Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T06:33:33.506Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

‘Not by the chair of my hinny hin hin’: some general properties of slips of the tongue in young children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Jeri J. Jaeger*
Affiliation:
State University of New York at Buffalo
*
Department of Linguistics, 685 Baldy Hall, State University of New York at Buffalo, Buffalo, NY 14260, USA.

Abstract

Young children's slips of the tongue are a rich source of information about developing language processing and storage mechanisms. This paper presents an analysis of 907 slips from 32 children, ages 1;4–6;0, collected in naturalistic settings. It is found that these children make most of the same types and proportions of slips as adults: phonological errors outnumber lexical, which exceed phrasal. In phonological errors, anticipations are most common, followed by perseverations and exchanges; children make more completed anticipations and exchanges than adults, probably due to less mature self-monitoring. Like adults, children make more substitutions than additions or omissions. Children's slips support a theory of speech planning in which propositional, syntactic, intonational, content word, function word and phonological levels have somewhat independent status; however, there is little evidence for a derivational morphology level at this age.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1992

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

An earlier version of this paper was presented at the Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, San Francisco, December 1987. I would like to thank LouAnn Gerken, Karin Michelson, Robert Van Valin and David Wilkins for their input to this paper. This research was partly supported by a U. C. Davis Faculty Research Grant, and much of the data was collected while the author was on the faculty at U. C. Davis.

References

REFERENCES

Aitchison, J. & Straf, M. (1982). Lexical storage and retrieval: a developing skill? In Cutler, A. (ed.), Slips of the tongue and language production. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Berg, T. (1987). A cross-linguistic comparison of slips of the tongue. Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Cutler, A. (1980). Errors of stress and intonation. In Fromkin, V. (ed.), Errors in linguistic performance; slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cutler, A. (1982). The reliability of speech error data. In Cutler, A. (ed.), Slips of the tongue and language production. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review 93, 283321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fromkin, V. (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language 47, 2752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrett, M. F. (1975). The analysis of sentence production. In Bower, G. H. (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 9. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Garrett, M. F. (1980). Levels of processing in sentence production. In Butterworth, B. (ed.), Language production, Vol. 1. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gerken, L. (in press). A slip-of-the-tongue approach to language development. In Charles-Luce, J., Luce, P. & Sawusch, J. (eds), Spoken language: perception, production and development. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Goldfield, B. A. & Snow, C. E. (1989). Individual differences in language acquisition. In Berko Gleason, J. (ed.), The development of language (2nd edn). Columbus, OH: Merrill.Google Scholar
Harley, T. A. (1984). A critique of top-down independent level models of speech production: evidence from non-plan-internal speech errors. Cognitive Science 8, 191219.Google Scholar
Jaeger, J. J. (1986). On the acquisition of abstract representations for English vowels. Phonology Yearbook 3, 7195.Google Scholar
Jaeger, J. J. & Wilkins, D. P. (in press). Some lexical and semantic properties of young children's slips of the tongue. In K. Nelson (ed.), Children's Language, Vol. X.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1983). Monitoring and self-repair in speech. Cognition 14, 41104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Specking; from intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Meringer, R. (1908). Aus dem Leben der Sprache: Versprechen, Kindersprache, Nachahmungstrieb. Berlin: Behrs Verlag.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nooteboom, S. (1973). The tongue slips into patterns. In Fromkin, V. (ed.), Speech errors as linguistic evidence. Hawthorne, NY: Mouton.Google Scholar
Nooteboom, S. (1980). Speaking and unspeaking: detection and correction of phonological and lexical errors in spontaneous speech. In Fromkin, V. (ed.), Errors in linguistic performance; slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (1983). Sublexical units and suprasegmental structure in speech production planning. In MacNeilage, P. F. (ed.), The production of speech. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. & Klatt, D. H. (1979). The limited use of distinctive features and markedness in speech production: evidence from speech error data. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18, 4155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stemberger, J. P. (1989). Speech errors in early child language production. Journal of Memory and Language 28, 164–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tent, J. & Clark, J. E. (1980). An experimental investigation into the perception of slips of the tongue. Journal of Phonetics 8, 317–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1981). Phonology and the development of the lexicon: evidence from children's errors. Journal of Child Language 8, 239–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warren, H. (1986). Slips of the tongue in very young children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 11, 569–98.Google Scholar
Wijnen, F. (1990). The development of sentence planning. Journal of Child Language 17, 651–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed