Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T02:27:41.515Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Re-introduction of referents in Italian children's narratives

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Margherita Orsolini*
Affiliation:
Università La Sapienza, Roma
Franca Rossi
Affiliation:
Università La Sapienza, Roma
Clotilde Pontecorvo
Affiliation:
Università La Sapienza, Roma
*
[*] Address for correspondence: Dipartimento di Psicologia, N. 38, Via dei Marsi 78, 00185 Rome, Italy. E-mail: orsolini@unich.it

Abstract

In this study we investigate the re-introduction of referents in the Frog stories told by Italian children aged 4–10 (N = 100). We found that for every age group full nouns are the most frequent forms used for reference re-introduction. Null forms, such as clitic pronouns or person/number inflection on the verb, are the second most frequent forms. A detailed analysis of null forms shows that children of different ages exploit different properties of the verbal and non-verbal context which can make a referent predictable. Compared to preschoolers, elementary school children are more likely to use null forms when the semantic content of the verb, or the structure of the preceding text make referents inferrable. On the other hand, preschoolers tend to exploit the importance of a character in the story plot, or the visual availability of the referent in the non-verbal context, as properties that make an entity salient enough to prevent the speaker from using overt linguistic forms such as full nouns. Our study confirms results of previous research, showing that elementary school children are more competent than preschoolers in integrating the semantic content of the current utterance into the context generated by previous discourse.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Ariel, M. (1990). Accessing noun-phrase antecedents. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bamberg, M. (1987). The acquisition of narratives. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A. & Slobin, D. I. (1986). Coding Manual: temporality in discourse. University of California, Berkeley. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Berretta, N. (1985). I pronomi clitici nell'italiano parlato. In Hoitus, G. & Radtke, E. (Hrsg.) Gesprochenes Italienisch in Geschichte und Gegenwart. Tübingen: Gunter Narr.Google Scholar
Berretta, N. (1990). Catene anaforiche in prospettiva funzionale: antecedenti difficili. Rivista di Linguistica 2, 91120.Google Scholar
Ehlich, K. (1982). Anaphora and deixis: same, similar, or different? In Jarvella, R. J. & Klein, W. (eds), Speech, place and action. Studies in deixis and related topics. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A. (1989). Mechanisms that improve referential access. Cognition 32, 99156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Givon, T. (1979). Syntax and semantics Vol. 12. Discourse and syntax. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hickman, M. (1980). Creating referents in discourse: A developmental analysis of linguistic cohesion. In Kreiman, J. & Ojeda, A. E. (eds), Papers from the parasession on pronouns and anaphora. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Hickman, M. (1991). The development of discourse cohesion: some functional and cross-linguistic issues. In Pie'raut-le-Bonniec, G. (ed), From basic language to discourse bases. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1981). The grammatical marking of thematic structure in the development of language production. In Deutsch, W. (ed), The child's construction of language. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985). Language and cognitive processes from a developmental perspective. Language and Cognitive Processes 1, 6185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1991). CHILDES: tools for analyzing language. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., Levy, E. & Tyler, L. (1982). Producing interpretable discourse: the establishment and maintenance of reference. In Jarvella, R. J. & Klein, W. (eds), Speech place and action. Studies in deixis and related topics. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., Tyler, L. & Koster, C. (1993). Integrative processes in utterance resolution. Journal of Memory and Language 32, 647–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1924). Le langage et la pensée chez l'enfant. Neuchâtel, Switzerland: Delachaux et Niestlé.Google Scholar
Rossi, F. & Pontecorvo, C. (1994). Italian children's oral and written narratives: the maintenance and re-introduction of referents. Paper presented at the First Lisbon Meeting on Child Language.Google Scholar
Sanford, A. J. & Garrod, S. C. (1989). What, when and how?: Questions of immediacy in anaphoric reference resolution. Language and Cognitive Processes 4, (3/4), 235–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simone, R. (1990). Testo. In Simone, R., Fondamenti di linguistica. Bari: Laterza.Google Scholar