Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T21:40:20.517Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parents’ gesture adaptations to children with autism spectrum disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 October 2019

Hanako YOSHIDA*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Houston, USA
Paul CIRINO
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Houston, USA
Sarah S. MIRE
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological, Health, and Learning Sciences, University of Houston, USA
Joseph M. BURLING
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Ohio State University, USA
Sunbok LEE
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Houston, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Fred J. Heyne Building, 3695 Cullen Blvd., Houston, TX77204, USA. E-mail: yoshida@uh.edu

Abstract

The present study focused on parents’ social cue use in relation to young children's attention. Participants were ten parent–child dyads; all children were 36 to 60 months old and were either typically developing (TD) or were diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Children wore a head-mounted camera that recorded the proximate child view while their parent played with them. The study compared the following between the TD and ASD groups: (a) frequency of parent's gesture use; (b) parents’ monitoring of their child's face; and (c) how children looked at parents’ gestures. Results from Bayesian estimation indicated that, compared to the TD group, parents of children with ASD produced more gestures, more closely monitored their children's faces, and provided more scaffolding for their children's visual experiences. Our findings suggest the importance of further investigating parents’ visual and gestural scaffolding as a potential developmental mechanism for children's early learning, including for children with ASD.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

APA (American Psychiatric Association) (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
APA (American Psychiatric Association) (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Aslin, R. N. (2009). How infants view natural scenes gathered from a head-mounted camera. Optometry and Vision Science, 86(6), 561–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bacon, A.L., Fein, D., Morris, R., Waterhouse, L., & Allen, D. (1998). The responses of autistic children to the distress of others. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 129–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interaction. Child Development, 55(4), 1278–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, J. K., Messinger, D. S., Lyons, K. K., & Grantz, C. J. (2010). A pilot study of maternal sensitivity in the context of emergent autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(8), 988–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baranek, G. T. (2002). Efficacy of sensory and motor interventions for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(5), 397422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006). Seeing it differently: visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(6), 258–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brand, R. J., Baldwin, D. A., & Ashburn, L. A. (2002). Evidence for ‘motionese’: modifications in mothers’ infant-directed action. Developmental Science, 5, 7283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brigham, N. B., Yoder, P. J., Jarzynka, M. A., & Tapp, J. (2010). The sequential relationship between parent attentional cues and sustained attention to objects in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(2), 200–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Developmental Science, 8(6), 535–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. (1983). Child's talk: learning to use language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Burling, J., & Yoshida, H. (2019). Visual constancies amidst changes in handled objects for 5- to 24-month-old infants. Child Development, 90(2), 352461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capone, N. C., & McGregor, K. K. (2004). Gesture development: a review for clinical and research practices. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(1), 173–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carbone, V. J., O'Brien, L., Sweeney-Kerwin, E. J., & Albert, K. M. (2013). Teaching eye contact to children with autism: a conceptual analysis and single case study. Education and Treatment of Children, 36(2), 139–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, i–174.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2002). Interrelations among social-cognitive skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 91106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassel, T. D., Messinger, D. S., Ibanez, L. V., Haltigan, J. D., Acosta, S. I., & Buchman, A. C. (2007). Early social and emotional communication in the infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders: an examination of the broad phenotype. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 122–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Drew, A., & Cox, A. (2003). Predicting language outcome in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38(3), 265–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C., & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language, 30(1), 213–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Datavyu Team (2014). Datavyu: a video coding tool. Databrary Project, New York University. Online <http://datavyu.org>..>Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deàk, G., Flom, R., & Pick, A. D. (2000). Effects of gesture and target on 12- and 18-month-olds’ joint visual attention to objects in front of or behind them. Developmental Psychology, 36, 511–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Campos, A. C., Savelsbergh, G. J., & Rocha, N. A. C. F. (2012). What do we know about the atypical development of exploratory actions during infancy? Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(6), 2228–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dent-Read, C., & Zukow-Goldring, P. (1997). Introduction: ecological realism, dynamic systems and epigenetic systems approaches to development. In Dent-Read, C. & Zukow-Goldring, P. (Eds.), Evolving explanations of development: ecological approaches to organism-environment systems (pp. 122). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dimitrova, N., & Moro, C. (2013). Common ground on object use associates with caregivers’ gestures, Infant Behavior and Development, 36(4), 618–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dimitrova, N., Özçalışkan, Ş., & Adamson, L. B. (2016). Parents' translations of child gesture facilitate word learning in children with autism, Down syndrome and typical development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(1), 221–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., Joe, C. M., Bazhenova, O. V., & Porges, S. W. (2003). Mother–child interaction in autistic and nonautistic children: characteristics of maternal approach behaviours and child social responses. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 277–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eigsti, I. M. (2013). A review of embodiment in autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 224. doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00224Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1972). Hand movements. Journal of Communication, 22(4), 353–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
El-Ghoroury, N. H., & Romanczyk, R. G. (1999). Play interactions of family members towards children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(3), 249–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elsabbagh, M., Fernandes, J., Webb, S. J., Dawson, G., Charman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2013). Disengagement of visual attention in infancy is associated with emerging autism in toddlerhood. Biological Psychiatry, 74(3), 189–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. S. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: user's guide and technical manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: user's guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Gogate, L., Maganti, M., & Bahrick, L. E. (2015). Cross-cultural evidence for multimodal motherese: Asian-Indian mothers’ adaptive use of synchronous words and gestures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 129, 110–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E., & Iverson, J. M. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science, 10, 778–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomez, J. C., Sarriá, E., & Tamarit, J. (1993). The comparative study of early communication and theories of mind: ontogeny, phylogeny, and pathology. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goodrich, B., Gabry, J., Ali, I., & Brilleman, S. (2018). rstanarm: Bayesian applied regression modeling via Stan. R package version 2.17.4. Online <http://mc-stan.org/>..>Google Scholar
Goodwyn, S. W., & Acredolo, L. P. (1998). Encouraging symbolic gestures: a new perspective on the relationship between gesture and speech. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 79, 6173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwyn, S. W., Acredolo, L. P., & Brown, C. A. (2000). Impact of symbolic gesturing on early language development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(2), 81103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanna, J. E., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2004). Pragmatic effects on reference resolution in a collaborative task: evidence from eye movements. Cognitive Science, 28, 105–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollingworth, A., & Maxcey-Richard, A. M. (2013). Selective maintenance in visual working memory does not require sustained visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(4), 1047–58.Google Scholar
Horst, J. S., Samuelson, L. K., Kucker, S. C., & McMurray, B. (2011). What's new? Children prefer novelty in referent selection. Cognition, 118(2), 234–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Longobardi, E., & Caselli, M. C. (1999). Gesturing in mother–child interactions. Cognitive Development, 14(1), 5775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jellema, T., & Lorteije, J., Van Rijn, S., Wout, M., de Haan, E., van Engeland, H., & Kemner, C. (2009). Involuntary interpretation of social cues is compromised in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 2, 192204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. (1997). Linking parental perception to interactions in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(1), 3957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krippendorff, K. (2004). Content analysis: an introduction to its methodology (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Kruschke, J. K., & Liddell, T. M. (2018). The Bayesian new statistics: hypothesis testing, estimation, meta-analysis, and power analysis from a Bayesian perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 178206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kyger, M. F. (2013). Parents provide children with social cues for word learning (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from <https://escholarship.org/uc/item/27h4d4t8>..>Google Scholar
Landa, R. J., Holman, K. C., & Garrett-Mayer, E. (2007). Social and communication development in toddlers with early and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(7), 853–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1115–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, S. H., & Chapieski, M. L. (1989). Joint attention and infant toy exploration: effects of Down syndrome and prematurity. Child Development, 60, 103–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leekam, S. R., Hunnisett, E., & Moore, C. (1998). Targets and cues: gaze-following in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(7), 951–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leekam, S. R., López, B., & Moore, C. (2000). Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 261–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemanek, K. L., Stone, W. L., & Fishel, P. T. (1993). Parent–child interactions in handicapped preschoolers: the relation between parent behaviors and compliance. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 6877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loveland, K. A., & Landry, S. H. (1986). Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16(3), 335–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyster, R. J., Kadlec, M. B., Carter, A., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2008). Language assessment and development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(8),1426–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, H.B., & Whitney, D. R. (1947). On a test of whether one of two random variables is stochastically larger than the other. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 18, 5060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matatyaho, D. J., & Gogate, L. J. (2008). Type of maternal object motion during synchronous naming predicts preverbal infants’ learning of word–object relations. Infancy, 13(2), 172–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeish, D. (2016). On using Bayesian methods to address small sample problems. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 23(5), 750–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Medeiros, K., & Winsler, A. (2014). Parent–child gesture use during problem solving in autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(8), 1946–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint attention, developmental level, and symptom presentation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6(3), 389401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: the contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27(5), 657–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1987). Nonverbal communication and play correlates of language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(3), 349–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, P., & Stella, J. (2000). Joint attention, social orienting, and nonverbal communication in autism. In Wetherby, A. M. & Prizant, B. M. (Eds.), Autism spectrum disorders: a transactional developmental perspective (Communication and Language Intervention Series, Vol 9) (pp. 5577). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Muthén, B., & Asparouhov, T. (2012). Bayesian structural equation modeling: a more flexible representation of substantive theory. Psychological Methods, 17(3), 313–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Namy, L. L., Acredolo, L., & Goodwyn, S. (2000). Verbal labels and gestural routines in parental communication with young children. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(2), 6379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Namy, L. L., & Nolan, S. A. (2004). Characterizing changes in parent labelling and gesturing and their relation to early communicative development. Journal of Child Language, 31(4), 821–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Neill, M., Bard, K. A., Linnell, M., & Fluck, M. (2005). Maternal gestures with 20-month-old infants in two contexts. Developmental Science, 8(4), 352–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozonoff, S., Goodlin-Jones, B. L., & Solomon, M. (2005). Evidence-based assessment of autism spectrum disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(3), 523–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., & McCarthy, G. (2005). Neural basis of eye gaze processing deficits in autism. Brain, 128(5), 1038–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennington, L., & McConachie, H. (2001). Interaction between children with cerebral palsy and their mothers: the effects of speech intelligibility. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36(3), 371–93.Google ScholarPubMed
Pereira, A. F., James, K. H., Jones, S. S., & Smith, L. B. (2010) Early biases and developmental changes in self-generated object views. Journal of Vision, 10(11), 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pereira, A. P., Smith, L. B., & Yu, C. (2014). A bottom-up view of toddler word learning. Psychological Bulletin & Review, 21(1), 178–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramachandran, V. S., & Oberman, L. M. (2006). Broken mirrors: a theory of autism. Scientific American, 295(5), 62–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rickert, M., Yu, C., & Favata, A. (2010). Joint attention through the hands: investigating the timing of object labeling in dyadic social interaction. 2010 IEEE 9th International Conference on Development and Learning (pp. 114–19). doi:10.1109/DEVLRN.2010.5578857CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruff, H. A., & Capozzoli, M. C. (2003). Development of attention and distractibility in the first 4 years of life. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 877–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shumway, S., & Wetherby, A. M. (2009). Communicative acts of children with autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(5), 1139–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sigman, M., Mundy, P., Sherman, T., & Ungerer, J. (1986). Social interactions of autistic, mentally retarded and normal children and their caregivers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27(5), 647–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2008). Modeling longitudinal change in the language abilities of children with autism: parent behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1691–704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, R. L. et al. (2005). Autism spectrum disorders: Interventions and treatments for children and youth. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.Google Scholar
Skwerer, D. P., Jordan, S. E., Brukilacchio, B. H., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2016). Comparing methods for assessing receptive language skills in minimally verbal children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 20(5), 591604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. B., Yu, C., & Pereira, A. F. (2011). Not your mother's view: the dynamics of toddler visual experience. Developmental Science, 14(1), 917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, L. B., Yu, C., Yoshida, H., & Fausey, C. (2015). Contributions of head-mounted cameras to studying the visual environments of infants and young children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(3), 407–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, M., Finelli, J., Marvin, A., Garrett-Mayer, E., Bauman, M., & Landa, R. (2007). Response to joint attention in toddlers at risk for autism spectrum disorder: a prospective study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 3748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takarae, Y., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. A. (2012). Development of visual sensorimotor systems and their cognitive mediation in autism. In Preedy, V. R. (Ed.), Handbook of growth and growth monitoring in health and disease (pp. 1379–93). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talbott, M. R., Nelson, C. A., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2015). Maternal gesture use and language development in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(1), 414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57(6), 1454–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ungerer, J. A., & Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play and language comprehension in autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 20(2), 318–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Rutgers, A. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Swinkels, S. H. N., van Daalen, E., Dietz, C., Naber, F. B. A., Buitelaar, J. K., & van Engeland, H. (2007). Parental sensitivity and attachment in children with autism spectrum disorder. Child Development, 78, 597608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, X., Desalvo, N., Zhao, X., Feng, T., Loveland, K. A., Shi, W., & Gnawali, O. (2014). Eye contact reminder system for people with autism. Paper presented at the 6th International Conference on Mobile Computing, Applications and Services, Austin, TX, 6–7 November 2014. doi:10.4108/icst.mobicase.2014.257796CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wetherby, A., Watt, N., Morgan, L., & Shumway, S. (2007). Social communication profiles of children with autism spectrum disorders late in the second year of life. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(5), 960–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 25(4), 287–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoshida, H., & Burling, J. M. (2013). Dynamic shift in isolating referents: from social to self-generated input. 2013 IEEE Third Joint International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL), 12. Online <https://doi.org/10.1109/DevLrn.2013.6652570>.CrossRef.>Google Scholar
Yoshida, H., & Fausey, C. M. (2019) Visual objects as they are encountered by young language learners. In Horst, J. & Torkildsen, J. (Eds.), International handbook of language development (pp. 115–27). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Yoshida, H., & Smith, L. B. (2008). What's in view for toddlers? Using a head camera to study visual experience. Infancy, 13(3), 229–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2012). Embodied attention and word learning by toddlers. Cognition, 125(2), 244–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2016). The social origins of sustained attention in one-year-old human infants. Current Biology, 26(9), 1235–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zammit, M., & Schafer, G. (2011). Maternal label and gesture use affects acquisition of specific object names. Journal of Child Language, 38(1), 201–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zukow-Goldring, P. (1996). Sensitive caregiving fosters the comprehension of speech: when gestures speak louder than words. Early Development & Parenting, 5, 195211.3.0.CO;2-H>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zukow-Goldring, P. (1997). A social ecological realist approach to the emergence of the lexicon: educating attention to amodal invariants in gesture and speech. In Dent-Read, C. & Zukow-Goldring, P. (Eds.), Evolving explanations of development: ecological approaches to organism–environment systems (pp. 199250). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar