Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-xh45t Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-20T06:40:22.020Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

To advance translational science, remove these roadblocks to studying team knowledge integration

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2025

Bethany K. Laursen*
Affiliation:
Michigan Institute for Clinical & Health Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
M.S. AtKisson
Affiliation:
AtKisson Training Group, LLC, Falmouth, MA, USA
Hannah B. Love
Affiliation:
Divergent Science, LLC, Fort Collins, CO, USA
Kristine Glauber
Affiliation:
Duke University School of Medicine, Clinical and Translational Science Institute, Durham, NC, USA
*
Corresponding author: B. K. Laursen; Email: laursenb@umich.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Information

Type
Perspective
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is used to distribute the re-used or adapted article and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Association for Clinical and Translational Science

When challenges such as big data, the reproducibility crisis, social determinants of health, and artificial intelligence demand knowledge synthesis across domains, how many translational teams instead end up as “fake interdisciplinary collaborations” [Reference Dai1]? Such collaborations list diverse experts on the proposal, but their team members don’t integrate each other’s expertise [Reference Dai1]. Instead, each person or sub-group works in multidisciplinary “parallel play” or performs transactional handoffs with the others. When more synthesis and co-learning is called for, such multidisciplinary strategies are not only misleading, but they are also ineffective and inefficient (Figure 1). They miss opportunities for insights and ultimately slow progress on complex health issues like obesity and teenage depression [Reference Ciesielski, Aldrich, Marsit, Hiatt and Williams2]. In a time of tightening resources, how can translational teams harness knowledge integration to accelerate breakthroughs?

Figure 1. A modern interpretation of the ancient parable of six blind men learning about an elephant without integrating their perspectives.

We don’t know yet. Fields such as the science of team science and integrative interdisciplinary studies have identified several keys to successful integration of expertise: boundary spanners, epistemic inclusivity, shared mental models, etc. [3,Reference Darbellay4]. But the recent National Academies consensus study on team science notes, “Not all recommended practices will work for every team” [3, p3]. Why? How do these strategies apply, and what impact do they have, for translational research breakthroughs?

The emerging science of translation, or translational science, seeks to answer such questions in order to remove translational research roadblocks and accelerate health solutions [Reference Austin5]. However, there are fundamental, conceptual roadblocks hindering the study of expertise integration in translational teams, creating barriers to evidence-based recommendations. There is thus a pressing need for translational scientists to overcome these conceptual roadblocks. Otherwise, translational teams and those who support them will continue to rely on hope and habit for the integrative insights that they need for innovative solutions.

Conceptual roadblock 1: What is “team science” in translational research settings?

The US National Institutes of Health (NIH) National Center for Advancing Translational Science (NCATS) lists knowledge integration as an example approach to cross-disciplinary team science [6]. In our experience, however, teamwork in health research is not often thought of as knowledge integration. Rather, it is framed as task coordination or even delegation on the way “from bench to bedside.” This is evident in NIH funding programs that require leadership or team management plans but not integrated conceptual frameworks, such as the NIH HEAL Initiative’s R15 grant [7]. By contrast, other programs call for integrated conceptual frameworks, problem statements, and research methods. Examples include the US National Science Foundation’s Manufacturing Systems Integration [8] and the Convergence Accelerator [9] and the US Department of Agriculture’s Sustainable Agricultural Systems [10].

Framing team science as primarily a coordination problem is a red herring for translational scientists: it focuses attention on member interactions rather than on the epistemic changes such interactions could create. How, when, or if coordination leads to epistemic synthesis has not been established in the translational science literature or in translational research practice.

Task coordination alone offers little support for leveraging the meanings, assumptions, and nuances that come with each member’s expertise [Reference Lin, Frey and Wu11]. When assumptions diverge, miscommunication, hard feelings, and serious mistakes are more likely [Reference Eigenbrode, O’Rourke, Wulfhorst, Althoff and Goldberg12,Reference O’Rourke, Rinkus, Cardenas, McLeskey and Gosselin13]. Lacking effective tools and strategies for bridging divergent perspectives, teams often resort to shallow interactions (multidisciplinarity), more homogenous teams (narrow interdisciplinarity), oversimplified questions, hierarchical norms, and/or voice suppression. These simplifying strategies undermine team effectiveness for complex problems by reducing psychological safety, creativity, and insight [Reference Ciesielski, Aldrich, Marsit, Hiatt and Williams2,Reference Nembhard and Edmondson14]. When studying translational research, translational scientists cannot afford to focus only on traditional project delivery strategies such as clarity of team roles or project management practices. Future translational science must uncover how such teamwork strategies are linked to epistemic synthesis and breakthroughs.

Conceptual roadblock 2: What do “integrated insights” look like for translational teams?

Some translational teams aim to answer narrow research questions, work within existing paradigms, or bundle information without synthesizing it. At times, these may be worthwhile goals, however incremental, that are appropriately addressed with multidisciplinary or narrowly interdisciplinary approaches. When teams overcome impasses in such work, translational scientists may count these moments as “integrated insights.”

By contrast, what if the translational goal is to discover interacting causal factors, reframe cross-scale problems, synthesize disparate phenomena, reveal practice-based evidence, or advise decision makers under uncertainty [Reference Ciesielski, Aldrich, Marsit, Hiatt and Williams2]? These insights require novel connections between highly diverse sources of information, and they may appear in surprising forms. If translational teams and scientists are satisfied with incremental insights, they may never even look for breakthroughs.

Conceptual roadblock 3: How can translational scientists observe and analyze insights unfolding in translational teams?

There are methodological challenges for observing and analyzing insights as they unfold in translational teams. First, what processes lead to novel insights? We have been calling it knowledge or expertise “integration” [Reference Lux, Marg, Schneider and Darbellay15], but it could be macrocognition writ large [Reference Fiore, Rosen, Smith-Jentsch, Salas, Letsky and Warner16], sense making in particular [Reference Turner, Allen, Hawamdeh and Mastanamma17], collaborative problem solving [Reference Fiore, Hall and Darbellay18], interdisciplinary learning [Reference Boix Mansilla, Frodeman, Klein and Pacheco19], interdisciplinary reasoning [Reference Laursen20], boundary spanning [Reference Klein21], or something else. The nature of the process has implications for the methods used to observe it. For example, translational scientists looking for sense making may look for expressions of uncertainty and declarations of how to proceed [Reference Dervin and Naumer22]. But if they look for boundary spanning, they may look for identity or role talk and brokering moves like translation and negotiation [Reference Martin and Ibbotson23].

Second, however the process(es) are understood, translational scientists will need to decide how to demarcate them for data collection and analysis. For example, should analysts include the social interactions (social networking, trust formation, information management, task coordination, etc.) that support epistemic blending [Reference Love, Fosdick, Cross, Suter and Egan24]? Such choices will be informed by one’s theory of what the process is. However, these theories are often underspecified, and each analyst must justify their own boundary decisions. The field has no shared understanding of what kinds of justifications would be reasonable in this space.

Third, the methodological options are wide open for most of these processes with little guidance on how to choose among them. For example, a meeting transcript can be analyzed for argumentation or for epistemic networks, and both outputs – or neither – could be taken as evidence of collaborative reasoning, since that theory is underdeveloped.

Conceptual roadblock 4: How can translational scientists discover the causal links between integrative processes and insights to guide team science interventions?

Even if translational scientists define and describe the insight-making processes likely at work and the insights these produce, that would still not uncover causal links between the processes and outcomes. Informative methods must be embedded in an overall study design that supports causal inferences. Depending on one’s philosophy of science, these designs could include randomized controlled trials (RCTs) or variations on the General Elimination Methodology. Examples include process tracing case studies and qualitative comparative analysis [Reference Scriven25]. Uncovering causal mechanisms will be essential for designing team interventions, tools, and strategies that facilitate well-calibrated integration.

Conclusion

Conceptual clarity, efficiency, and rigor are essential to build a robust evidence base of what works for translational team knowledge integration. To understand the processes that underlie knowledge integration, intrepid scholars must first dismantle the conceptual roadblocks hindering the study of integration in translational science. This work will demand a blend of theory and practice, merging fresh ideas with established ones. We invite all passionate scholars to join in tackling these roadblocks to create the evidence base for supporting science teams in authentic interdisciplinary collaboration, advancing the breakthroughs society urgently needs.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Carol Scott, Caroline Freitag, Elias Samuels, Elizabeth LaPensee, Inna Smirnova, Julie Lumeng, Julie Mennes, Kate Helegda, Michael O’Rourke, Patricia Piechowski, Shira Washington, and Tara Truax for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript. The authors used U-M GPT (https://umgpt.umich.edu) with the GPT-40 model on May 16, 2025, to draft the conclusion, which was then revised to its final form.

Author contributions

Bethany Laursen: Conceptualization, Project administration, Visualization, Writing-original draft, Writing-review & editing: M. S. AtKisson: Conceptualization, Writing-original draft, Writing-review & editing: Hannah Love: Conceptualization, Writing-original draft, Writing-review & editing: Kristine Glauber: Conceptualization, Writing-original draft, Writing-review & editing.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the US National Institutes of Health (BL, grant number UM1TR004404), (KG, grant number UL1TR002553).

Competing interests

The authors declare none.

References

Dai, L. What are fake interdisciplinary collaborations and why do they occur? Nature index . (https://www.nature.com/nature-index/news/what-are-fake-interdisciplinary-collaborations-and-why-do-they-occur) Accessed May 29, 2025.Google Scholar
Ciesielski, TH, Aldrich, MC, Marsit, CJ, Hiatt, RA, Williams, SM. Transdisciplinary approaches enhance the production of translational knowledge. Transl Res. 2017;182:123134.10.1016/j.trsl.2016.11.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. The Science and Practice of Team Science. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press, 2025.Google Scholar
Darbellay, F. Elgar encyclopedia of interdisciplinarity and transdisciplinarity. Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar Publishing; 2024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Austin, CP. Opportunities and challenges in translational science. Clin Transl Sci. 2021;14:16291647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Translational science principles. US NIH National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, (https://ncats.nih.gov/about/about-translational-science/principles) Accessed June 2, 2025.Google Scholar
US National Institutes of Health. HEAL initiative: Pain research enhancement program (PREP) (R15 clinical trial optional) , (https://grants.nih.gov/grants/guide/rfa-files/RFA-AT-25-003.html) Accessed July 18, 2025.Google Scholar
US National Science Foundation. Manufacturing systems integration (MSI) , (https://www.nsf.gov/funding/opportunities/msi-manufacturing-systems-integration) Accessed July 18, 2025.Google Scholar
US National Science Foundation. Convergence accelerator, (https://www.nsf.gov/funding/initiatives/convergence-accelerator) Accessed July 18, 2025.Google Scholar
USDA National Institute of Food and Agriculture. AFRI sustainable agricultural systems, (https://www.nifa.usda.gov/grants/programs/agriculture-food-research-initiative/afri-sustainable-agricultural-systems) Accessed July 18, 2025.Google Scholar
Lin, Y, Frey, CB, Wu, L. Remote collaboration fuses fewer breakthrough ideas. Nature. 2023;623:987991.10.1038/s41586-023-06767-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eigenbrode, SD, O’Rourke, M, Wulfhorst, JD, Althoff, DM, Goldberg, CS. Employing philosophical dialogue in collaborative science. BioSci. 2007;57:5564.10.1641/B570109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Rourke, M, Rinkus, MA, Cardenas, E, McLeskey, C. Communication practice for team science. In: Gosselin, D, ed. A practical guide for developing cross-disciplinary collaboration skills. Cham, Switzerland: Springer Nature, 2023:83102.10.1007/978-3-031-37220-9_5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nembhard, IM, Edmondson, AC. Making it safe: The effects of leader inclusiveness and professional status on psychological safety and improvement efforts in health care teams. J Organ Behav. 2006;27:941966.Google Scholar
Lux, A, Marg, O, Schneider, F. Integration. In: Darbellay, F, ed. Elgar encyclopedia of interdisciplinarity and transdisciplinarity. Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar Press, 2024:277280.Google Scholar
Fiore, SM, Rosen, MA, Smith-Jentsch, KA, Salas, E, Letsky, M, Warner, N. Toward an understanding of macrocognition in teams: predicting processes in complex collaborative contexts. Hum Factors. 2010;52:203224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, JR, Allen, J, Hawamdeh, S, Mastanamma, G. The multifaceted sensemaking theory: A systematic literature review and content analysis on sensemaking. Systems. 2023;11:145.10.3390/systems11030145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiore, SM, Hall, KL. Collaborative problem solving. In: Darbellay, F, ed. Elgar encyclopedia of interdisciplinarity and transdisciplinarity. Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar Publishing, 2024:7074.10.4337/9781035317967.ch16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boix Mansilla, V. Interdisciplinary learning: A cognitive-epistemological foundation. In: Frodeman, R, Klein, JT, Pacheco, RCS, eds. The Oxford handbook of interdisciplinarity. Second edition. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 2017:261275.Google Scholar
Laursen, BK. What is collaborative, interdisciplinary reasoning? The heart of interdisciplinary team research. Inf Sci. 2018;21:75106.Google Scholar
Klein, JT. Beyond interdisciplinarity: Boundary work, communication and collaboration. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 2021.10.1093/oso/9780197571149.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dervin, B, Naumer, CM. Sense-making. Encyclopedia of library and information sciences. Third edition. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis, 2009:26964707.Google Scholar
Martin, L, Ibbotson, P. Boundary spanning as identity work in university business engagement roles. Stud High Educ. 2021;46:12721284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Love, HB, Fosdick, BK, Cross, JE, Suter, M, Egan, D. Towards understanding the characteristics of successful and unsuccessful collaborations: A case-based team science study. Humanit soc sci commun. 2022;9:371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scriven, M. A summative evaluation of RCT methodology & an alternative approach to causal research. J Multidiscip Eval. 2008;5:1124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. A modern interpretation of the ancient parable of six blind men learning about an elephant without integrating their perspectives.