Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T18:04:44.480Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Remembered voices*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Richard Ely*
Affiliation:
Tufts University
Allyssa McCabe
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts at Lowell, Harvard University
*
Address for correspondence: Department of Child Study, Tufts University, Medford, MA, 02155, USA.

Abstract

The speech children spontaneously quote was examined in two studies. In Study 1, a corpus of personal narratives from 96 children aged 4;0 to 9;0 was analysed; Study 2 investigated reported speech in 25 younger children aged 1;2 to 5;2 interacting with their parents. In both studies, the frequency of reported speech increased with age. Direct quotation was more common than indirect or summarized quotation at all ages. In Study 1, children quoted themselves more frequently than any other speaker, and their mothers more frequently than their fathers. Directives were the most commonly reported speech act from the distant past in both older (Study 1) and younger (Study 2) children. In Study 1, girls used reported speech more frequently than did boys, and their quotations were more direct in form than were those of boys.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The support and technical assistance from Jean Berko Gleason and the Child Language Laboratory at Boston University, the assistance of Jim Cacos, and the valuable commentary of two anonymous reviewers are gratefully acknowledged.

References

REFERENCES

Bakhtin, M. M. (1986). Speech genres and other late essays. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Bellinger, D. & Gleason, J. B. (1982). Sex differences in parental directives to young children. Sex Roles 8, 1123–39.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P., Keenan, J. M. & Potts, G. R. (1986). The self-referencing effect with imagery encoding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51, 897906.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. & Lucariello, J. (1989). Monologue as narrative recreation of the world. In Nelson, K. (ed.), Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Coulmas, F. (1986). Reported speech: some general issues. In Coulmas, F. (ed.), Direct and indirect speech. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. & Gordon, D. (1986). The development of requests. In Schiefelbusch, R. L. (ed.), Language competence: assessments and interventions. San Diego: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
Fagot, B. I. & Hagan, R. (1991). Observations of parent reactions to sex-stereotyped behaviors: age and sex effects. Child Development 62, 617–28.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. (1977). Play with language and speech. In Mitchell-Kernan, C. & Ervin, S. (eds), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Genette, G. (1988). Narrative discourse revisited. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. B. (1980). The acquisition of social speech and politeness formulae. In Giles, H., Robinson, W. P. & Smith, P. M. (eds), Language: social psychological perspectives. Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. B. & Greif, E. B. (1983). Men's speech to young children. In Thorne, B., Kramerae, C. & Henley, M. (eds), Language, gender and society. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. B., Hay, D. & Cain, L. (1988). Social and affective determinants of language acquisition. In Rice, M. L. & Schiefelbusch, R. L. (eds), The teachability of language. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. B., Pearlman, R. Y. & Greif, E. R. (1984). What's the magic word: learning language through politeness routines. Discourse Processes 7, 493502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, G. S., Bottoms, B. L., Schwartz-Kenney, B. M. & Rudy, L. (1991). Children's testimony about a stressful event: improving children's reports. Journal of Narrative and Life History 1, 6999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M. H. (1990). He-said-she-said: talk as social organization among black children. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Haas, A. (1979). The acquisition of genderlect. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 237, 101–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haberland, H. (1986). Reported speech in Danish. In Coulmas, F. (ed.), Direct and indirect speech. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Keenan, J. M., MacWhinney, B. & Mayhew, D. (1977). Pragmatics in memory: a study of natural conversation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 16, 549–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kris, E. (1956/1975). The selected papers of Ernst Kris. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Landis, T. Y. (1982). Interactions between text and prior knowledge in children's memory for prose. Child Development 53, 811–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucariello, J. & Nelson, K. (1987). Remember and planning talk between mothers and children. Discourse Processes 10, 219–35.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Snow, C. (1990). The Child Language Data Exchange System: an update. Journal of Child Language 17, 457–72.Google Scholar
Markus, H. (1980). The self in thought and memory. In Wegner, D. M. & Vallacher, R. R. (eds), The self in social psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McCabe, A. (1991 a). Plans, routines, and memories: inspired telling – a response to Katherine Nelson. Journal of Narrative and Life History 1, 129–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCabe, A. (1991 b). Preface: structure as a way of understanding. In McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (eds), Developing narrative structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McCabe, A. & Lipscomb, T. J. (1988). Sex differences in children's verbal aggression. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly 34, 389401.Google Scholar
Miller, P. J., Mintz, J., Hoogstra, L., Fung, H. & Potts, R. (1992). The narrated self: young children's construction of self in relations to others in conversational stories of personal experience. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly 38, 4567.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1981). John Dean's memory: a case study. Cognition 9, 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neisser, U. (1982). Memory: what are the important questions? In Neisser, U. (ed.), Memory observed: remembering in natural contexts. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1986). Event knowledge: structure and function in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1988). The ontogeny of memory for real events. In Neisser, U. & Winograd, E. (eds), Remembering reconsidered: ecological and traditional approaches to the study of memory. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1990). Remembering, forgetting, and infantile amnesia. In Fivush, R. & Hudson, J. A. (eds), Knowing and remembering in young children. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1991). Remembering and telling: a developmental story. Journal of Narrative and Life History 1, 100–27.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. & Rinott, N. (1988). Fathers' involvement in the care of their infants and their attributions of cognitive competence. Child Development 59, 652–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ochs, E. (1988). Culture and language development. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Ong, W. J. (1982). Orality and literacy: the technologizing of the word. New York: Methuen.Google Scholar
Peterson, C. & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: three ways of looking at a child's narrative. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preece, A. (1990). Collaborators and critics: peer influence on narrative development. Paper presented at the Fifth International Congress for the Study of Child Language. Budapest, Hungary.Google Scholar
Purcell-Gates, V. (1988). Lexical and syntactic knowledge of written narrative held by well-read-to kindergartners and second graders. Research in the teaching of English 22, 128–60.Google Scholar
Ross, B. M. (1991). Remembering the personal past: descriptions of autobiographical memory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. (1967). Recognition memory of syntactic and semantic aspects of connective discourse. Perception and Psychophysics 2, 437–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about the there and then: the emergence of displaced reference in parent-child discourse. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1976). A classification of illocutionary acts. Language in Society 5, 123.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1990). Building memories: the ontogeny of autobiography. In Cicchetti, D. & Beeghly, M. (eds), The self in transition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Spence, D. (1982). Narrative truth and historical truth. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Staley, C. M. (1982). Sex-related differences in the style of children's language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 11, 141–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tannen, D. (1989). Talking voices: repetition, dialogue and imagery in conversational discourse. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Tannen, D. (1990). You just don't understand: women and men in conversation. New York: Ballantine.Google Scholar
Todd, C. M. & Perlmutter, M. (1980). Reality recalled by preschool children. In Perlmutter, M. (ed.), Children's memory. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Vološinov, V. N. (1986). Marxism and the philosophy of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Welty, E. (1984). One writer's beginnings. New York: Warner.Google Scholar
Wolf, D. & Hicks, D. (1989). The voices within narratives: the development of intertextuality in young children's stories. Discourse Processes 12, 329–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar