Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T01:21:02.742Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Talker familiarity and spoken word recognition in school-age children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 August 2014

SUSANNAH V. LEVI*
Affiliation:
New York University
*
Address for correspondence: Susannah Levi, New York University, Department of Communicative Sciences and Disorders, 665 Broadway, 9th floor, New York, NY 10012.

Abstract

Research with adults has shown that spoken language processing is improved when listeners are familiar with talkers' voices, known as the familiar talker advantage. The current study explored whether this ability extends to school-age children, who are still acquiring language. Children were familiarized with the voices of three German–English bilingual talkers and were tested on the speech of six bilinguals, three of whom were familiar. Results revealed that children do show improved spoken language processing when they are familiar with the talkers, but this improvement was limited to highly familiar lexical items. This restriction of the familiar talker advantage is attributed to differences in the representation of highly familiar and less familiar lexical items. In addition, children did not exhibit accent-general learning; despite having been exposed to German-accented talkers during training, there was no improvement for novel German-accented talkers.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This work was supported by a grant from the NIH-NIDCD (1R03DC009851-01A2). I would like to thank Gabrielle Alfano, Josh Barocas, Jennifer Bruno, Stephanie Lee, Emma Mack, Alexandra Muratore, Sydney Robert, and Margo Waltz for help with data collection, Adam Buchwald and Richard Schwartz for comments on previous versions of this paper, and the children and families for their participation.

References

REFERENCES

Allen, J. S. & Miller, J. L. (2004). Listener sensitivity to individual talker differences in voice-onset-time. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 115, 3171–83.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J. & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 390412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barker, B. A. & Newman, R. S. (2004). Listen to your mother! The role of talker familiarity in infant streaming. Cognition 94, B4553.Google Scholar
Bartholomeus, B. (1973). Voice identification by nursery school children. Canadian Journal of Psychology 27(4), 464–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. (2010). lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. Online: <http://lme4.r-forge.r-project.org/>..>Google Scholar
Benkí, J. (2003). Quantitative evaluation of lexical status, word frequency, and neighborhood density as context effects in spoken word recognition. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 113(3), 1689–705.Google Scholar
Bent, T. (2014). Children's perception of foreign-accented words. Journal of Child Language (forthcoming).Google Scholar
Best, C. T. (1993). Emergence of language-specific constraints in perception of native and non-native speech: a window on early phonological development. In de Boysson, B., de Schonen, S., Jusczyk, P., McNeilage, P. & Morton, J. (eds), Developmental neurocognition: speech and face processing during the first year of life, 289304. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradlow, A. R. & Bent, T. (2008). Perceptual adaptation to non-native speech. Cognition 106(2), 707–29.Google Scholar
Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J. & Johnsen, S. K. (1997). TONI-3: test of nonverbal intelligence, 3rd ed. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Carle, E. (1969). The very hungry caterpillar. New York, NY: Philomel Books.Google Scholar
Clarke, C. M. & Garrett, M. F. (2004). Rapid adaptation to foreign-accented English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 116(6), 3647–58.Google Scholar
Clopper, C. G. & Pisoni, D. B. (2004). Effects of talker variability on perceptual learning of dialects. Language and Speech 47(3), 207–39.Google Scholar
Cortese, M. J. & Khanna, M. M. (2008). Age of acquisition ratings for 3,000 monosyllabic words. Behavior Research Methods 40(3), 791–4.Google Scholar
Creel, S. C., Aslin, R. N. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). Heeding the voice of experience: the role of talker variation in lexical access. Cognition 106, 633–64.Google Scholar
Creel, S. C. & Jimenez, S. R. (2012). Differences in talker recognition by preschoolers and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 113, 487509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisner, F. & McQueen, J. M. (2005). The specificity of perceptual learning in speech processing. Perception & Psychophysics 67, 224–38.Google Scholar
Felty, R. A. (2007). Context effects in spoken word recognition of English and German by native and non-native listeners. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Fennell, C. T. & Werker, J. F. (2003). Early word learners' ability to access phonetic detail in well-known words. Language and Speech 46(2/3), 245–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldinger, S. D., Pisoni, D. B. & Logan, J. S. (1991). On the nature of talker variability effects in recall of spoken word lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 17(1), 152–62.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 434–46.Google Scholar
Jesse, A., McQueen, J. M. & Page, M. (2007). The locus of talker-specific effects in spoken-word recognition. In Trouvain, J. & Barry, W. J. (eds), International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS 2007), 1921–4. Dudweiler: Pirrot.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W. (1986). Toward a model of the development of speech perception. In Perkell, J. S. & Klatt, D. H. (eds), Invariance and variability in speech processes, 119. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W. (1992). Developing phonological categories from the speech signal. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds), Phonological development: models, research, implications, 1764. Parkton, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Kraljic, T., Brennan, S. E. & Samuel, A. G. (2008). Accommodating variation: dialects, idiolects, and speech processing. Cognition 107, 5481.Google Scholar
Kraljic, T. & Samuel, A. G. (2007). Perceptual adjustments to multiple talkers. Journal of Memory and Language 56, 115.Google Scholar
Kučera, H. & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N. & Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science 255, 606–8.Google Scholar
Ladefoged, P. & Broadbent, D. E. (1957). Information conveyed by vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 29, 98104.Google Scholar
Levi, S. V. & Schwartz, R. G. (2013). The development of language-specific and language-independent talker processing. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 56, 913–20.Google Scholar
Levi, S. V., Winters, S. J. & Pisoni, D. B. (2007). Speaker-independent factors affecting the perception of foreign accent in a second language. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 121, 2327–38.Google Scholar
Levi, S. V., Winters, S. J. & Pisoni, D. B. (2011). Effects of cross-language voice training on speech perception: Whose familiar voices are more intelligible? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 130(6), 4053–62.Google Scholar
Mann, V. A., Diamond, R. & Carey, S. (1979). Development of voice recognition: parallels with face recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 27, 153–65.Google Scholar
Markham, D. & Hazan, V. (2004). The effect of talker- and listener-related factors on intelligibility for a real-word, open-set perception task. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 47, 725–37.Google Scholar
McLennan, C. T. & Luce, P. A. (2005). Examining the time course of indexical specificity effects in spoken word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 31(2), 306–21.Google Scholar
McQueen, J. M., Cutler, A. & Norris, D. (2006). Phonological abstraction in the mental lexicon. Cognitive Science 30(6), 1113–26.Google Scholar
Metsala, J. L. (1997). An examination of word frequency and neighborhood density in the development of spoken-word recognition. Memory & Cognition 25(1), 4756.Google Scholar
Moher, M., Feigenson, L. & Halberda, J. (2010). A one-to-one bias and fast mapping support preschoolers’ learning about faces and voices. Cognitive Science 34(5), 719–51.Google Scholar
Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D. & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 16, 519–33.Google Scholar
Nelson, P. B. & Soli, S. (2000). Acoustical barriers to learning: children at risk in every classroom. Language, Speech, and Hearing Research Services in Schools 31, 356–61.Google Scholar
Newman, R. S. & Evers, S. (2007). The effect of talker familiarity on stream segregation. Journal of Phonetics 35, 85103.Google Scholar
Norris, D., McQueen, J. M. & Cutler, A. (2003). Perceptual learning in speech. Cognitive Psychology 47, 204–38.Google Scholar
Nygaard, L. C. & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Talker-specific learning in speech perception. Perception & Psychophysics 60(3), 355–76.Google Scholar
Nygaard, L. C., Sommers, M. S. & Pisoni, D. B. (1994). Speech perception as a talker-contingent process. Psychological Science 5(1), 42–6.Google Scholar
Palmeri, T. J., Goldinger, S. D. & Pisoni, D. B. (1993). Episodic encoding of voice attributes and recognition memory for spoken words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 19(2), 309–28.Google Scholar
Samuel, A. G. & Kraljic, T. (2009). Perceptual learning for speech. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics 71(6), 1207–18.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. & Church, B. A. (1992). Auditory priming: implicit and explicit memory for words and voices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18(5), 915–30.Google Scholar
Schmale, R., Cristia, A. & Seidl, A. (2012). Toddlers recognize words in an unfamiliar accent after brief exposure. Developmental Science 15(6), 732–8.Google Scholar
Schmale, R. & Seidl, A. (2009). Accommodating variability in voice and foreign accent: flexibility of early word representations. Developmental Science 12(4), 583601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, W., Eschman, A. & Zuccolotto, A. (2007). E-Prime 2·0 Professional. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools, Inc.Google Scholar
Schroeder, M. R. (1968). Reference signal for signal quality studies. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 44, 1735–6.Google Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H. & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, 4th ed. (CELF-4). Toronto, CA: Psychological Corporation/A Harcourt Assessment Company.Google Scholar
Sidaras, S. K., Alexander, J. E. D. & Nygaard, L. C. (2009). Perceptual learning of systematic variation in Spanish-accented speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125(5), 3306–16.Google Scholar
Sjerps, M. J., Mitterer, H. & McQueen, J. M. (2011). Listening to different speakers: on the time-course of perceptual compensation for vocal-tract characteristics. Neuropsychologia 49, 3831–46.Google Scholar
Spence, M. J., Rollins, P. R. & Jerger, S. (2002). Children's recognition of cartoon voices. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 45(1), 214–22.Google Scholar
Stager, C. L. & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception tasks than in word-learning tasks. Nature 388, 381–2.Google Scholar
Storkel, H. L. (2013). A corpus of consonant–vowel–consonant real words and nonwords: comparison of phonotactic probability, neighborhood density, and consonant age of acquisition. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 11591167.Google Scholar
Storkel, H. L. & Hoover, J. R. (2010). An online calculator to compute phonotactic probability and neighborhood density on the basis of child corpora of spoken American English. Behavior Research Methods 42(2), 497506.Google Scholar
Sumby, W. H. & Pollack, I. (1954). Visual contribution to speech intelligibility in noise. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 26(2), 212–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swingley, D. & Aslin, R. N. (2000). Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children. Cognition 76, 147–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review 80(5), 352–73.Google Scholar
van Heugten, M. & Johnson, E. K. (2014). Learning to contend with accents in infancy: benefits of brief speaker exposure. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 143(1), 340350.Google Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S. & Donoso, A. (2011). Processing of indexical information requires time: evidence from change deafness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 64(8), 1484–93.Google Scholar
Walley, A. C. (1993). The role of vocabulary development in children's spoken word recognition and segmentation ability. Developmental Review 13(3), 286350.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F. & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development 7, 4963.Google Scholar
White, K. S. & Aslin, R. N. (2011). Adaptation to novel accents by toddlers. Developmental Science 14(2), 372–84.Google Scholar
White, K. S. & Morgan, J. L. (2008). Sub-segmental detail in early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 114–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, K. S., Yee, E., Blumstein, S. E. & Morgan, J. L. (2013). Adults show less sensitivity to phonetic detail in unfamiliar words, too. Journal of Memory and Language 68, 362–78.Google Scholar
Yonan, C. A. & Sommers, M. S. (2000). The effects of talker familiarity on spoken word identification in younger and older listeners. Psychology and Aging 15(1), 8899.Google Scholar