. (in press). Variability and consistency in early language learning: The wordbank project. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., & Thomas, E. M. (2009). Bilingual first-language development: Dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(2), 213237. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909004015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., & Nadig, A. (2018). Bilingual children with autism spectrum disorders: The impact of amount of language exposure on vocabulary and morphological skills at school age. Autism Research, 11(12), 16671678. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., Schott, E., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2020). Bilingual adjusted vocabulary: A developmentally-informed bilingual vocabulary measure. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/x7s4uCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, J. C., Dale, P. S., & Li, P. (2008). Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 35(03), 515531. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000907008641CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F. (2010). Bilingual: Life and Reality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. https://doi.org/10.4159/9780674056459CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamers, J. F., & Blanc, M. H. A. (2000). Bilinguality and bilingualism (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, B. B., & Klopfer, S. O. (2006). Optimal full matching and related designs via network flows. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics, 15(3), 609627. JSTOR. https://doi.org/10.1198/106186006X137047CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74(5), 13681378. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00612CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39(1), 127. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000910000759CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E., & Tian, C. (2005). Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. Journal of Communication Disorders, 38(4), 271278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2005.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E., Welsh, S., Place, S., & Ribot, K. M. (2014). Properties of dual language input that shape bilingual development and properties of environments that shape dual language input. In Grüter, T. & Paradis, J. (Eds.), Input and experience in bilingual development (pp. 119140). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.13.07hofGoogle Scholar
Hoffmann, C. (2001). Towards a description of trilingual competence. International Journal of Bilingualism, 5(1), 117. https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069010050010101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236248. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104(1), 5369. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.104.1.53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, K., & Fogle, N. (2006). Raising bilingual children: Common parental concerns and current research. Center for Applied Linguistics. http://www.cal.org/resource-center/resource-archive/digestsGoogle Scholar
Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11(3), F916. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00671.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLeod, S., Harrison, L. J., Whiteford, C., & Walker, S. (2016). Multilingualism and speech-language competence in early childhood: Impact on academic and social-emotional outcomes at school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 34, 5366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.08.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mieszkowska, K., Łuniewska, M., Kołak, J., Kacprzak, A., Wodniecka, Z., & Haman, E. (2017). Home language will not take care of itself: Vocabulary knowledge in trilingual children in the United Kingdom. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01358CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montanari, S. (2010). Translation equivalents and the emergence of multiple lexicons in early trilingual development. First Language, 30(1), 102125. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723709350528CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K. (2010). All-day recordings to investigate vocabulary development: A case study of a trilingual toddler. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 31(4), 213222. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740109358628CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. (2002). Language and literacy in bilingual children. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76(4), 763782. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00498-i1Google ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J. (2007). Bilingual children with specific language impairment: Theoretical and applied issues. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(3), 551564. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Genesee, F., & Crago, M. B. (2011). Dual language development and disorders (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18(01), 4158. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400009863CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43(1), 93120. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1993.tb00174.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E. (2011). Properties of dual language exposure that influence two-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82(6), 18341849. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01660.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potgieter, A. P. (2016). Lexical and grammatical development in trilingual speakers if ixiXhosa, English and Afrikaans. South African Journal of Communication Disorders, 63(2), a141. http://doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v63i2.141CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qi, C. H., Kaiser, A. P., Milan, S., & Hancock, H. (2006). Language performance of low-income African American and European American preschool children on the PPVT–III. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 37(1), 516. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2006/002)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quay, S. (2011). Trilingual toddlers at daycare centres: The role of caregivers and peers in language development. International Journal of Multilingualism, 8(1), 2241. https://doi.org/10.1080/14790711003671853CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Rescorla, L., & Achenbach, T. M. (2002). Use of the Language Development Survey (LDS) in a national probability sample of children 18 to 35 months old. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(4), 733743. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/059)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheele, A. F., Leseman, P. P. M., & Mayo, A. Y. (2010). The home language environment of monolingual and bilingual children and their language proficiency. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31(1), 117140. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716409990191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schott, E., Kremin, L. V., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2019). Child Bi- and Multilingualism in the Home in Canada: Rates and Language Pairs [Poster]. International Symposium on Bilingualism 12, Edmonton, Canada. https://files.osf.io/v1/resources/2gzfw/providers/osfstorage/5d13d2a6e200da00197266cd?pid=2gzfw&direct&public_file=True&version=1&mode=render&action=downloadGoogle Scholar
Song, L., Spier, E. T., & Tamis-Lemonda, C. S. (2014). Reciprocal influences between maternal language and children's language and cognitive development in low-income families. Journal of Child Language, 41(2), 305326. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000912000700CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statistics Canada. (2017). Montréal [Census metropolitan area], Quebec and Nova Scotia [Province] (Table 2016 Census. Statistics Canada Catalogue no. 98-316-X2016001). https://www12.statcan.gc.ca/census-recensement/2016/dp-pd/prof/index.cfm?Lang=EGoogle Scholar
Surrain, S., & Luk, G. (2019). Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(2), 401415. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000682CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.Google Scholar
Thordardottir, E. (2011). The relationship between bilingual exposure and vocabulary development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 426445. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006911403202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thordardottir, E., Rothenberg, A., Rivard, M.-E., & Naves, R. (2006). Bilingual assessment: Can overall proficiency be estimated from separate measurement of two languages? Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 4(1), 121. https://doi.org/10.1080/14769670500215647CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trudeau, N., Frank, H., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (1999). Une adaptation en français Québécois du MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory. La revue d'orthophonie et d'audiologie, 23(2), 6173. https://cjslpa.ca/detail.php?ID=81&lang=frGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2013a). Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(01), 86110. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2013b). Current issues in multilingual first language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 33, 2150. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190513000044CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Côté et al. supplementary material

Côté et al. supplementary material

Download Côté et al. supplementary material(File)
File 59.1 KB