Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T06:16:12.078Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Preschoolers are sensitive to accent distance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 August 2019

Drew WEATHERHEAD*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, BC, Canada
Ori FRIEDMAN
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
Katherine S. WHITE
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
*
*Corresponding Author: Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, 2329 West Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada, V6T 1Z4. E-mail: drew.weatherhead@psych.ubc.ca

Abstract

Can children tell how different a speaker's accent is from their own? In Experiment 1 (N = 84), four- and five-year-olds heard speakers with different accents and indicated where they thought each speaker lived relative to a reference point on a map that represented their current location. Five-year-olds generally placed speakers with stronger accents (as judged by adults) at more distant locations than speakers with weaker accents. In contrast, four-year-olds did not show differences in where they placed speakers with different accents. In Experiment 2 (N = 56), the same sentences were low-pass filtered so that only prosodic information remained. This time, children judged which of five possible aliens had produced each utterance, given a reference speaker. Children of both ages showed differences in which alien they chose based on accent, and generally rated speakers with foreign accents as more different from their native accent than speakers with regional accents. Together, the findings show that preschoolers perceive accent distance, that children may be sensitive to the distinction between foreign and regional accents, and that preschoolers likely use prosody to differentiate among accents.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson-Hsieh, J., Johnson, R., & Koehler, K. (1992). The relationship between native speaker judgments of nonnative pronunciation and deviance in segmentals, prosody, and syllable structure. Language Learning, 42(4), 529–55.Google Scholar
Atagi, E., & Bent, T. (2016). Auditory free classification of native and nonnative speech by nonnative listeners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(2), 241–63.Google Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2017). Praat: doing phonetics by computer (Version 6.0.28) [Computer program]. Retrieved from <http://www.praat.org/>..>Google Scholar
Brennan, E. M., & Brennan, J. S. (1981). Measurements of accent and attitude toward Mexican-American speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 10(5), 487501.Google Scholar
Carmichael, L. (2000). Measurable degrees of foreign accent: a correlational study of perception, production, and acquisition. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Washington, WA, USA.Google Scholar
Clarke, C. M., & Garrett, M. F. (2004). Rapid adaptation to foreign-accented English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 116(6), 3647–58.Google Scholar
Creel, S. C. (2018). Accent detection and social cognition: evidence of protracted learning. Developmental Science, 21, e12524. doi:10.1111/desc.12524Google Scholar
DeJesus, J. M., Hwang, H. G., Dautel, J. B., & Kinzler, K. D. (2017). Bilingual children's social preferences hinge on accent. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 164, 178–91.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1984). The detection of French accent by American listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 76(3), 692707.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Munro, M. J., & MacKay, I. R. A. (1995). Factors affecting strength of perceived foreign accent in a second language. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97(5), 3125–34.Google Scholar
Floccia, C., Butler, J., Girard, F., & Goslin, J. (2009). Categorization of regional and foreign accent in 5- to 7-year-old British children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33(4), 366–75.Google Scholar
Floccia, C., Goslin, J., Girard, F., & Konopczynski, G. (2006). Does a regional accent perturb speech processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(5), 1276–93.Google Scholar
Girard, F., Floccia, C., & Goslin, J. (2008). Perception and awareness of accents in young children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 409–33.Google Scholar
Goslin, J., Duffy, H., & Floccia, C. (2012). An ERP investigation of regional and foreign accent processing. Brain and Language, 122(2), 92102.Google Scholar
Grover, C., Jamieson, D. G., & Dobrovolsky, M. B. (1987). Intonation in English, French and German: perception and production. Language and Speech, 30(3), 277–96.Google Scholar
Hahn, L. D. (2004). Primary stress and intelligibility: research to motivate the teaching of suprasegmentals. TESOL Quarterly, 38(2), 201–23.Google Scholar
Hendriks, B., van Meurs, F., & de Groot, E. (2017). The effects of degrees of Dutch accentedness in ELF and in French, German and Spanish. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 27(1), 4466.Google Scholar
Jones, Z., Yan, Q., Wagner, L., & Clopper, C. G. (2017). The development of dialect classification across the lifespan. Journal of Phonetics, 60, 2037.Google Scholar
Kang, O. (2010). Salient prosodic features on judgements of second language accent. In Speech Prosody 2010 – Fifth International Conference, paper 016. Online <http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.302.1841&rep=rep1&type=pdf>..>Google Scholar
Kang, O., Rubin, D., & Pickering, L. (2010). Suprasegmental measures of accentedness and judgments of language learner proficiency in oral English. Modern Language Journal, 94(4), 554–66.Google Scholar
Kang, O., Thomson, R. I., & Moran, M. (2018). Empirical approaches to measuring the intelligibility of different varieties of English in predicting listener comprehension. Language Learning, 68, 115–46.Google Scholar
Kinzler, K. D., Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2011). Children's selective trust in native-accented speakers. Developmental Science, 14(1), 106–11.Google Scholar
Kinzler, K. D., & Dautel, J. B. (2012). Children's essentialist reasoning about language and race. Developmental Science, 15(1), 131–8.Google Scholar
Kinzler, K. D., & DeJesus, J. M. (2013a). Children's sociolinguistic evaluations of nice foreigners and mean Americans. Developmental Psychology, 49(4), 655–64.Google Scholar
Kinzler, K. D., & DeJesus, J. M. (2013b). Northern = smart and Southern = nice: the development of accent attitudes in the United States. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(6), 1146–58.Google Scholar
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(30), 12577–80.Google Scholar
Munro, M. J. (1995). Nonsegmental factors in foreign accent: ratings of filtered speech. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17(1), 1734.Google Scholar
Munro, M. J., & Derwing, T. M. (1995). Foreign accent, comprehensibility, and intelligibility in the speech of second language learners. Language Learning, 45, 7397.Google Scholar
Munro, M. J., & Derwing, T. M. (2001). Modeling perceptions of the accentedness and comprehensibility of L2 speech: the role of speaking rate. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23(4), 451–68.Google Scholar
Nejjari, W., Gerritsen, M., Van der Haagen, M., & Korzilius, H. (2012). Responses to Dutch-accented English. World Englishes, 31(2), 248–67.Google Scholar
Nesdale, D., & Rooney, R. (1996). Evaluations and stereotyping of accented speakers by pre-adolescent children. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 15(2), 133–54.Google Scholar
Ryan, E. B., Carranza, M. A., & Moffie, R. W. (1977). Reactions toward varying degrees of accentedness in the speech of Spanish–English bilinguals. Language and Speech, 20(3), 267–73.Google Scholar
Shah, A. P. (2003). Acoustic indicators of Spanish-accented English. Canadian Acoustics, 31(3), 42–3.Google Scholar
Tajima, K., Port, R., & Dalby, J. (1997). Effects of temporal correction on intelligibility of foreign-accented English. Journal of Phonetics, 25(1), 124.Google Scholar
Trofimovich, P., & Baker, W. (2006). Learning second language suprasegmentals: effect of L2 experience on prosody and fluency characteristics of L2 speech. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28(1), 130.Google Scholar
Wagner, L., Clopper, C. G., & Pate, J. K. (2014). Children's perception of dialect variation. Journal of Child Language, 41(5), 1062–84.Google Scholar
Weatherhead, D., Friedman, O., & White, K. S. (2018). Accent, language, and race: 4–6-year-old children's inferences differ by speaker cue. Child Development, 89, 1613–24.Google Scholar
Weatherhead, D., White, K. S., & Friedman, O. (2016). Where are you from? Preschoolers infer background from accent. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 143, 171–8.Google Scholar
Weinberger, S. H. (2014). Speech Accent Archive. George Mason University. Retrieved from <http://accent.gmu.edu>..>Google Scholar