Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T01:43:10.062Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Experimental Studies of the Mental Speed of Schizophrenics

I. Effects of a Stimulant and a Depressant Drug

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2018

Anne Broadhurst*
Affiliation:
Springfield Hospital, Upper Tooting

Extract

It has been clinically observed that psychiatric patients in general (6, 11) and schizophrenic patients in particular (1, 4) show abnormalities of mental speed, being “retarded” or slower than normals on many measures. Confirmatory evidence on this point is to be found but much of the early work on speed of schizophrenic reactivity used measures of speed of motor performance (12, 13) or of reaction time under various conditions (6), ignoring more fundamental slowness of thought processes. The present studies are concentrated on the recent finding that schizophrenics show abnormally slow mental speed measured in a problem-solving situation (4, 18, 19). The aim of the investigation was to discover the exact conditions under which this abnormality appears, and, thence, by manipulating the experimental conditions, to be able to bring speed of mental functioning under experimental control. This paper describes the attempt to bring speed under control by means of drugs. A second paper (2) deals with the effect of practice upon mental speed.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1958 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Babcock, Harriet, Dementia Precox. A Psychological Study, 1933. New York: Sci. Press Co.Google Scholar
Broadhurst, Anne, “Experimental studies of the mental speed of schizophrenics. II. Effects of practice”, J. Ment. Sci., 1958, 104, 1130.Google Scholar
Brody, M. B., “Mental testing”, ibid., 1944, 90, 127157.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J., Differential cognitive tests, 1953. Office of Naval Research report. Contract No. N625585. Bur. Med. Surg., U.S. Navy, London Branch. Google Scholar
Franks, C. M., and Laverty, S. G., “Sodium amytal and eye-lid conditioning”, J. Ment. Sci., 1955, 101, 654663.Google Scholar
Franz, S. I., “The times of some mental processes in the retardation and excitement of insanity”, Amer. J. Psychol., 1906, 17, 3868.Google Scholar
Furneaux, W. D., “Some speed, error and difficulty relationships within a problem-solving situation”, Nature, 1952, 170, 3738.Google Scholar
Idem , “A note on the Nufferno tests of inductive reasoning abilities, including summaries of such norms as have so far been established”, Parts 1–5. Nuffield Research Unit (Selection of Students) Internal Report No. F.I. April, 1953, revised August, 1954. Unpublished.Google Scholar
Hetherington, R., “Psychological changes due to E.C.T. as measured by a battery of tests given repeatedly”, J. Ment. Sci., 1952, 98, 334341.Google Scholar
Hilgard, E. R., and Marquis, D. G., Conditioning and Learning, 1940. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Huston, P. E., “The reflex time of the patellar tendon reflex in normal and schizophrenic subjects”, J. gen. Psychol., 1935, 13, 341.Google Scholar
Idem and Shakow, D., “Learning in schizophrenia. I. Pursuit learning”, J. Pers., 1948, 17, 5274.Google Scholar
Iidem , “Learning capacity in schizophrenia with special reference to the concept of deterioration” Amer. J. Psychiat., 1949, 105, 881888.Google Scholar
Landis, C., and Clausen, J., “Changes in sensory and motor performances induced by active psychiatric treatment”, J. Psychol., 1955, 40, 275305.Google Scholar
Layman, J. W., “A quantitative study of certain changes in schizophrenic patients under the influence of sodium amytal”, J. gen. Psychol., 1940, 22, 6786.Google Scholar
Lorr, M., Jenkins, R. L., and O'Connor, J. P., “Factors descriptive of psychopathology and behavior of hospitalized psychotics”, J. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1955, 50, 7886.Google Scholar
Mainord, W. A., “Some effects of sodium amytal on ‘deteriorated’ schizophrenics”, J. consult. Psychol., 1953, 17, 5457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, Elizabeth H., An experimental investigation of intellectual speed and power in mental disorders. 1953. Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis, University of London Google Scholar
Ogilvie, B.C., A study of intellectual slowness in schizophrenia, 1954. Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis, University of London Google Scholar
Osgood, C. E., Method and Theory in Experimental Psychology, 1953. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sandler, J., “A test of the significance of the difference between the means of correlated measures based on a simplification of Student's t“, Brit. J. Psychol., 1955, 46, 225226.Google Scholar
Senf, Rita, Huston, P. E., and Cohen, B. P., “Thinking deficit in schizophrenia and changes with amytal”, J. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1955, 50, 383387.Google Scholar
Shagass, C., and Naiman, J., “The sedation threshold, manifest anxiety, and some aspects of ego function”, Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 1955, 74, 397406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iidem , “The sedation threshold as an objective index of manifest anxiety in psychoneurosis”, J. psychosom. Res., 1956, 1, 4957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shakow, D., “The nature of schizophrenic deterioration”, Nerv. Ment. Dis. Monogr., 1946, No. 70.Google Scholar
Shapiro, M. B., and Nelson, Elizabeth H., “An investigation of the nature of cognitive impairment in co-operative psychiatric patients”, Brit. J. med. Psychol., 1955, 28, 239256.Google Scholar
Steinberg, Hannah and Summerfield, A., “Influence of a depressant drug on acquisition in rote learning”, Quart. J. exper. Psychol., 1957, 9, 138145.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.