Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T19:50:33.240Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Young children's treating of utterances as unreliable sources of knowledge*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Elizabeth J. Robinson*
Affiliation:
University of Birmingham
Peter Mitchell
Affiliation:
University of Wales, Swansea
Rebecca Nye
Affiliation:
University of Birmingham
*
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK.

Abstract

In two investigations (N = 62 and 59), three- and four-year-old children sometimes disbelieved what they were told about the unexpected contents of a deceptive box, even when they had seen the adult speaker look inside the box before s/he told them what s/he saw, and despite being able to recall the utterance: utterances were treated as unreliable sources of knowledge compared with seeing directly. Those who did believe the utterance were no better at recalling their prior belief about the box's contents (now treated as false), than those who saw inside the box. However using a narrative procedure, we replicated Zaitchik's (1991) result that children are more likely to acknowledge another's belief when they are told about reality, than when they see reality for themselves. We argue that these children were acknowledging alternative rather than false belief.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The research reported here was funded by the economic and Social Research Council, UK.

References

REFERENCES

Chandler, M. (1988). Doubt and developing theories of mind. In Astington, J. W., Harris, P. L. & Olson, D. R. (eds), Developing theories of mind. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Astington, J. W. (1988). The development of children's understanding of representational change. In Astington, J. W., Harris, P. L. & Olson, D. R. (eds), Developing theories of mind. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Graf, P. (1988). Knowing how you know: young children's ability to identify and remember the sources of their beliefs. Child Development 59, 1366–71.Google Scholar
Mitchell, P., Robinson, E. J., Nye, R. M. & Isaacs, J. E. (1994). When speech conflicts with seeing: young children's understanding of informational priority. Unpublished ms.Google Scholar
Moore, C., Pure, K. & Furrow, D. (1990). Children's understanding of the modal expression of certainty and uncertainty and its relation to the development of a representational theory of mind. Child Development 61, 722–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Neill, D. K., Astington, J. W. & Flavell, J. H. (1992). Young children's understanding of the role that sensory experiences play in knowledge acquisition. Child Development 63, 474–90.Google Scholar
O'Neill, D. K. & Gopnik, A. (1991). Young children's ability to identify the sources of their beliefs. Developmental Psychology 27, 390–7.Google Scholar
Pea, R. D. (1980). The development of negation in early child language. In Olson, D. R. (ed.), The social foundations of language and thought. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (1988). Developing semantics for theories of mind: from propositional attitudes to mental representation. In Astington, J. W., Harris, P. L. & Olson, D. R. (eds), Developing theories of mind. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. London: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Leekam, S. R. & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: the case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 5, 125–37.Google Scholar
Pillow, B. H. (1993). Preschool children's understanding of the relationship between modality of perceptual access and knowledge of perceptual properties. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 11, 371–90.Google Scholar
Riggs, K. & Robinson, E. J. (in press). What people say and what they think: children's judgments of false belief in relation to their recall of false messages. British Journal of Developmental Psychology.Google Scholar
Shatz, M., Wellman, H. & Silber, S. (1983). The acquisition of mental verbs: a systematic investigation of the first reference to mental state. Cognition 14, 301–21.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child's theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. & Bartsch, K. (1988). Young children's reasoning about beliefs. Cognition 30, 239–77.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H. & Hartl, M. (1991). Against the Cartesian view on mind: young children's difficulty with own false beliefs. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 9, 125–38.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., Hogrefe, G. J. & Perner, J. (1988). Children's understanding of informational access as a source of knowledge. Child Development 59, 386–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition 13, 103–28.Google Scholar
Zaitchik, D. (1991). Is only seeing really believing? Sources of the true belief in the false belief task. Cognitive Development 6, 91103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar