Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T20:36:11.525Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Girl Who Cried Pain: A Bias against Women in the Treatment of Pain

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2021

Extract

To the woman, God said, “I will greatly multiply your pain in child bearing; in pain you shall bring forth children, yet your desire shall be for your husband, and he shall rule over you.”

Genesis 3:16

There is now a well-established body of literature documenting the pervasive inadequate treatment of pain in this country. There have also been allegations, and some data, supporting the notion that women are more likely than men to be undertreated or inappropriately diagnosed and treated for their pain.

One particularly troublesome study indicated that women are more likely to be given sedatives for their pain and men to be given pain medication. Speculation as to why this difference might exist has included the following: Women complain more than men; women are not accurate reporters of their pain; men are more stoic so that when they do complain of pain, “it's real”; and women are better able to tolerate pain or have better coping skills than men.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © American Society of Law, Medicine and Ethics 2001

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

See Payne, R., “Practice Guidelines for Cancer Pain Treatment: Issues Pertinent to the Revision of National Guidelines,” Oncology, 12, no. 11A (1998): 169–75; McCaffery, M., “Pain Control: Barriers to the Use of Available Information,” Cancer, 70, no. 5 (supplement) (1992): 1438–49; Bernabei, R. et al., “Managing Pain in Elderly Patients with Cancer,” JAMA, 279, no. 23 (1998): 1877–82. See also Wall, P., Jones, M., Defeating Pain, The War Against a Silent Epidemic (New York: Plenum, 1991).Google Scholar
See Calderone, K.L., “The Influence of Gender on the Frequency of Pain and Sedative Medication Administered to Postoperative Patients,” Sex Roles, 23 (1990): 1112, 713–25.Google Scholar
See Walker, P.L., Cook, D.C., “Brief Communication: Gender and Sex: Vive la Difference,” American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 106, no. 2 (1998): 255–59, who underscore maintaining the distinction between “sex” (the anatomical or chromosomal categories of male and female) and “gender” (socially constructed roles that are related to sex distinctions). It should be noted that while isolating the influence of sex and gender on pain response and treatment is the focus of this article, we do not mean to dismiss the powerful influence of class, race, culture, education, and other such variables that likely affect pain response and treatment.Google Scholar
See Blumenthal, S.J. and Wood, S.F., “Women's Health Care: Federal Initiatives, Policies, and Directions,” in Gallant, S. and Keita, G.P., eds., Health Care for Women: Psychological, Social & Behavioral Influences (Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association, 1997): 5771.Google Scholar
See National Institutes of Health Revitalization Act of 1993, Pub. L. No. 103–43, 107 Stat. 22 (June 10, 1993).Google Scholar
Unruh, A.M., “Gender Variations in Clinical Pain Experience,” Pain, 65 (1996): 123–67.Google Scholar
See Berkley, K.J., “Sex Differences in Pain,” Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 20, no. 3 (1997): 371–80.Google Scholar
See Fillingim, R.B. and Maixner, W., “Gender Differences in the Responses to Noxious Stimuli,” Pain Forum, 4, no. 4 (1995), 209–21.Google Scholar
Id. at 209.Google Scholar
Unruh, A.M., “Why Can't a Woman Be More Like a Man?,” Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 20, no. 3 (1997): At 467.Google Scholar
See Carr, T.D., Lemanek, K.L., and Armstrong, F.D., “Pain and Fear Ratings: Clinical Implications of Age and Gender Differences,” Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 15, no. 5 (1998): 305–13; Krogstad, B.S., Jokstad, A., and Vassend, O., “The Reporting of Pain, Somatic Complaints, and Anxiety in a Group of Patients with TMD Before and 2 Years After Treatment. Sex Differences,” Journal of Orofacial Pain, 10, no. 3 (1996): 263–69; Plesh, O. et al., “Gender Difference in Jaw Pain by Clenching,” Journal of Oral Rehabilitation, 25, no. 4 (1998): 258–63 (corroborating that women have a higher incidence than men of temporomandibular pain). See also France, C.R. and Suchowiecki, S., “A Comparison of Diffuse Noxious Inhibitory Controls in Men and Women,” Pain, 81, no. 1-2 (1999): 77–84 (finding that women exhibited significantly lower pain thresholds than men and reported significantly greater pain in response to both ischemia and electrocutaneous noxious stimulation).Google Scholar
See Berkley, , supra note 7.Google Scholar
See Giamberardino, M.A. et al., “Pain Threshold Variations in Somatic Wall Tissues as a Function of Menstrual Cycle, Segmental Site and Tissue Depth in Non-Dysmenorrheic Women, Dysmenorrheic Women and Men,” Pain, 71 (1997): 187–97.Google Scholar
See Fillingim, R.B. et al., “Ischemic But Not Thermal Pain Sensitivity Varies Across the Menstrual Cycle,” Psychosomatic Medicine, 59 (1997): 512–20.Google Scholar
See Glaros, A.G., Baharloo, L., Glass, E.G., “Effect of Parafunctional Clenching and Estrogen on Temporomandibular Disorder Pain,” Journal of Craniomandibular Practice, 16, no. 2 (1998): 7883.Google Scholar
See Dao, T.T., Knight, K., and Ton-That, V., “Modulation of Myofascial Pain by the Reproductive Hormones: A Preliminary Report,” The Journal of Prosthetic Dentistry, 79 (1998): 663–70.Google Scholar
Id. at 667.Google Scholar
See McEwen, B.S., Alves, S.E., Bulloch, K., Weiland, N.G., “Clinically Relevant Basic Science Studies of Gender Differences and Sex Hormone Effects,” Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 34, no. 3 (1998), 251–59, in which the authors present a review of studies depicting the array of neurochemical and structural effects of ovarian hormones, including their influence on cognitive function and pain sensitivity. Female rats showed less opioid-mediated stress-induced analgesia than male rats when exposed to a variety of stressors, and male rats demonstrated greater swim stress analgesia and less predator-evoked analgesia than females.Google Scholar
See Mogil, J.S. et al., “Identification of a Sex-Specific Quantitative Trait Locus Mediating Nonopioid Stress-Induced Analgesia in Female Mice,” The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, no. 20 (1997): 79958002.Google Scholar
Fillingim, and Maixner, , supra note 8, at 214.Google Scholar
See Aloisi, A.M., Zimmermann, M., Herdegen, T., “Sex-Dependent Effects of Formalin and Restraint on c-Fos Expression in the Septum and Hippocampus of the Rat,” Neuroscience, 81, no. 4 (1997): 951958. See also Aloisi, A.M., “Sex Differences in Pain-Induced Effects on the Septo-Hippocampal System,” Brain Research Reviews, 25, no. 3 (1997): 397–406.Google Scholar
See Mayer, E.A. et al., “Review Article: Gender-Related Differences in Functional Gastrointestinal Disorders,” Alimentary Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 13, no. 2 (supplement) (1999): 6569.Google Scholar
See Paulson, P.E. et al., “Gender Differences in Pain Perception and Patterns of Cerebral Activation During Noxious Heat Stimulation in Humans,” Pain, 76 (1998), 223–29.Google Scholar
Berkley, , supra note 7.Google Scholar
Berkley, , supra note 7.Google Scholar
Unruh, , supra note 6.Google Scholar
See De Vries, G.J. and Boyle, P.A., “Double Duty for Sex Differences in the Brain,” Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 92, no. 2 (1998): 205–13.Google Scholar
One difficulty in interpreting evidence from research studies is the individual variability of the pain response. Greater variability makes research on pain responses more difficult, as it decreases power and thus increases the likelihood of having insignificant results due to an insufficient number of subjects studied. This has been recently corroborated in a meta-analysis by Riley and colleagues, who determined that only seven of thirty-four studies reviewed on gender differences in pain response had adequate sample sizes. This implies that gender differences have been underestimated rather than overestimated in pain research. See Riley, J.L. III et al., “Sex Differences in the Perception of Noxious Experimental Stimuli: A Meta-Analysis,” Pain, 74 (1998): 181–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bendelow, G., Pain and Gender (New York: Prentice Hall, 2000): At 17, citing Neisser, V., Cognition and Reality (San Francisco: Freeman, 1976): At 214.Google Scholar
Duncan describes how Cartesian (i.e., Descartes') mind-body dualism is inaccurately equated with medical reductionism, the latter of which tends to dismiss mind (psychology) and favor body (physiology) in the diagnostic encounter. See Duncan, G., “Mind-Body Dualism and the Biopsychosocial Model of Pain: What Did Descartes Really Say?,” Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, 25, no. 4 (2000): 485-513. A more holistic approach is supported somewhat by Melzack and Wall's gate-control theory of pain, in which a neural mechanism in the spinal cord is thought to function like a gate to control the flow of nerve impulses into the central nervous system. Whether sensory transmission is increased or decreased (causing, respectively, a greater or lesser pain intensity perception) is influenced by cognitive and emotional input such as anxiety, mood state, attention, and past experiences. Bendelow and Williams state that the gate-control theory “signals the end of the mind/body split with regard to pain.” However, these authors acknowledge that currently “the biological remains dominant over the social.” Indeed, Duncan points out that the contemporary biopsychosocial model of pain does not entirely escape mind-body dualism. See also Melzack, R. and Wall, P., The Challenge of Pain (Harmondsworth, England: Penguin, 1988); Bendelow, G.A. and Williams, S.J., “Transcending the Dualisms: Towards a Sociology of Pain,” Sociology of Health and Illness, 17, no. 2 (1995): 139–65, at 143.Google Scholar
Unruh, , supra note 6, at 157.Google Scholar
See Lazarus, R.S. and Folkman, S., Stress, Appraisal, and Coping (New York: Springer, 1984), as cited in O'Leary, A. and Helgeson, V.S., “Psychosocial Factors and Women's Health: Integrating Mind, Heart, and Body,” in Gallant, S.J., Keita, G.P., and Royak-Shaler, R., eds., Health Care for Women: Psychological, Social and Behavioral Influences (Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association, 1997): 25–71.Google Scholar
Unruh, , supra note 6.Google Scholar
Id. at 158.Google Scholar
Weir, R. et al., “Gender Differences in Psychosocial Adjustment to Chronic Pain and Expenditures for Health Care Services Used,” The Clinical Journal of Pain, 12 (1996): 277–90, at 287.Google Scholar
Unruh, , supra note 6, at 149, citing Astor-Dubin, L. and Hammen, C., “Cognitive Versus Behavioral Coping Response of Men and Women: A Brief Report,” Cognitive Research Therapy, 8 (1984): 8590; Brehms, C. and Johnson, M.E., “Problem-Solving Appraisal and Coping Style: The Influence of Sex-Role Orientation and Gender,” The Journal of Psychology, 123 (1989): 187–94; Holm, J.E. et al., “The Role of Stress in Recurrent Tension Headache,” Headache, 26 (1986): 160–67; Stone, A.A. and Neale, J.M., “New Measure of Daily Coping: Development and Preliminary Results,” Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 46 (1984): 892–906; Verbrugge, L.M., “Sex Differences in Complaints and Diagnoses,” Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 3 (1980): 327–55; Vingerhoets, A.J.J.M. and Van Heck, G.L., “Gender, Coping and Psychosomatic Symptoms,” Psychological Medicine, 20 (1990): 125–35.Google Scholar
Thoits, P.A., “Gender Differences in Coping with Emotional Distress,” in Eckenrode, J., ed., The Social Context of Coping (New York: Plenum, 1991): 107–38, as cited in O'Leary, , Helgeson, , supra note 35, at 30.Google Scholar
See Unruh, A.M., Ritchie, J., Merskey, H., “Does Gender Affect Appraisal of Pain and Pain Coping Strategies?,” The Clinical Journal of Pain, 15, no. 1 (1999), 3140.Google Scholar
See Jensen, I., et al., “Coping with Long-Term Musculoskeletal Pain and Its Consequence: Is Gender a Factor?,” Pain, 57 (1994): 167–72.Google Scholar
Unruh, , supra note 6, at 149. See also Buckelew, S.P. et al., “Health Locus of Control, Gender Differences and Adjustment to Persistent Pain,” Pain, 42 (1990): 287–94; Keefe, F.J. et al., “Analyzing Pain in Rheumatoid Arthritis Patients,” Pain, 46 (1991): 153–60; and Strong, J., Ashton, R. and Stewart, A., “Chronic Low Back Pain: Towards an Integrated Psychosocial Assessment Model,” Clinical Psychology, 69 (1994), 1058–63.Google Scholar
O'Leary, and Helgeson, , supra note 35; Unruh, , supra note 6; Weir, et al., supra note 38. There is some evidence, however, that health-care utilization rates do not differ among elderly men and women and that older females tend to report lower levels of health-care utilization than older males. See Hunt, K. et al., “Are Women More Ready to Consult Than Men? Gender Differences in Family Practitioner Consultation for Common Chronic Conditions,” Journal of Health Services and Research Policy, 4, no. 2 (1999), 96100. See also Riley, J.L., Gilbert, G.H., Heft, M.W., “Orofacial Pain Symptom Prevalence: Selective Sex Differences in the Elderly?,” Pain, 76, no. 1–2 (1998), 97-104.Google Scholar
Miaskowski reports that women report more visits to health-care providers on retrospective assessment, but on prospective studies, men and women seek health care at the same rate. Researchers have attributed this difference to the possibility that men are more reluctant than women to admit visiting health-care providers or less likely to remember such visits. See Miaskowski, C., “The Role of Sex and Gender in Pain Perception and Responses to Treatment,” in Gatchel, R.J. and Turk, D.C., eds., Psychosocial Factors in Pain: Critical Perspectives (New York: The Guildford Press, 1999): 401–11, at 406. In one prospective study, E. Berkanovic, C. Telesky, and S. Reeder found that perceived efficacy of health care, perceived seriousness of one's symptom, and reported disability from that symptom were more highly correlated with health-care utilization than was gender, although older women were found to have accessed a physician more than men. “Structural and Social Psychological Factors in the Decision to Seek Medical Care for Symptoms,” Medical Care, XIX, no. 7 (1981): 693–709.Google Scholar
Bendelow, , supra note 32. See also Bendelow, G., “Pain Perceptions, Emotions and Gender,” Sociology of Health and Illness, 15 (1993): 273–94; Pollack, W., Real Boys: Rescuing Our Sons from the Myths of Boyhood (New York: Henry Holt & Co., 1998).Google Scholar
Pollack, , supra note 46, at 11.Google Scholar
Miaskowski, , supra note 45, at 404.Google Scholar
See Levine, F.M. and De Simone, L.L., “The Effects of Experimenter Gender on Pain Report in Male and Female Subjects,” Pain, 44 (1991): 6972.Google Scholar
See Davidson, D.J. and Freudenburg, W.R., “Gender and Environmental Risk Concerns: A Review and Analysis of Available Research,” Environment and Behavior, 28, no. 3 (1996): 302–39.Google Scholar
Women have been found to be more other-directed in conversation and to frequently seek support and validation in their communication encounters. Men have been found to be more goal-directed and status-conscious in communication and to give advice more often. See Tannen, D., You Just Don't Understand: Women and Men in Conversation (New York: Ballantine, 1990).Google Scholar
Miaskowski, C., “Women and Pain,” Critical Care Nursing Clinics of North America, 9, no. 4 (1997): At 467.Google Scholar
Unruh, et al., supra note 41. See also Bendelow, , supra note 32.Google Scholar
Miaskowski, , supra note 45.Google Scholar
See Faherty, B.S. and Grier, M.R., “Analgesic Medication for Elderly People Post-Surgery,” Nursing Research, 33, no. 6 (1984): 369–72.Google Scholar
Calderone, , supra note 2. Frequency of pain medication dosing was studied rather than the amount of analgesic being dosed since the latter is related to body weight and men might be administered higher doses based on body weight, not gender bias.Google Scholar
See Beyer, J.E. et al., “Patterns of Postoperative Analgesic Use with Adults and Children Following Cardiac Surgery,” Pain, 17, no. 1 (1983), 7181, as cited in Miaskowsi, , supra note 45. Miaskowski cautions readers that most studies that have looked at gender differences in post-operative pain medication administration did not evaluate whether men and women received adequate amounts of analgesic medication. See Miaskowsi, , supra notes 45 and 52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
See Cleeland, C.S. et al., “Pain and Its Treatment in Outpatients with Metastatic Cancer,” N. Engl. J. Med., 330, no. 9 (1994): 592–96.Google Scholar
Breitbart, W. et al., “The Undertreatment of Pain in Ambulatory AIDS Patients,” Pain, 65, no. 2 (1996), 243–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
See McDonald, D.D., “Gender and Ethnic Stereotyping and Narcotic Analgesic Administration,” Research in Nursing and Health, 17 (1994): 4549. McDonald, however, did not control for patient weight and thus could not rule out that men were given more narcotic analgesics based on greater body weight.Google Scholar
However, Raftery and colleagues found that female patients admitted to the emergency room reported more pain than males, were perceived by health-care providers to experience more pain, received more pain medications and more potent analgesics, and were less likely to receive no pain medications. Patient perception of pain and pain severity was the strongest predictor of number and strength of medications given for pain. The sex of the patient or health-care provider did not predict the number, type, and strength of medications received. Thus, health-care provider gender biases may fluctuate by geographical region or medical specialty. See Raftery, K.A., Smith-Coggins, R., and Chen, A.H., “Gender-Associated Differences in Emergency Department Pain Management,” Annals of Emergency Medicine, 26, no. 4 (1995): 414–21.Google Scholar
See Elderkin-Thompson, V. and Waitzkin, H., “Differences in Clinical Communication by Gender,” Journal of General Internal Medicine, 14 (1999): 112–21.Google Scholar
See Johnson, P.A., Golman, L., and Orav, E.J., “Gender Differences in the Management of Acute Chest Pain: Support for the ‘Yentl Syndrome,’” Journal of General Internal Medicine, 11 (1996): 209–17.Google Scholar
Weir, et al., supra note 38. “Specialist” was not defined, and the possibility exists that some women were referred by a gynecologist who was considered a “specialist,” but served as the woman's primary care provider.Google Scholar
See Lack, D.Z., “Women and Pain: Another Feminist Issue,” Women and Therapy, 1 (1982): 5564, as cited in Unruh, , supra note 6, at 159.Google Scholar
Elderkin-Thompson, and Waitzkin, , supra note 62. In addition, Crook and Tunks found that women with persistent pain were significantly more likely than men with persistent pain to receive psychotropic drugs from their family practitioner. See Crook, J. and Tunks, E., “Women with Pain,” in Tunks, E., Bellissimo, A., Roy, R., eds., Chronic Pain: Psychosocial Factors in Rehabilitation (Malabar, Florida: R.E. Krieger Publishing Company, 1990): 3748.Google Scholar
See McCaffery, M. and Ferrell, B.R., “Does the Gender Gap Affect Your Pain Control Decisions?,” Nursing, 22, no. 8 (1992): 4851.Google Scholar
See McDonald, D. and Bridge, R.G., “Gender Stereotyping and Nursing Care,” Research in Nursing and Health, 14 (1991): 373–78.Google Scholar
See Hadjistavropoulos, T., McMurty, B., and Craig, K.D., “Beautiful Faces in Pain: Biases and Accuracy in the Perception of Pain,” Psychology and Health, 11 (1996): 411–20.Google Scholar
Id. at 417. The hypothesis that stereotyping based on attractiveness may actually influence individuals' pain responses (i.e., attractive individuals learn to actively cope better with pain and thus experience less pain intensity, and unattractive individuals learn more passive pain coping mechanisms and experience greater pain intensity) was considered.Google Scholar
See Hadjistavropoulos, T., Hadjistavropoulos, H.D. and Craig, K.D., “Appearance-Based Information about Coping with Pain: Valid or Biased?,” Social Science and Medicine, 40, no. 4 (1995): 537–43; Hadjistavropoulos, H.D., Ross, M., and von Baeyer, C.L., “Are Physicians' Ratings of Pain Affected by Patients' Physical Attractiveness?,” Social Science and Medicine, 31 (1990): 69–72.Google Scholar
See Ebert, S.M., Liu, X., and Woosley, R.L., “Female Gender as a Risk Factor for Drug-Induced Cardiac Arrhythmias: Evaluation of Clinical and Experimental Evidence,” Journal of Women's Health, 7, no. 5 (1998): 547–57. See also Thompson, P.M. and Wolf, J.L., “The Sexual Revolution in Science: What Gender-Based Research Is Telling Us,” Journal of Investigative Medicine, 47, no. 3 (1999): 106–13.Google Scholar
See Gear, R.W. et al., “Gender Difference in Analgesic Response to the Kappa-Opioid Pentazocine,” Neuroscience Letters, 205, no. 3 (1996): 207–09; Gear, R.W. et al., “Kappa-Opioids Produce Significantly Greater Analgesia in Women Than in Men,” Nature Medicine, 2, no. 11 (1996): 1248–50. See also Jamner, L.D. et al., “Pain Inhibition, Nicotine, and Gender,” Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 6, no. 1 (1998): 96-106 (finding that nicotine had a direct pain-inhibitory effect in men but not in women); Robinson, M.E. et al., “Sex Differences in Response to Cutaneous Anesthesia: A Double Blind Randomized Study,” Pain, 77 (1998): 143–49 (finding that men responded differently from women to the pain inhibitory effects of a local anesthetic, e.g., locally anesthetized men rated a pressure stimulus as less painful than anesthetized women).Google Scholar
See Mogil, J., “Does He Feel Her Pain? Maybe Not,” University of Illinois Alumnae LAS Newsletter, Spring (1999): At 7.Google Scholar
Weir, et al., supra note 38, at 278, stating that “several current hypotheses from the biological perspective are that pain perception is probably related to sensory factors rather than differences in attitudes or emotional responses, and that endogenous pain inhibition may be affected by hormonal variation.”Google Scholar
Although episodes of acute, severe pain will often be associated with an increase in heart rate or blood pressure, and increased heart rate is used in some studies (e.g., with infants) as a proxy measure of pain, others discourage clinicians from relying on these physiological measures as evidence of a patient's pain. See McCaffery, M. and Ferrell, B.R., “How Vital Are Vital Signs?,” Nursing, 22, no. 1 (1992): At 45 (“No firm clinical evidence exists to support the assumption that moderate to severe pain is always accompanied by a change in vital signs.”).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unruh, , supra note 39.Google Scholar
O'Leary, , Helgeson, , supra note 35; Unruh, , supra note 6; Weir, et al., supra note 38.Google Scholar
Unruh, , supra note 6, at 159. See also Katz, P.P. and Criswell, L.A., “Differences in Symptom Reports Between Men and Women with Rheumatoid Arthritis,” Arthritis Care & Research, 9, no. 6 (1996): 441–48.Google Scholar
Id. at 441.Google Scholar
Miaskowski, , supra note 45, at 406.Google Scholar
McCaffery, and Ferrell, , supra note 67.Google Scholar
Bendelow, , supra note 32, at 130. See also Nurofen, , Pain Relief Study (London: Kings Fund, 1989), as cited in Bendelow, , supra note 32, at 36.Google Scholar
See Johansson, E.E. et al., “The Meanings of Pain: An Exploration of Women's Descriptions of Symptoms,” Social Science and Medicine, 48, no. 12 (1999), 17911802.Google Scholar
Fillingim, and Maixner, , supra note 8.Google Scholar
Bendelow, , “Pain Perceptions, Emotions, and Gender,” supra note 46, at 286.Google Scholar
Bendelow, , supra note 32, at 53–55.Google Scholar
Id. at 115–16.Google Scholar
Crook, and Tunks, , supra note 66, at 38.Google Scholar
Unruh, , supra note 39.Google Scholar
See Verbrugge, L.M., “Females and Illness: Recent Trends in Sex Differences in the United States,” Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 17 (1976): 387403, as cited in Weir et al., supra note 38, at 288. Bendelow also describes how women's more predominant involvement in the domestic sphere has associated them more with the “natural” world in the form of bodily functions, whereas men have been more involved in the “public world of work and therefore ‘higher’ cultural and mental processes.” The perception thus may exist that men's “higher” functioning, if interrupted by the presence of pain, should receive a higher priority for medical intervention. See Bendelow, , supra note 32, at 131.Google Scholar
Marshall, N.L. states, “combining work and family has become the norm for women rather than the exception.” Marshall, N.L., “Combining Work and Family,” in Gallant, S.J., Keita, G.P., and Royak-Shaler, R., eds., Health Care for Women: Psychological, Social and Behavioral Influences (Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association, 1997): 163–74, at 163.Google Scholar
McCaffery, and Ferrell, , supra note 76.Google Scholar
Johansson, et al., supra note 84, at 1792.Google Scholar
See Baszanger, I., “Deciphering Chronic Pain,” Sociology of Health and Illness, 14 (1992): 181215, as cited in Johansson, et al., supra note 84, at 1792.Google Scholar
Studies have found that the most significant predictor of inadequate pain relief is a discrepancy between the patient and physician regarding the severity of the patient's pain. See Cleeland, et al., supra note 58.Google Scholar
Dresser, R., “What Bioethics Can Learn from the Women's Health Movement,” in Wolf, S.M., ed., Feminism & Bioethics (New York: Oxford University Press, 1996): 144–59, at 147, citing Sherwin, S., No Longer Patient: Feminist Ethics and Health Care (Philadelphia: Temple University Press, 1992): At 193.Google Scholar
Id. at 147.Google Scholar
Parks, J.A., “Why Gender Matters to the Euthanasia Debate: On Decisional Capacity and the Rejection of Women's Death Requests,” The Hastings Center Report, 30, no. 1 (2000): 3036, at 33.Google Scholar
Miles, S.H. and August, A., “Courts, Gender and the ‘Right to Die,’” Journal of Law, Medicine & Health Care, 18 (1990): 8595.Google Scholar
Parks, , supra note 99. See also Rothenberg, K.H., “Gender Matters: Implications for Clinical Research and Women's Health Care,” Houston Law Review, 32, no. 5 (1996), 1201–72.Google Scholar
Grace, V.M., “Problems of Communication, Diagnosis, and Treatment Experienced by Women Using the New Zealand Health Services for Chronic Pelvic Pain: A Quantitative Analysis,” Health Care for Women International, 16 (1995): 521–35.Google Scholar
Id. at 525.Google Scholar
See Sherwin, , supra note 97, at 48. See also Smith, J.F., “Communicative Ethics in Medicine: The Physician-Patient Relationship,” in Wolf, S.M., ed., Feminism & Bioethics (New York: Oxford University Press, 1996): 184215, at 194 (“Women who do speak assertively are often taken to be domineering rather than dominant, emotional rather than rational, biased rather than authoritative, and complaining rather than assertive.”). Kate Nicholson, a disability lawyer who is writing a book about her experiences living with chronic neuropathic pain, and who commented on an earlier version of this manuscript, expressed concern that acknowledging gender differences in pain response might lead to gender stereotyping. She made a point of trying to report her pain to health-care providers factually and unemotionally, and recalled one female pain specialist's comment to her, “You are not crazy; you're not like my other patients.” Additionally, she recalled a male acquaintance's adamant demand from emergency room staff to give him something for his acute pain (“You SOB's, you are giving me something for pain and you're giving it to me right now!”). He got the pain medication. Yet Nicholson was aware that if she had done something similar, “I'd have been perceived extremely differently, in all likelihood.”Google Scholar
See Unruh, , supra note 6, at 158, summarizing the literature on this point.Google Scholar
See Crook, and Tunks, , supra note 66, at 37, citing Engel, G., “‘Psychogenic’ Pain and the Pain-Prone Patient,” American Journal of Medicine, 26 (1959): 899918.Google Scholar
See Lennane, K.J. and Lennane, R.J., “Alleged Psychogenic Disorders in Women — A Possible Manifestation of Sexual Prejudice,” N. Engl. J. Med., 288 (1973): 288–92, as cited in Unruh, , supra note 6, at 158.Google Scholar
Women are four times as likely as men to be diagnosed with a major depression, and twice as likely as men to be diagnosed with a general depression, although rates vary with ethnicity and culture. Possible reasons for this may include that women are more willing to seek help and thus may be diagnosed more frequently; there may be biological differences that predispose women to developing depression (e.g., effects of reproductive hormones); or women may be more likely to suffer from depression due to external stressors (e.g., the effects of sexism, domestic violence, lower pay, and relationship stressors). See Culbertson, F.M., “Depression and Gender,” American Psychologist, 52, no. 1 (1997), 2531. See also O'Leary, and Helgeson, , supra note 35. Also, female chronic pain patients are more likely than male chronic pain patients to be diagnosed with and treated for depression. See Haley, W.E., Turner, J.A., and Romano, J.M., “Depression in Chronic Pain Patients: Relation to Pain, Activity, and Sex Differences,” Pain, 23 (1985): 337–43.Google Scholar
See Vallerand, A.H., “Gender Differences in Pain,” Image: Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 27, no. 3 (1995): At 237.Google Scholar
Smith, , supra note 106, at 190.Google Scholar
See Roter, D.L. and Hall, J.A., “Gender Differences in Patient-Physician Communication,” in Gallant, S.J., Keita, G.P., and Royak-Shaler, R., eds., Health Care for Women: Psychological, Social & Behavioral influences (Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association, (1997): 5771.Google Scholar
Smith, , supra note 106.Google Scholar
Female physicians, however, have been found to spend more time with patients, engage in more positive talk, ask more questions, and elicit more responses from patients. See Roter, and Hall, , supra note 113. See also Bensing, J., Van den Brink-Muinen, A., and de Bakker, D., “Differences Between Male and Female General Practitioners in the Care of Psychosocial Problems,” Medical Care, 31 (1993): 219–29.Google Scholar
Hadjistavropoulos, , McMurtry, , and Craig, , supra note 69; Hadjistavropoulos, , Hadjistavropoulos, , and Craig, , “Appearance-Based Information about Coping with Pain: Valid or Biased?,” supra note 71; Hadjistavropoulos, , Ross, , and von Baeyer, , “Are Physicians' Ratings of Pain Affected by Patients' Physical Attractiveness?,” supra note 71.Google Scholar
See Selfe, S.A., Matthews, Z., Stones, R.W., “Factors Influencing Outcome in Consultations for Chronic Pelvic Pain,” Journal of Women's Health, 7, no. 8 (1998): 1041–48.Google Scholar
In a Connecticut focus-group study with chronic pain patients, participants reported visiting multiple physicians (quoting “60 to 100”) in order to find a diagnosis and a practitioner with whom they felt comfortable. See Grantham, S. and Robbins, M., The Connecticut Pain Management Initiative: Focus Group Report (Boston: John Snow, Inc., February 11, 2000).Google Scholar
Marshall, , supra note 92.Google Scholar
About 55 to 65 percent of those who consult complementary practitioners are women, the highest users being those aged 35 to 60. See Zollman, C. and Vickers, A., “ABCs of Complementary Medicine: Users and Practitioners of Complementary Medicine,” British Medical Journal, 319, no. 7213 (1999): 836–38; and Ernst, E., “Prevalence of Use of Complementary/Alternative Medicine: A Systematic Review,” Bulletin of the World Health Organization, 78, no. 2 (2000): 252–57.Google Scholar
Controversy over use of the term “alternative” rather than “complementary” medicine demonstrates the point being made here. The former describes therapies that are not sanctioned by conventional medicine and which patients choose instead of conventional medical therapy. The latter views such therapies as complementing conventional medical therapies. The goal would be for women (as well as men) to have access to both traditional and non-traditional therapies for pain management, with a focus on a holistic approach that provides optimal pain relief. This holistic approach is the accepted standard for many pain clinics and inpatient pain teams, but adequate access to such care is limited for many individuals — either because the pain teams and clinics are not available in their area or they do not get the referral they need.Google Scholar
Davidson and Freudenberg conclude that women in general, as a result of their socialization, are not as likely to develop a distinction between themselves as individuals and the world around them, whereas men are socialized to objectify and control their environment and to define themselves as separate from the world around them. Men would thus be more apt to try to separate biological and psychosocial pain etiologies, whereas women would tend to view them more holistically. See Davidson, D.J. and Freudenburg, W.R., “Gender and Environmental Risk Concerns: A Review and Analysis of Available Research,” Environment and Behavior, 28, no. 3 (1996): 302–39. This theory is affirmed by Bendelow's findings that women spoke of pain experiences more holistically as compared to men and that “men were significantly less inclined to think that the emotional component of pain perception had any importance.” See Bendelow, , supra note 32, at 90.Google Scholar
See Kwolek, D.S. et al., “Gender Differences in Clinical Evaluation: Narrowing the Gap with Women's Health Clinical Skills Workshop,” Academic Medicine, 73, no. 10 (supplement) (1998): S8890.Google Scholar
That is, women may: (1) have their pain complaints erroneously dismissed as being emotionally-based and therefore “not real” when there is no significant psychological component to the pain; (2) have the likely psychological components that accompany chronic pain be misidentified by health-care providers as the cause, rather than the result of their unrelieved pain, leading to a discounting of the pain; or (3) have the psychological problem that is the source of their pain be discounted and not adequately addressed. All three are inappropriate and reveal a disdain for psychosocial contributors to pain over evidence of organic causation. See Duncan, , supra note 33.Google Scholar
Bendelow found that men who were given an opportunity to discuss the emotional aspects of their pain experiences did so and were grateful for the opportunity, even though they did not initially acknowledge emotions as contributing to their pain. See Bendelow, , supra note 32, at 90–94.Google Scholar
Johansson, et al., supra note 84, at 1800.Google Scholar
Some institutions have already begun addressing the impact of JCAHO pain management standards. See Pasero, C., McCaffery, M., and Gordon, D.B., “Build Institutional Commitment to Improving Pain Management,” Nursing Management, 30, no. 1 (1999): 2733.Google Scholar