Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T07:15:04.090Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Making sense of voting “habits”: Applying the process model of behavior change to a series of large-scale get-out-the-vote experiments

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2023

John Ternovski*
Affiliation:
McCourt School of Public Policy at Georgetown University, Washington, DC, USA Office of Labor and Economic Analysis, U.S. Air Force Academy, Colorado Springs, CO, USA
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

I apply a new theoretical framework to voting to more cohesively bridge the economic cost-benefit model of voting with the psychology-motivated voting-as-a-habit literature. This new theoretical frame gives greater clarity as to how a vote in one election might beget a vote in another election, while yielding testable predictions as to which circumstances are more favorable for developing turnout persistence. To test these predictions, I make use of a novel dataset consisting of nine large-N, door-to-door voter mobilization field experiments in various election contexts (with ∼1.8 million voters in total). Consistent with prior empirical research, my analysis finds that being nudged to vote in one election leads to increased turnout four years later. But the main contribution of this paper is that the theoretical framework’s predictions and the corresponding empirical results make sense of turnout persistence heterogeneities that have been detected in certain prior empirical studies but not others.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of American Political Science Association

Introduction

There is consensus among empiricists in American politics that voting is habit-forming (Aldrich, Montgomery, and Wood, Reference Aldrich, Montgomery and Wood2011; Coppock and Green, Reference Coppock and Green2016; Denny and Doyle, Reference Denny and Doyle2009; Franklin and Hobolt, Reference Franklin and Hobolt2011; Garcia Bedolla and Michelson, Reference Garcia Bedolla and Michelson2012; Gerber, Green and Shachar, Reference Gerber, Green and Shachar2003; Meredith, Reference Meredith2009; Solvak and Vassil, Reference Solvak and Vassil2018). But there are key components of the theoretical framework that are, as yet, underdeveloped. For one, a voting “habit” does not satisfy psychologists’ definition of habit as an action that becomes automatic from continual repetition and a stable context (Danner, Aarts, and De Vries, Reference Danner, Aarts and De Vries2008; Lally and Gardner, Reference Lally and Gardner2013; Moors and De Houwer, Reference Moors and De Houwer2006; Wood, Tam, and Witt, Reference Wood, Tam and Witt2005; Wood, Quinn, and Kashy, Reference Wood, Quinn and Kashy2002) – the long gaps of time between elections and different election contexts preclude voting from becoming truly automatic (Dinas, Reference Dinas2012). More pressing is that there is insufficient existing research (for an overview, see Coppock and Green, Reference Coppock and Green2016) to provide guidance why voting is habit-forming in some circumstances but not in others. To address these issues, I apply a new theoretical framework, the Process Model for Behavior Change (PMBC) (Duckworth and Gross, Reference Duckworth and Gross2020), to voting in order to more cohesively bridge the economic cost-benefit model of voting with the psychology-motivated voting-as-a-habit literature.

This new theoretical frame gives greater clarity as to how a vote in one election might beget a vote in the future, while yielding testable predictions as to which circumstances are more favorable for developing turnout persistence. The PMBC first requires that an individual’s attention be triggered by some environmental cue about Election Day. Then, the individual deliberates whether to vote, which can be represented by a cost-benefit equation (e.g., DellaVigna et al., Reference DellaVigna, List, Malmendier and Rao2016; Riker and Ordeshook, Reference Riker and Ordeshook1968). However, the PMBC framework also notes that this deliberative stage can be skipped via a heuristic shortcut. To increase the likelihood of triggering a heuristic shortcut in downstream elections, the attention cues should be similar to that of the last election that individual voted in. As such, turnout persistence across different election types (presidential elections vs. local elections) and saliencies (competitive vs. non-competitive) should be less likely. Finally, heuristic shortcuts are made when the individual obtains positive reinforcement for their past behavior. So, in an election context, an individual may feel that they made the “right” decision in turning out to vote if their preferred candidate won.

To test these predictions, I make use of a novel dataset consisting of nine door-to-door voter mobilization field experiments in various election contexts (with ∼1.8 million voters in total). Consistent with prior empirical research, my analysis finds that being nudged to vote in one election does lead to increased turnout four years later. But the main contribution of this paper is that the PMBC predictions and the corresponding empirical results make sense of turnout persistence heterogeneities that have been detected in certain prior empirical studies but not others: some studies have found that being turned out to vote in a low-salience election upstream still led to downstream turnout persistence (Coppock and Green Reference Coppock and Green2016; Garcia Bedolla and Michelson Reference Garcia Bedolla and Michelson2012) but other studies did not (Hill and Kousser Reference Hill and Kousser2016; Michelson Reference Michelson2003). In the same vein, Coppock and Green (Reference Coppock and Green2016), Hill and Kousser (Reference Hill and Kousser2016), Michelson (Reference Michelson2003), Green and Shachar (Reference Green and Shachar2000) found stronger turnout persistence in elections of the same type (e.g., a midterm election followed by a presidential election), while Garcia Bedolla and Michelson (Reference Garcia Bedolla and Michelson2012) and Gerber, Green, and Shachar (Reference Gerber, Green and Shachar2003) did not.Footnote 1

In the largest experimental analysis of turnout persistence to date, I find that being induced to vote in low-salience elections does not lead to voting in downstream high-salience elections, but being induced to vote in high-salience elections had large turnout persistence effects in downstream high-salience elections. Similarly, I find that turnout persistence exists for elections of the same type but not for disparate elections. Finally, I find suggestive evidence that voting in an election where one’s (likely) preferred candidate won is associated with larger turnout persistence effects.

The theory

The Process Model of Behavior Change (PMBC) (Duckworth and Gross, Reference Duckworth and Gross2020) allows us to make sense of persistent voting behaviors and why they occur when they do. The PMBC stipulates that any behavior occurs as part of a recursive cycle of four stages (Figure 1, left). First, we encounter a situation (e.g., it is Election Day). Some element(s) of the situation demand our attention (e.g., seeing news coverage of the upcoming election). We appraise the situation (i.e., apply our personal cost-benefit equationFootnote 2 to determine if voting is worth the effort) and pick the appropriate response (i.e., vote or not). But this model also emphasizes that because appraisal is cognitively taxing, there exist shortcuts past the Appraisal Stage (Figure 1, right). To be specific, this cognitive shortcut economizes “cognitive effort… because we have responded the same way in the same context and gotten a similar reward” (Duckworth and Gross Reference Duckworth and Gross2020, 41). Past voting causing future voting would mean voters are sidestepping the Appraisal Stage and not weighing the costs and benefits of voting.

Figure 1. PMBC model as applied to voting with (right) and without turnout persistence (left).

While repetition is necessary to make voting habitual, because the PMBC is a process-driven model, we can focus on the mechanism behind the development of shortcuts past the Appraisal Stage. We do not need to adjudicate whether persistent voting is a “habit” in the strict psychological sense.Footnote 3 Rather, one can construe this shortcut as a failure to deliberate over whether an action is worth taking (e.g., “I voted in the last election and I was happy that I did, so I may as well vote again”). This is distinct from a deliberative thought process that has the individual thinking about how much time they’ll wait in line at the polling place, the times someone praised them for voting, etc. The cognitive shortcut is a failure to even cursorily engage in the specifics of a cost-benefit assessment. And so, the PMBC model allows us to identify three elements that are necessary for voting to persist:

  1. 1. An individual’s attention must be triggered by some environmental cue about Election Day (i.e., the Attention Stage).

  2. 2. Previous voting experience must validate that the voting calculus in the Appraisal Stage was correct. (i.e., the cost-benefit calculation should clearly show that the voter made the “right” choice in showing up at the polls.)

  3. 3. There must be positive reinforcement as voting behavior is repeated for the voter to begin to skip the Appraisal Stage and show up at the polls as a cognitive shortcut.

These necessary conditions yield threeFootnote 4 testable predictions:

Prediction 1: Voters induced to vote in one election type (e.g., presidential) will exhibit stronger turnout persistence in future elections of the same type. To bypass the Appraisal Stage, the contextual cues in the Attention Stage need to be similar to previous instances where the outcome of the Situation Stage was positive. While the difference in election types is most clear between local and federal elections, even presidential and midterm elections are different in salience,Footnote 5 who votes,Footnote 6 how campaigns spend money,Footnote 7 and which voters campaigns target (Garcia Bedolla and Michelson, Reference Garcia Bedolla and Michelson2012). Although early experimental voting-as-a-habit literature did not anticipate differences across election types (e.g., Green and Gerber, Reference Green and Gerber2002; Gerber, Green, and Shachar Reference Gerber, Green and Shachar2003), empirical differences in turnout persistence across election contexts were detected but limited by sample size (e.g., Garcia Bedolla and Michelson Reference Garcia Bedolla and Michelson2012); larger-scale analyses presented empirical evidence of stronger turnout persistence across elections of the same type but did not present a theory to explain the peculiarity (Coppock and Green Reference Coppock and Green2016). As such, though election type incorporates many interrelated characteristics, it is prominent in the turnout persistence literature and presents a replicable benchmark for PMBC predictions.

Prediction 2: Voters induced to vote in a low-salience election will not exhibit stronger turnout persistence in higher-salience elections. Footnote 8 This prediction is closely related to Prediction 1, but isolates salience from other characteristics inherent in election types. Election salience has been used extensively in developing a theory of voter behavior (e.g., Arceneaux and Nickerson, Reference Arceneaux and Nickerson2009; Malhotra et al. Reference Malhotra, Michelson, Rogers and Valenzuela2011), as it proxies how much election information and advertising a voter is exposed to (e.g., Hernandez, Anduiza, Rico, Reference Hernandez, Anduiza and Rico2021). Prior research on turnout persistence in this context has produced inconsistent results (e.g., Bedolla and Michelson’s (Reference Garcia Bedolla and Michelson2012) downstream Complier Average Causal Effects ranged from −0.82 to 1.59). The absence of turnout persistence after low-salience upstream elections has generally been ascribed to methodological limitations, such as insufficient sample size or upstream effects that were too small (see Coppock and Green’s (Reference Coppock and Green2016) review).

The PMBC can help us make sense of these inconsistencies. The contextual cues in high-salience and low-salience information environments are likely different: talking to a local candidate who is canvassing the neighborhood for the first time is a novel cue, whereas a co-worker talking about the upcoming presidential election may be a recurring cue.Footnote 9 Novel cues are more likely to trigger a re-appraisal of the cost-benefit equation and thus make heuristic decision-making less likely.

Prediction 3: Voting in an election where one’s chosen candidate won should be positively associated with turnout persistence. The PMBC model stipulates that positive reinforcement is key to strengthening the heuristic shortcut past the Appraisal Stage. As such, casting a vote in an election where your preferred candidate wins should provide a larger “warm glow” than individuals who voted and their candidate lost.Footnote 10 Since a larger warm glow term makes it more likely that the outcome of the cost-benefit analysis in the Appraisal Stage is positive, the individual would get some positive reinforcement, which should make subsequent voting in an election with similar context cues more likely.Footnote 11 The impact of election outcome on turnout persistence has generally not been addressed in prior research,Footnote 12 but this prediction is broadly consistent with the “winner effect” literature, which finds that voters’ sentiments about the democratic process improve when the participant had voted for the winning candidate (e.g., Sinclair, Smith, and Tucker, Reference Sinclair, Smith and Tucker2018).

Evaluating the predictions empirically

To assess these predictions, I build on Garcia Bedolla and Michelson’s pioneering (Reference Garcia Bedolla and Michelson2012) research on the impacts of real-world GOTV programs. I partnered with a US labor organization that runs door-to-door outreach programs with millions of working-class Americans. The organization’s sampling criteria were based on their election goals and logistical availability.Footnote 13, Footnote 14 The organization randomized a percentage of their program sample to an uncontacted control group in nine separate field experiments. The experiments were conducted in 8 different states and included five different election contexts, with a total of over 1.8 million households. For a summary of the experiments, see Table 1.Footnote 15 All subjects were matched to subsequent elections’ voter files.

Table 1. Overview of all experiments

These data were initially analyzed as part of a consulting project but the subgroups associated with the predictions in this paper were never assessed. Because I am not able to provide objective of evidence that this is the case, I decided that submitting a pre-analysis plan may appear misleading. For a comprehensive discussion of my decision to not preregister, please see SM, Section 4. In lieu of a pre-analysis plan, I attempt to maximize data and analytical transparency and emphasize the need to replicate these findings.

The initial upstream effect appears small at just 0.3 percentage points (p < 0.001), but it is nearly identical to effects found in similarly high-salience election contexts (Gerber and Green, Reference Green and Gerber2019; see SM, Section 5.1 for more details). To analyze turnout persistence, I use the analysis strategy in Coppock and Green (Reference Coppock and Green2016), where V 1 is defined as voting in the upstream election and V 2 is voting in a downstream election, Z is an indicator denoting whether or not the individual was assigned to receive a doorknock GOTV treatment at time 1. The main estimand of interest is a Complier Average Causal Effect (CACE) – the effect of voting in an upstream election on downstream voting among those who vote because they receive the GOTV doorknock. The estimator is thus ${\widehat {CACE}} = \;{{\hat E{\rm{[}}{V_{2i}}{\rm{|}}{Z_i} = 1\left] { - \;\hat E{\rm{[}}{V_{2i}}{\rm{|}}{Z_i} = 0} \right]} \over {\hat E{\rm{[}}{V_{1i}}{\rm{|}}{Z_i} = 1\left] { - \;\hat E{\rm{[}}{V_{1i}}{\rm{|}}{Z_i} = 0} \right]}}$ for every household i in the experiment. This is estimated via two-stage least squares as is standard practice (Angrist, Imbens, and Rubin, Reference Angrist, Imbens and Rubin1996; Landau and Emsley, Reference Landau and Emsley2020).

Since the initial canvasses were conducted anywhere from 2014 to 2017, the only downstream election for which I can use all of the data is the 2018 general election. Footnote 16,Footnote 17 When I regress data from all experiments (with experiment fixed effects and robust standard errors) in a 2SLS regression with voting in the 2018 general election as the instrumented variable, I estimate a CACE of 0.57 (p = 0.004). Footnote 18, Footnote 19 In other words, of those voters successfully turned out upstream, over 57% turned out again in the 2018 general election. However, the key contribution of this study is that the variety of election contexts and the large sample size allow for the testing the circumstances in which turnout persistence is more (or less) likely to occur. Table 2 summarizes my predictions, my empirical findings, and the empirical findings of previous experimental studies that looked at similar populations of voters.

Table 2. Summary of predictions and results across multiple studies

* = One set of studies did yield results that were consistent with my prediction, but a second set of studies did not. The authors believe the second set failed to replicate due to inadequate statistical power. ** = These studies only had data on the effect of voting in one election type upstream on voting in a different type of election downstream. Thus, we are unable to determine if the effects they find are higher for elections of the same type (i.e., we have no counterfactual within study).

Prediction 1: Voters induced to vote in one election type will exhibit stronger turnout persistence in future elections of the same type. In my data, there are four cases where the upstream and downstream election is of the same type (federal midterm). Footnote 20 In Table 3, I present the initial upstream turnout effect in the first column; in the next two columns, I show downstream CACEs of disparate election types (midterm-on-presidential-primary and midterm-on-presidential), and in the final column, I present the CACE of like election types (midterm-on-midterm). Downstream voting persistence among compliers was small-to-null in the presidential elections, Footnote 21 but was very strong in the following midterm election (96% of voters who were successfully turned out by the canvass voted four years later (p = 0.006)). My data, therefore, comport with the PMBC model, Coppock and Green (Reference Coppock and Green2016), and the pioneering observational work by Aldrich, Montgomery, and Wood (Reference Aldrich, Montgomery and Wood2011).

Table 3. Individuals who were successfully turned out to vote in 2014 midterm elections continued to vote in 2018 midterm but not in 2016 presidential elections

First column denotes the upstream ITT effect estimated via OLS regression. Subsequent columns estimate CACE for different downstream elections. Robust standard errors in parentheses. *** = p-value ≤ 0.001, ** = p ≤ 0.01.

Prediction 2: Voters induced to vote in a low-salience election will not exhibit stronger turnout persistence in higher-salience elections. A common proxy for election salience is actual turnout on Election Day (e.g., Coppock and Green, Reference Coppock and Green2016; Rolfe, Reference Rolfe2012). The 2015 PA Supreme Court election and the 2015 Philadelphia Mayoral Primary clearly qualify with turnout well under 30%. Footnote 22 As seen in Table 4, the canvass in Pennsylvania and Philadelphia had an upstream treatment effect size that was about three times as large as that of the rest of the experiments (0.85 percentage points versus 0.26 percentage points); nevertheless, we find that there were no lasting downstream effects among compliers (the first row cell in light gray). In contrast, nearly 90% of participants induced to vote by the initial GOTV contact in the remaining states turned out downstream in the 2018 midterm election (p = 0.004). These results are highly robust to other definitions of salience (e.g., no president/governor/senate race on ballot); see SM, Section 5.4 for more details.

Table 4. Salience and downstream CACEs

This table examines the impact of voting in the elections described in each row on downstream turnout in the 2018 midterm election. Robust standard errors in parentheses. *** = p-value ≤ 0.001, ** = p ≤ 0.01.

Prediction 3: Voting in an election where one’s chosen candidate won should be positively associated with turnout persistence formation. While we are not able to determine how positive an experience a voter had at the polls, we do have one observable variable: who won the election. Although it is not possible to know who a given participant voted for, the targeting criteria of our partner organization are overwhelmingly Democratic. So, for this analysis, I remove registered Republicans and assume individuals with missingFootnote 23 registration data prefer the Democratic candidate to win. If this assumption is violated, we will be less likely to detect an effect in elections where the Democratic candidate won, which makes for a conservative test of Prediction 3.

I find that participating in an upstream election where the Democratic candidate won was associated with persistent downstream effects among compliers four years later and no turnout persistence in states where the Republican won (see Table 5). One concern is that the election outcome may not be truly exogenous, but Eggers et al. (Reference Eggers, Fowler, Hainmueller, Hall and Snyder2015) found compelling evidence that the outcomes of close elections are as-if random.Footnote 24 Three of the four states in this analysis were top-of-the-ticket tossups (IL 2014, MI 2014, NC 2016) and Iowa 2014 had a competitive down-ballot race.Footnote 25 It is also possible that this result is driven by a difference in voter identification laws (i.e., more stringent voter ID requirements may make voters’ experience at the polls less positive). However, the voting identification laws are similar across the two groups of states.Footnote 26 Still, these results are only based on a comparison of two sets of experiments and there may be other unobserved confounders.Footnote Footnote 28 For instance, one plausible confounder is the baseline turnout propensities across these two groups are markedly different (see SM, Table 7.5). Given these data limitations, the analysis of Prediction 3 is the most exploratory and should be replicated before generalizing from this result.

Table 5. Successfully being turned out to vote in the elections where the voters’ (likely) preferred candidate won makes those voters more likely to vote in 2018 midterm election Footnote 27

First column denotes the upstream ITT effect estimated via OLS regression. Subsequent columns estimate CACE for different downstream elections. Robust standard errors in parentheses. ** = p ≤ 0.001, ** = p ≤ 0.01, * = p ≤ 0.05.

Discussion and conclusions

My results indicate that the PMBC theoretical framework may provide additional insight into why turnout persistence forms in some contexts but not others. Its predictions validate in a novel large-scale dataset, but since the predictions were not preregistered before the data were collected, it is imperative to replicate these results in future studies and assess some of the alternative explanations that might yield the same results.

One possible alternative explanation for turnout persistence, particularly for Prediction 1 (persistence across similar election types) is raised by what Rogers and Frey (Reference Rogers and Frey2014) termed “rip currents,” where compliance to an upstream GOTV intervention leads to subsequent attention from campaigns and non-profits. It’s not that there is a permanent change to an individual’s cost-benefit equation or the act of voting creates a self-reinforcing intrinsic impetus to vote; rather, the rip currents hypothesis claims that the people who are successfully nudged to the polls by a campaign are subsequently targeted by more campaigns and non-profits with nudges that are similarly successful in inducing these individuals to vote in downstream elections.

The few existing studies on this subject suggest that this is unlikely to be the main driver behind turnout persistence. Turnout persistence isn’t more pronounced in battleground states (Coppock and Green, Reference Coppock and Green2016) and since battleground states attract more campaign activity, those voters who are successfully mobilized in an upstream election should receive more campaign attention under the rip current hypothesis. Footnote 29 Rogers et al. (Reference Rogers, Green, Ternovski and Young2017) explicitly analyzed whether campaigns, in downstream elections, were more likely to contact participants who were in an upstream treatment group than participants in an upstream control group. The study merged all subjects of a 2011 GOTV experiment to two databases of 2016 campaign contact information. Footnote 30 While Rogers et al. (Reference Rogers, Green, Ternovski and Young2017) did find modest increases in downstream contact of the 2011 treatment group, the differences were so small that they concluded that turnout persistence “cannot plausibly be attributed to the treatment and control groups’ differential exposure to mobilization activity.” (p. 92). Footnote 31

That said, repeated campaign contact may be key to building voting persistence in the PMBC model: the question is whether the subsequent campaign contact affects the Appraisal Stage (the cost-benefit equation of voting) or simply calls attention to an upcoming election. Future research should attempt to disentangle these effects; one possible design would randomize continual informational treatments (e.g., identical text messages reminding an individual of an upcoming Election Day every election) versus a one-off, heavy-touch GOTV intervention. PMBC would predict that while the information treatment should have a lower upstream effect, compliers in the informational treatment should develop stronger turnout persistence years later (as compared to compliers in the heavy-touch condition).

An alternative explanation for Prediction 2 (weak turnout persistence after mobilization in low-salience elections) is that voters in low-salience elections were already high-propensity voters and they “complied” with the treatment because they were reminded of a lesser known election (e.g., Dale and Strauss, Reference Dale and Strauss2009). Since these are already high-propensity voters, they may be more likely to encounter ceiling effects downstream.Footnote 32 However, downstream 2018 midterm turnout in the control groups of the 2015 PA and the 2015 Philadelphia studies (low-salience elections) was 72.3% versus 73.8% among the control groups in the high-salience elections. This indicates that the high-salience and low-salience groups had comparable baseline turnout propensities downstream. In other words, we see that voters had high turnout treatment effects upstream in low-salience elections (perhaps because they were reminded of that election), but those compliers did not continue to vote in subsequent elections. On the other hand, compliers with comparable baseline turnout propensities downstream who were successfully turned out in more high-salience elections upstream, continued to vote in the 2018 midterm at very high rates. It is, however, possible that the same treatment script works differently in low-salience versus high-salience elections. It may be that in low-salience elections, the noticeable reminder (Dale and Strauss, Reference Dale and Strauss2009) resonates with voters (without developing habit), but in high-salience elections, it is plan-making (which may help develop habit).

PMBC also overlaps with existing theoretical frameworks – particularly in regard to Prediction 3 (being more likely to vote in a subsequent election after one’s preferred candidate won). For one, the winning candidate may be able to provide more resources to that voter. Even if there is a lack of “rip currents” (i.e., subsequent campaign contact), the winning campaign may increase that voter’s resources by implementing policies favorable to the voterFootnote 33 or otherwise lowering the cost of voting for supporters but not opponents. Another explanation may come from the robust “winner effect” literature, where voters who voted for the winning candidate feel more strongly about electoral and democratic legitimacy, perception of fairness, political efficacy, and other variables more closely related to the Appraisal Stage than habit (e.g., Sinclair, Smith, and Tucker, Reference Sinclair, Smith and Tucker2018). A better test of Prediction 3 might involve nonpartisan, low-salience ballot initiatives – are supporters of such winning ballot initiatives more likely to exhibit turnout persistence?

Another related alternative explanation is that voting starts a self-reinforcing process (e.g., Rogers and Frey Reference Rogers and Frey2014). This explanation is related to the informational reductions in transaction costs (e.g., learning the best time to go to one’s local polling place), inflating the warm glow of voting (as in Sinclair, Smith, and Tucker, Reference Sinclair, Smith and Tucker2018), or the social benefits of voting. Under this explanation, the economic model is sufficient to explain turnout persistence without the need for PMBC. However, it is difficult to reconcile this mechanism with the finding that turnout persistence fades faster for some upstream interventions (e.g., social pressure (Davenport et al., Reference Davenport, Gerber, Green, Larimer, Mann and Panagopoulos2010)) but not others (Coppock and Green, Reference Coppock and Green2016). If turnout persistence is always driven by a self-reinforcing adjustment of costs, we would expect that the specifics of the initial motivation for voting shouldn’t affect the longevity of effects.Footnote 34

There may still be other explanations that do not comport with the PMBC model and in no way should my empirical assessment be viewed as conclusive evidence that turnout persistence is explained solely by the PMBC theoretical framework. The goal of this paper is to present a new theoretical framework with empirical evidence illustrating this framework’s value. Future studies should directly test the predictions that come out of my application of PMBC to turnout persistence. Specifically, I predict that to increase the chances of developing turnout persistence, the following conditions should hold. First, the contextual cues in one election should be as similar as possible in future elections. This implies that organizations tasked with increasing turnout may want to attempt using the same prompt to remind voters that it’s Election Day from one election to the next. It’s also important that voters successfully persuaded to turn out to the polls have a net positive experience voting. This makes it more likely that a voter will begin to skip the Appraisal Stage and adopt a cognitive shortcut in response to a contextual cue. The implication is the voters who did not have a positive experience voting (e.g., long lines, their preferred candidate lost) may benefit from receiving a different contextual cue next election to stop the development of a non-voting habit.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/XPS.2023.25

Data availability statement

The data, code, and any additional materials required to replicate all analyses in this article are available at the Journal of Experimental Political Science Dataverse within the Harvard Dataverse Network, at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/G07AO1

Competing interests

Data were shared after the contractual relationship between the author and the partner organization was concluded. As such, there were no conflicts of interest.

Ethics statement

A de-identified dataset was shared by a partner organization with the author. As such, since there is no Personally Identifiable Information (PII), this was not human subject research and did not require IRB review.

Footnotes

The views expressed in this article are those of the author and not necessarily those of the United States Air Force Academy, the Air Force, the Department of Defense, or the U.S. Government.

This article has earned badges for transparent research practices: Open Data and Open Materials. For details see the Data Availability Statement.

1 For a detailed analysis of existing research through the PMBC lens, please see the Supplementary Materials (SM), Sections 1 and 2.

2 For more details, see SM, Section 1.

3 For a more details, see SM, Section 2.

4 There is a fourth highly exploratory prediction on how household income can affect turnout persistence included in SM, Section 5.3.

5 Presidential elections have an information environment that is more crowded than that of midterms – driven at least partly by substantial differences in campaign spending (e.g., Jackson Reference Jackson2000, Reference Jackson2002).

6 For instance, younger voters are much less likely to vote in the midterms than in presidential elections (Leighley and Nagler, Reference Leighley and Nagler2018).

7 For instance, both 2012 presidential campaigns allocated substantially more money to campaign ads than the average Senate campaign in 2014 (Koerth, Reference Koerth2018).

8 I did not have sufficient data to test the converse (cf., Gerber, Green, and Shachar (Reference Gerber, Green and Shachar2003), Cutts, Fieldhouse, and John (Reference Cutts, Fieldhouse and John2009) and a subset of Garcia Bedolla and Michelson’s (Reference Garcia Bedolla and Michelson2012) experiments).

9 Campaigns recognize these differences: “[i]n high-salience elections, campaigns target unlikely voters out of the belief that everyone else is going to vote without their encouragement, whereas in low-salience elections they assume the opposite and focus on those voters who have reliably voted in the past” (Arceneaux and Nickerson, Reference Arceneaux and Nickerson2009; p. 5).

10 Some voters may experience a warm glow from the act of voting alone (e.g., third-party voters), but we are unable to observe the utility a voter assigns to their experience at the polls. Instead, we must rely on observable differences between election contexts and a set of assumptions about how those differing election contexts might reasonably affect voter utility. For a more extensive discussion of the “warm glow” variable in the calculus of voting, see SM, Section 1.

11 There are alternative theoretical frameworks that speak to these same predictions (see the Discussion and Conclusions section).

12 The one exception is Fujiwara, Meng, and Vogl (Reference Fujiwara, Meng and Vogl2016), but they address this prediction indirectly (via perception of pivotality rather than actual election outcome).

13 The geography had to be dense enough to warrant the cost of door-to-door canvassing.

14 Since PMBC is designed to be broadly portable across different types of people and contexts, PMBC predictions need not be tested in a nationally representative sample.

15 For more details on the data used, please see SM, Section 3.

16 2018 primary turnout was not available as part of this project.

17 It is possible to analyze downstream CACEs for 2016 primary and general elections for a smaller subset of my data, as seen below.

18 With and without covariates.

19 Since the initial upstream effect is small, it is important to check for weak instruments (e.g., Andrews, Stock, and Sun Reference Andrews, Stock and Sun2019). For a full analysis of weak instruments, see SM, Section 5.2. In short, one experiment, Washington 2014, had an upstream treatment effect indistinguishable from zero (p = 0.95) and is omitted from all subsequent analyses, as it would bias turnout persistence upward. Note that including the Washington data does not substantially change the reported results.

20 I omit the 2016 North Carolina presidential experiment from this comparison as I do not have data for the 2020 presidential election. That said, the downstream effect in North Carolina on the following midterm election was not significant (p = 0.277), but the magnitude was high (0.70). It is likely this analysis is underpowered and so the results do not provide clear evidence against or in support of Prediction 1.

21 Note that there is a somewhat higher (but non-significant) CACE in the presidential primary, which may be driven by lower baseline turnout as predicted in Coppock and Green (Reference Coppock and Green2016).

22 Local media coverage of these election has called turnout “low” (Holmberg, Reference Holmberg2015) and “bad” (Kerkestra Reference Kerkestra2015).

23 Party registration is missing for all voters in IL and MI, so limiting the sample to only registered Democrats is not feasible.

24 As a result, outcomes of close races have been used to estimate causal impacts of incumbency (e.g., Ariga et al. Reference Ariga, Horiuchi, Mansilla and Umeda2016; Kendall and Rekas Reference Kendall and Rekkas2012; Lee Reference Lee2008).

25 The results hold even if I omit Iowa 2014 (NC 2016 CACE = −0.02, p = 0.989).

26 In Iowa and Michigan, an ID is requested and, if it is not presented, voters must sign an oath or affidavit verifying their identity and are allowed to cast a regular ballot; North Carolina and Illinois both do not require any form of ID to vote (NCSL 2023).

27 Since the election context is different in the 2017 Virginia experiment, I exclude it from this comparison. However, adding the VA data results in a highly similar CACE of 1.31 (p = 0.02).

28 There is evidence that the Dem Win subgroup suffers from a weak instrument (see SM, Section 5.2). As is recommended in e.g., Staiger and Stock (Reference Staiger and Stock1997), I use Anderson-Rubin weak-instrument-robust tests. These tests find evidence of turnout persistence in the Dem Win subsample (p = 0.001) and no turnout persistence in the Dem Loss subsample (p-value = 0.503). This is consistent with the reported CACEs.

29 Coppock and Green (Reference Coppock and Green2016) also provide an overview of other studies that looked at subsequent campaign contact across upstream treatment conditions; Dinas (Reference Dinas2012) and Green, McGrath, and Aronow (Reference Green, McGrath and Aronow2013) found very modest effects of treatment on subsequent campaign contact.

30 The first database was maintained by the Obama for America campaign (which ran Barak Obama’s campaign in 2008 and 2012) and the second, by Catalist, a Democratic-leaning data clearinghouse that tracked voter contact data of several large Democratic-leaning non-profits (Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Green, Ternovski and Young2017).

31 The only sizable increase came in the form of direct mail (8.1 percentage points more in the treatment group), which is unlikely to explain the entirety of turnout persistence. A meta-analysis of direct mail outreach finds a 0.5 percentage point impact; even the most effective intervention (social pressure) has, on average, only a 2 percentage point impact on turnout (Green and Gerber Reference Green and Gerber2019; p. 214).

32 The issue of ceiling effects could also affect the analysis in Prediction 1. I address this possibility in SM, Section 6.

33 As recent studies like Costa (Reference Costa2021) show, despite recent increases in partisan polarization, voters still vote according to policy preferences.

34 For a more comprehensive discussion of how different intervention types – and particularly social pressure – might affect turnout persistence, please see SM, Sections 2 and 3.

References

Arceneaux, K. and Nickerson, D. W.. 2009. “Who is Mobilized to Vote? A Re-Analysis of 11 Field Experiments.” American Journal of Political Science 53(1): 116.10.1111/j.1540-5907.2008.00354.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aldrich, J. H., Montgomery, J. M. and Wood, W.. 2011. “Turnout as a Habit.” Political Behavior, 33(4): 535–63.10.1007/s11109-010-9148-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, I., Stock, J. H. and Sun, L.. 2019. “Weak Instruments in Instrumental Variables Regression: Theory and Practice.” Annual Review of Economics 11: 727–53.10.1146/annurev-economics-080218-025643CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angrist, J. D., Imbens, G. W. and Rubin, D. B.. 1996. “Identification of Causal Effects Using Instrumental Variables.” Journal of the American Statistical Association 91(434): 444–55.10.1080/01621459.1996.10476902CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ariga, K., Horiuchi, Y., Mansilla, R. and Umeda, M.. 2016. “No Sorting, No Advantage: Regression Discontinuity Estimates of Incumbency Advantage in Japan.” Electoral Studies 43: 2131.10.1016/j.electstud.2016.03.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppock, A. and Green, D. P.. 2016. “Is Voting Habit Forming? New Evidence from Experiments and Regression Discontinuities.” American Journal of Political Science 60(4): 1044–62.10.1111/ajps.12210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, M.. 2021. “Ideology, Not Affect: What Americans Want from Political Representation.” American Journal of Political Science 65(2): 342–58.10.1111/ajps.12571CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutts, D., Fieldhouse, E. and John, P.. 2009. “Is Voting Habit Forming? The Longitudinal Impact of a GOTV Campaign in the UK.” Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties, 19(3): 251–63.10.1080/17457280903073914CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dale, A. and Strauss, A.. 2009. “Don’t Forget to Vote: Text Message Reminders as a Mobilization Tool.” American Journal of Political Science 53(4): 787804.10.1111/j.1540-5907.2009.00401.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danner, U. N., Aarts, H. and De Vries, N. K.. 2008. “Habit vs. Intention in the Prediction of Future Behaviour: The Role of Frequency, Context Stability and Mental Accessibility of Past Behaviour.” British Journal of Social Psychology 47(2): 245–65.10.1348/014466607X230876CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davenport, T. C., Gerber, A. S., Green, D. P., Larimer, C. W., Mann, C. B. and Panagopoulos, C.. 2010. “The Enduring Effects of Social Pressure: Tracking Campaign Experiments over a Series of Elections.” Political Behavior 32(3): 423–30.10.1007/s11109-010-9122-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DellaVigna, S., List, J. A., Malmendier, U. and Rao, G.. 2016. “Voting to Tell Others.” The Review of Economic Studies 84(1): 143–81.10.1093/restud/rdw056CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denny, K. and Doyle, O.. 2009. “Does Voting History Matter? Analysing Persistence in Turnout.” American Journal of Political Science 53(1): 1735.10.1111/j.1540-5907.2008.00355.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinas, E. 2012. “The Formation of Voting Habits.” Journal of Elections, Public Opinion & Parties 22(4): 431–56.10.1080/17457289.2012.718280CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duckworth, A. L. and Gross, J. J.. 2020. “Behavior Change.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 161: 3949.10.1016/j.obhdp.2020.09.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eggers, A. C., Fowler, A., Hainmueller, J., Hall, A. B. and Snyder, J. M. Jr.. 2015. “On the Validity of the Regression Discontinuity Design for Estimating Electoral Effects: New Evidence from Over 40,000 Close Races.” American Journal of Political Science 59(1): 259–74.10.1111/ajps.12127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, M. N. and Hobolt, S. B.. 2011. “The Legacy of Lethargy: How Elections to the European Parliament Depress Turnout.” Electoral Studies 30(1): 6776.10.1016/j.electstud.2010.09.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fujiwara, T., Meng, K. and Vogl, T.. 2016. “Habit Formation in Voting: Evidence from Rainy Elections.” American Economic Journal: Applied Economics 8(4): 160–88.Google Scholar
Garcia Bedolla, L. and Michelson, M. R.. 2012. Mobilizing Inclusion. Yale University Press.10.12987/yale/9780300166781.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerber, A. S., Green, D. P. and Shachar, R.. 2003. “Voting May be Habit-Forming: Evidence from a Randomized Field Experiment.” American Journal of Political Science, 47(3): 540–50.10.1111/1540-5907.00038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. P. and Gerber, A. S.. 2002. “The Downstream Benefits of Experimentation.” Political Analysis 10(4): 394402.10.1093/pan/10.4.394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. P. and Gerber, A. S.. 2019. Get Out the Vote: How to Increase Voter Turnout. Brookings Institution Press.Google Scholar
Green, D. P., McGrath, M. C. and Aronow, P. M.. 2013. “Field Experiments and the Study of Voter Turnout.” Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties 23(1): 2748.10.1080/17457289.2012.728223CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. P. and Shachar, R.. 2000. “Habit Formation and Political Behaviour: Evidence of Consuetude in Voter Turnout.” British Journal of Political Science, 30: 561–73.10.1017/S0007123400000247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, E., Anduiza, E. and Rico, G.. 2021. “Affective Polarization and the Salience of Elections.” Electoral Studies 69: 102203.10.1016/j.electstud.2020.102203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, S. J. and Kousser, T.. 2016. “Turning Out Unlikely Voters? A Field Experiment in the Top-Two Primary.” Political Behavior 38(2): 413–32.10.1007/s11109-015-9319-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmberg, E. 2015. Expectedly low turnout as Democrats sweep judicial races. Public Source. https://www.publicsource.org/expectedly-low-turnout-as-democrats-sweep-judicial-races/ (November 4, 2015).Google Scholar
Jackson, R. A.. 2000. “Differential Influences on Participation in Midterm versus Presidential Elections.” The Social Science Journal 37(3): 385402.10.1016/S0362-3319(00)00076-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, R. A. 2002. “Gubernatorial and Senatorial Campaign Mobilization of Voters.” Political Research Quarterly 55(4): 825–44.Google Scholar
Kendall, C. and Rekkas, M.. 2012. “Incumbency Advantages in the Canadian Parliament.” Canadian Journal of Economics/Revue canadienne d'économique 45(4): 1560–85.10.1111/j.1540-5982.2012.01739.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerkestra, P. 2015. Philadelphia Voter Turnout Was Bad, But It Could Have Been Worse. Public Source. https://www.phillymag.com/citified/2015/11/04/philadelphia-voter-turnout/ (November 4, 2015).Google Scholar
Koerth, M. 2018. How Money Affects Elections. FiveThrityEight. https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/money-and-elections-a-complicated-love-story/ (September 10, 2018).Google Scholar
Lally, P. and Gardner, B.. 2013. “Promoting Habit Formation.” Health Psychology Review, 7(Suppl. 1): S13758.10.1080/17437199.2011.603640CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landau, S. and Emsley, R.. 2020. “Causal Inference: Efficacy and Mechanism Evaluation.” Principles and Practice of Clinical Trials, 122.Google Scholar
Lee, D. S. 2008. “Randomized Experiments from Non-Random Selection in US House Elections.” Journal of Econometrics, 142(2): 675–97.10.1016/j.jeconom.2007.05.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leighley, J. and Nagler, J.. 2018. Turnout and Representativeness in Off-Year Elections. Available at SSRN 3263016.10.2139/ssrn.3263016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malhotra, N., Michelson, M. R., Rogers, T. and Valenzuela, A. A.. 2011. “Text Messages as Mobilization Tools: The Conditional Effect of Habitual Voting and Election Salience.” American Politics Research 39(4): 664–81.10.1177/1532673X11398438CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meredith, M. 2009. “Persistence in Political Participation.” Quarterly Journal of Political Science 4(3): 187209.10.1561/100.00009015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michelson, M. R. 2003. “Dos Palos Revisited: Testing the Lasting Effects of Voter Mobilization.” In Annual Meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association.Google Scholar
Moors, A. and De Houwer, J.. 2006. “Automaticity: A Theoretical and Conceptual Analysis.” Psychological Bulletin 132(2): 297.10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.297CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
NCSL. 2023. Voter ID Laws. National Conference of State Legislatures. https://www.ncsl.org/elections-and-campaigns/voter-id#statebystate Google Scholar
Riker, W. H. and Ordeshook, P. C.. 1968. “A Theory of the Calculus of Voting.” American Political Science Review 62(1): 2542.10.2307/1953324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, T. and Frey, E.. 2014. Changing Behavior beyond the Here and Now. https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2410465 10.2139/ssrn.2410465CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, T., Green, D. P., Ternovski, J. and Young, C. F.. 2017. “Social Pressure and Voting: A Field Experiment Conducted in a High-Salience Election.” Electoral Studies 46: 87100.10.1016/j.electstud.2017.02.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolfe, M. 2012. Voter Turnout: A Social Theory of Political Participation. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139058513CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinclair, B., Smith, S. S. and Tucker, P. D.. 2018. “It’s Largely a Rigged System”: Voter Confidence and the Winner Effect in 2016.” Political Research Quarterly, 71(4), 854–68.10.1177/1065912918768006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solvak, M. and Vassil, K.. 2018. “Could Internet Voting Halt Declining Electoral Turnout? New Evidence That E-Voting Is Habit Forming.” Policy & Internet 10(1): 421.10.1002/poi3.160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staiger, D. and Stock, J. H.. 1997. “Instrumental Variables Regression with Weak Instruments.” Econometrica 65(3): 557–86.10.2307/2171753CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ternovski, J. 2023. “Replication Data for: Making Sense of Voting “Habits”: Applying the Process Model of Behavior Change to a Series of Large-Scale Get-Out-the-Vote Experiments.” Harvard Dataverse. https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/G07AO1 Google Scholar
Wood, W., Quinn, J. M. and Kashy, D. A.. 2002. “Habits in Everyday Life: Thought, Emotion, and Action.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1281.10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1281CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, W., Tam, L. and Witt, M. G.. 2005. “Changing Circumstances, Disrupting Habits.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(6), 918.10.1037/0022-3514.88.6.918CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. PMBC model as applied to voting with (right) and without turnout persistence (left).

Figure 1

Table 1. Overview of all experiments

Figure 2

Table 2. Summary of predictions and results across multiple studies

Figure 3

Table 3. Individuals who were successfully turned out to vote in 2014 midterm elections continued to vote in 2018 midterm but not in 2016 presidential elections

Figure 4

Table 4. Salience and downstream CACEs

Figure 5

Table 5. Successfully being turned out to vote in the elections where the voters’ (likely) preferred candidate won makes those voters more likely to vote in 2018 midterm election27

Supplementary material: File

Ternovski supplementary material

Ternovski supplementary material

Download Ternovski supplementary material(File)
File 101.8 KB
Supplementary material: Link

Ternovski Dataset

Link