Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T05:51:05.017Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Syllabic parsing in children: a developmental study using visual word-spotting in Spanish*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 February 2016

CARLOS J. ÁLVAREZ*
Affiliation:
Departamento de Psicología Cognitiva. Universidad de La Laguna (Tenerife, Spain)
GUACIMARA GARCÍA-SAAVEDRA
Affiliation:
Colegio Nuestra Señora de los Ángeles (Tenerife, Spain)
JUAN L. LUQUE
Affiliation:
Departamento de Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación. Universidad de Málaga (Spain)
MARCUS TAFT
Affiliation:
School of Psychology. University of New South Wales (Sydney, Australia)
*
Address for correspondence: Carlos J. Álvarez Glez., Departamento de Psicología Cognitiva, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de La Laguna, 38201 – Tenerife (Spain). tel: 00 34 22 317507; e-mail: calvarez@ull.es.

Abstract

Some inconsistency is observed in the results from studies of reading development regarding the role of the syllable in visual word recognition, perhaps due to a disparity between the tasks used. We adopted a word-spotting paradigm, with Spanish children of second grade (mean age: 7 years) and sixth grade (mean age: 11 years). The children were asked to detect one-syllable words that could be found at the beginning of pseudo-words, with the boundary between the word and the remaining letters being manipulated. The end of the embedded word could either match the syllabic boundary (e.g. the word FIN in the pseudo-word FINLO, where the syllable boundary is between N and L) or not (e.g. FINUS, where the syllable boundary is located between I and N). The results showed that children of both grades were faster in the syllabic than the non-syllabic condition, and that the magnitude of this effect was the same regardless of reading ability. The results suggest an early universality in the use of syllables in Spanish, regardless of reading level.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by Grants PSI2010/15184 and PSI2013-47959-P to the first author from the Spanish government. We also want to express our gratitude to the direction, teachers, and staff of the Finca España School (La Laguna, Tenerife) for allowing us to carry out this research. The authors would also like to thank the reviewers for their helpful comments.

References

REFERENCES

Álvarez, C. J., Carreiras, M. & de Vega, M. (2000). Syllable frequency effect in visual word recognition: evidence of sequential type processing. Psicológica 21, 341–74.Google Scholar
Álvarez, C. J., Carreiras, M. & Perea, M. (2004). Are syllables phonological units in visual word recognition? Language and Cognitive Processes 19, 427–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Álvarez, C. J., Carreiras, M. & Taft, M. (2001). Syllables and morphemes: contrasting frequency effects in Spanish. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 27, 545–55.Google Scholar
Barber, H., Vergara, M. & Carreiras, M. (2004). Syllable-frequency effects in visual word recognition: evidence from ERPs. Neuroreport 15, 545–8.Google Scholar
Carreiras, M., Álvarez, C. J. & de Vega, M. (1993). Syllable frequency and visual word recognition in Spanish. Journal of Memory and Language, 32 766780.Google Scholar
Carreiras, M. & Perea, M. (2004). Naming pseudowords in Spanish: effects of syllable frequency. Brain and Language 90, 393400.Google Scholar
Carrillo, M. (1994). Development of phonological awareness and reading acquisition: a study in Spanish language, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 6, 279–98.Google Scholar
Carrillo, M. & Marín, J. (1996). Desarrollo metafonológico y adquisición de la lectura: un programa de entrenamiento. Madrid: CIDE-MEC.Google Scholar
Chetail, F. & Mathey, S. (2008). Activation of syllable units during visual recognition of French words in Grade 2. Journal of Child Language 35, 883–94.Google Scholar
Chetail, F. & Mathey, S. (2009). The syllable frequency effect in visual recognition of French words: a study in skilled and beginning readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 22, 955–73.Google Scholar
Chetail, F. & Mathey, S. (2012). Effect of syllable congruency in sixth graders in the lexical decision task with masked priming. Scientific Studies of Reading 16, 537–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chetail, F. & Mathey, S. (2013). Interaction between phonemic abilities and syllable congruency effect in young readers. Journal of Child Language 40, 492508.Google Scholar
Colé, P., Magnan, A. & Grainger, J. (1999). Syllable-sized units in visual word recognition: evidence from skilled and beginning readers of French. Applied Psycholinguistics 20, 507–32.Google Scholar
Conrad, M., Carreiras, M. & Jacobs, A. M. (2008). Contrasting effects of token and type syllable frequency in lexical decision. Language and Cognitive Processes 23(2), 296326.Google Scholar
Conrad, M., Grainger, J. & Jacobs, A. M. (2007). Phonology as the source of syllable frequency effects in visual word recognition: evidence from French. Memory and Cognition 35(5), 974–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conrad, M. & Jacobs, A. M. (2004). Replicating syllable-frequency effects in Spanish in German: one more challenge to computational models of visual word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes 19, 369–90.Google Scholar
Conrad, M., Tamm, S., Carreiras, M. & Jacobs, A. M. (2010). Simulating syllable frequency effects within an interactive activation framework. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 22(5), 861–93.Google Scholar
Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y., Katz, L. & Tola, G. (1988). Awareness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children. Applied Psycholinguistics 9, 116.Google Scholar
Cuetos, F., Rodríguez, B., Ruano, E. & Arribas, D. (2009). Batería de Evaluación de los Procesos Lectores PROLEC-R. Madrid: Ediciones TEA.Google Scholar
Cutler, A. & Norris, D. (1988). The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14, 113–21.Google Scholar
Davis, C. J. & Perea, M. (2005). BuscaPalabras: a program for deriving orthographic and phonological neighborhood statistics and other psycholinguistic indices in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods 37, 665–71.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. (2009) Reading in the brain: the new science of how we read. New York: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Doignon, N. & Zagar, D. (2005). Illusory conjunctions in French: the nature of sublexical units in visual word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes 20, 443–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doignon, N. & Zagar, D. (2006). Les enfants en cours d'apprentissae de la lectura perçoivent-ils la syllable á l`écrit? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 60, 258–74.Google Scholar
Doignon-Camus, N. & Zagar, D. (2009). Les enfants apprentis lecteurs perçoivent-ils la syllabe à l’écrit? Le modèle DIAMS. In Marec-Breton, N., Besse, A. S., de La Haye, F., Bonneton, N. & Bonjour, E. (eds), Apprentissage du langage écrit: Approche cognitive, 3347. Rennes: Presses Universitaires de Rennes.Google Scholar
Doignon-Camus, N. & Zagar, D. (2014). The syllabic bridge: the first step in learning spelling-to-sound correspondences. Journal of Child Language 41, 1147–65.Google Scholar
Ecalle, J., Kleinsz, N. & Magnan, A. (2013). Computer-assisted learning in young poor readers: the effect of grapho-syllabic training on the development of word reading and reading comprehension. Computers in Human Behavior 29, 1368–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I. & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: a Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers 35, 116–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goikoetxea, E. (2005). Levels of phonological awareness in preliterate and literate Spanish-speaking children. Reading and Writing 18, 5179.Google Scholar
Hutzler, F., Bergmann, J., Conrad, M., Kronbichler, M., Stenneken, P. & Jacobs, A. M. (2004). Inhibitory effects of first syllable-frequency in lexical decision: an event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters 372, 179–84.Google Scholar
Jiménez, J. E., García, E., O'Shanahan, I. & Rojas, E. (2010). Do Spanish children use the syllable in visual word recognition in learning to read? Spanish Journal of Psychology 13, 6374.Google Scholar
Jiménez, J. E., Guzmán, R. & Artiles, C. (1997). Efectos de la frecuencia silábica posicional en el aprendizaje de la lectura. Cognitiva, 1, 327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiménez, J. E. & Hernández, I. (2000). Word identification and reading disorders in the Spanish language. Journal of Learning Disabilities 33(1), 4460.Google Scholar
Jiménez, J. E. & Ortiz, R. (2000). Metalinguistic awareness and reading acquisition in the Spanish language. Spanish Journal of Psychology 3, 3746.Google Scholar
Jiménez, J. E. & Rodrigo, M. (1994). Is it true that the differences in reading performance between students with and without LD cannot be explained by IQ? Journal of Learning Disabilities 27, 155–63.Google Scholar
Lander, K. & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: an 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology 100, 150–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luque, J. L., López-Zamora, M., Álvarez, C. J. & Bordoy, S. (2013). Beyond decoding deficit: inhibitory effect of positional syllable. Annals of Dyslexia 63, 239–52.Google Scholar
Macizo, P. & Van Petten, C. (2007). Syllable frequency in lexical decision and naming of English words. Reading and Writing 20, 295331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahé, G., Bonnefond, A. & Doignon-Camus, N. (2014). The time course of the syllable frequency effect in visual word recognition: evidence for both facilitatory and inhibitory effects in French. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 27, 171–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maïonchi-Pino, N., de Cara, B., Écalle, J. & Magnan, A. (2012a). Are French dyslexic children sensitive to consonant sonority in segmentation strategies? Preliminary evidence from a letter detection task. Research in Developmental Disabilities 33, 1223.Google Scholar
Maïonchi-Pino, N., de Cara, B., Écalle, J. & Magnan, A. (2012b). Do consonant sonority and status influence syllable-based segmentation strategies in a visual letter detection task? Developmental evidence in French children. Scientific Studies of Reading 16, 550–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maïonchi-Pino, N., Magnan, A. & Écalle, J. (2010a). Syllable frequency effects in visual word recognition: developmental approach in French children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 31, 7082.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maïonchi-Pino, N., Magnan, A. & Écalle, J. (2010b). The nature of the phonological processing in French dyslexic children: evidence for the phonological syllable and linguistic features’ role in silent reading and speech discrimination. Annals of Dyslexia 60, 123–50.Google Scholar
Mathey, S. & Zagar, D. (2002). Lexical similarity in visual word recognition: the effect of syllabic neighborhood in French. Current Psychology Letters: Behavior, Brain and Cognition 8, 107–21.Google Scholar
Mathey, S., Zagar, D., Doignon, N. & Seigneuric, A. (2006). The nature of the syllabic neighbourhood effect in French. Acta Psychologica 123(3), 372–93.Google Scholar
McQueen, J. M. (1996). Word spotting. Language and Cognitive Processes 11, 695–9.Google Scholar
Mehler, J., Dommergues, J. Y., Frauenfelder, U. & Segui, J. (1981). The syllable's role in speech segmentation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20, 298305.Google Scholar
Metsala, J. L. & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In Metsala, J. L. & Ehri, L. C. (eds), Word recognition in beginning literacy, 89120. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Orsolini, M., Fanari, R., Cerracchio, S. & Famiglietti, L. (2009). Phonological and lexical reading in Italian children with dislexia. Reading and Writing 22, 933–54.Google Scholar
Orsolini, M., Fanari, R., Tosi, V., De Nigris, B. & Carrieri, R. (2006). From phonological recoding to lexical reading: a longitudinal study on reading development in Italian. Language and Cognitive Processes 21, 576607.Google Scholar
Rapp, B. C. (1992). The nature of sublexical orthographic organization: the bigram trough hypothesis examined. Journal of Memory and Language 31, 3353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S. (1987). Sublexical structures in visual word recognition: access units or orthographic redundancy? In Coltheart, M. (ed.), Attention and performance, XII: the psychology of reading, 245–63. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Seymour, P., Aro, M. & Erskine, J. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology 94, 143–74.Google Scholar
Seymour, P. & Duncan, L (1997). Small versus large unit theories of reading acquisition. Dyslexia 3, 125–34.Google Scholar
Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition 55, 151218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taft, M. (2001). Processing of orthographic structure by adults of different reading ability. Language and Speech 44, 351–76.Google Scholar
Taft, M. & Álvarez, C. J. (2014). Coda optimization in the segmentation of English polysyllabic letter-strings. Experimental Psychology 61, 488–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taft, M., Álvarez, C. J. & Carreiras, M. (2007). Cross-language differences in the use of internal orthographic structure when reading polysyllabic words. Mental Lexicon 2(1), 4663.Google Scholar
Taft, M. & van Graan, F. (1998). Lack of phonological mediation in a semantic categorization task. Journal of Memory and Language 38, 203–24.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin 131(1), 329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed