Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T02:40:02.809Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Navigation in Birds and Other Animals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 October 2009

Wolfgang Wiltschko
Affiliation:
(Johann Wolfgang Goethe-University, Frankfurt)
Roswitha Wiltschko
Affiliation:
(Johann Wolfgang Goethe-University, Frankfurt)

Extract

Many animals have some kind of ‘home’ –a nest, a burrow, etc. This applies to insects that build nests and provide their larvae with food just as much as to numerous species of vertebrates. Rodents and moles are known to dig burrows; most other mammals, reptiles, amphibians and even many fresh-water fish do not roam around randomly either, but have well-defined home ranges with favourite resting places in particular cavities, under rocks or in dense thickets. Birds build nests for their young and fly out searching for food. All these animals permanently face orientation problems: they have to move swiftly and efficiently within their home range, and they must be capable of returning home when they have left their familiar area while exploring, fleeing, etc.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal Institute of Navigation 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Able, K. P. (1991). Common themes and variations in animal orientation systems. American Zoologist, 31, 157167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, R. R. (1989). Navigation and magnetoreception by horses and other non-human animals. Proc. RIN89 Orientation and Navigation: Birds, Humans and Other Animals. Royal Institute of Navigation, Cardiff 1989, Paper 12.Google Scholar
Benvenuti, S. & Ioalé, P. (1988). Initial orientation of homing pigeons: different sensitivity to altered magnetic fields in birds of different countries. Experientia, 44, 358359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berthold, P. (1988). The control of migration in European warblers. In Acta XIX Congressus Internationalis Ornithologicus 1986 (ed. Ouellet, H.), pp. 215294. University of Ottawa Press, Ottawa.Google Scholar
Billings, S. M. (1968). Homing in Leach's Petrels. Auk, 85, 3643.Google Scholar
Bovet, J. (1978). Homing in wild myomorph rodents: current problems. Animal Migration, Navigation, and Homing, (ed. Schmidt–Koenig, K. & Keeton, W. T.), pp. 405412. Springer, Heidelberg, Berlin, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bub, H. (1962). Heimfindeversuche mit Haussperlingen in Nordwestdeutschland. Falke, 9, 164171.Google Scholar
Exner, S. (1905). Über das Orientierungsvermögen der Brieftauben. Sitzungsberichte Akademie der Wissenschaft Wien, Mathematische Naturwissenschaft Klasse III, 114, 763790.Google Scholar
von Frisch, K. (1968). Dance, Language and Orientation of Bees. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass.Google Scholar
Griffin, D. R. (1944). The sensory basis of bird navigation. Quarterly Review of Biology, 19, 1531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, D. R. (1949). Homing experiments with Leach's Petrels. Auk, 57, 6174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grüter, M. & Wiltschko, R. (1990). Pigeon homing: the effect of local experience on initial orientation and homing success. Ethology, 84, 239255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heinroth, O. & Heinroth, K. (1941). Das Heimfinde-Vermögen der Brieftauben. Journal för Ornithologie, 89, 213256.Google Scholar
Helbig, A. J. (1991). Inheritance of migratory direction in a bird species: a cross-breeding experiment with SE- and SW-migrating blackcaps (Sylvia atricapilla). Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 28, 912.Google Scholar
Hölldobler, B. & Wilson, E. O. (1990). The Ants. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ioalè, P. (1984). Magnets and pigeon orientation. Monitore Zoologico ltaliano (n.s.), 18, 347358.Google Scholar
Janzen, D. H. (1971). Euglossine bees as long-distance pollinators of tropical plants. Science, 171, 203205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keeton, W. T. (1969). Orientation by pigeons: is the Sun necessary? Science, 165, 922928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keeton, W. T. (1971). Magnets interfere with pigeon homing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 68, 102106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keeton, W. T. (1973). Release-site bias as a possible guide to the ‘map’ component in pigeon homing. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 86, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keeton, W. T., Larkin, T. S. & Windsor, D. M. (1974). Normal fluctuations in the Earth's magnetic field influence pigeon orientation. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 95, 95103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenyon, K. W. & Rice, D. W. (1958). Homing of Laysan Albatross. Condor, 60, 36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiepenheuer, J. (1978). The effect of magnetic fields inversed during displacement on the homing behaviour of pigeons. In Animal Migration, Navigation, and Homing (ed. Schmidt-Koenig, K. & Keeton, W. T.), pp. 135142. Springer, Heidelberg, Berlin, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, G. (1953). Wird die Sonnenhöhe bei der Heimfindeorientierung verwendet? Journal für Ornithologie, 94, 201219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, G. (1959). Recent experiments on bird orientation. Ibis, 101, 399416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larkin, T. & Keeton, W. T. (1978). An apparent lunar rhythm in the day-to-day variations in initial bearings of homing pigeons. In Animal Migration, Navigation, and Homing (ed. Schmidt-Koenig, K. & Keeton, W. T.), pp. 92106. Springer, Heidelberg, Berlin, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindauer, M. (1975). The effective information contents of the dance of the honeybee. Fortschritte der Zoologie, 23, 3132.Google Scholar
Mather, J. G. & Baker, R. R. (1981). Magnetic sense of direction in woodmice for route-based navigation. Nature, 291, 152155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathis, A. & Moore, F. R. (1988). Geomagnetism and the homeward orientation of the box turtle, Terrapene Carolina. Ethology, 78, 265274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, G. V. T. (1952). An investigation of homing ability in two species of gulls. Ibis, 94, 243264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, G. V. T. (1953). Sun navigation in homing pigeons. Journal of Experimental Biology, 30, 243267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, B. R. (1988). Magnetic fields and orientation in homing pigeons: experiments of the late Keeton, W. T.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 85, 49074909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueller, H. C. (1966). Homing and distance-orientation in bats. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 23. 403421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murie, M. (1963). Homing and orientation of deermice. Journal of Mammalogy, 44, 338349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papi, F. (1976). The olfactory navigation system of the homing pigeon. Verhandlungen der Deutschen zoologischen Gesellschaft, 69, 184205.Google Scholar
Papi, F. (1986). Pigeon navigation: solved problems and open questions. Monitore Zoologico Italiano (n.s.), 20, 471517.Google Scholar
Papi, F., Ioalè, P., Fiaschi, V. & Benvenuti, S. (1984). Pigeon homing: the effect of outward-journey detours on orientation. Monitore Zoologico Italiano (n.s.), 18, 5387.Google Scholar
Pardi, L., Ugolini, A., Faqi, A. S., Scapini, F. & Ercolini, A. (1988). Zonal recovering in equatorial sandhoppers: interaction between magnetic and solar orientation. In Behavioural Adaption to Intertidal Life (ed. Chelazzi, G. & Vannini, M.), pp. 7992. Plenum Press, New York, London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, J. B. (1986). Two magnetoreception pathways in a migratory salamander. Science, 233,. 765767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quine, D. B. (1982). Infrasound: a potential cue for homing pigeons. In Avian Navigation (ed. Papi, F. & Wallraff, H. G.), pp. 373376. Springer, Heidelberg, Berlin, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynaud, G. (1900). The orientation of birds. Bird-Lore, 2, 101108 and 141147.Google Scholar
Robinson, W. L. & Falls, J. B. (1965). A study of homing of meadow mice. American Midland Naturalist, 73, 188224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodda, G. H. (1984). The orientation and navigation of juvenile alligators: evidence of magnetic sensitivity. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 154, 649658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rüppell, W. (1934). Versuche zur Ortstreue und Fernorientierung der Vögel III. Heimfinde-Versuche mit Rauchschwalben (Hirundo rustica) und Mehlschwalben (Delichon urbica) Warnat, von H. (Berlin-Charlottenburg). Vogelzug, 5, 161166.Google Scholar
Rüppell, W. (1936). Heimfindeversuche mit Staren 1934. Journal für Ornithologie, 83, 462524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rüppell, W. (1936). Heimfindeversuche mit Staren und Schwalben 1935, 84, 180198.Google Scholar
Rüppell, W. (1937). Heimfindeversuche mit Staren, Rauchschwalben, Wendehälsen, Rotrückenwürgern und Habichten (1936). Journal für Ornithologie, 85, 120135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schlichte, H-J. (1973). Untersuchungen über die Bedeutung optischer Parameter für das Heimkehrverhalten der Brieftauben. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 32, 257280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt-Koenig, K. (1960). Internal clocks and homing. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology, 25, 389393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt-Koenig, K. (1970). Ein Versuche, theoretisch möogliche Navigationsverfahren von Vögeln zu klassifizieren und relevante sinnesphysiologische Probleme zu umreiβen. Verhandlungen der Deutschen zoologischen Gesellschaft, 64, 243244.Google Scholar
Schmidt-Koenig, K. (1987). Bird navigation: has olfactory orientation solved the problem? Quarterly Review of Biology, 62, 3147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schöps, M. (1991). Der Einfluβ von Infraschall auf das Orientierungssystem von Brieftauben. Verhandlungen der Deutschen zoologischen Gesellschaft, 84, 361362.Google Scholar
Tögel, A. & Wiltschko, R. (1992). Detour experiments with homing pigeons: information obtained during the outward journey is included in the navigational process. Behavioral Ecology & Sociology, 3, 7379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uchida, T. & Kuwabara, M. (1951). The homing instinct of the honeybee, Apis mellifica. Journal of the Faculty of Science, Hokkaido University, 10, 8796.Google Scholar
Ugolini, A. (1986). Homing ability in Polistes gallicus L. (Hymenoptera, Vespidae). Monitore Zoologico Italiano (n.s.), 20, 115.Google Scholar
Viguier, C. (1882). Le sens de l'orientation et ses organes chez les animaux et chez l'homme. Revue Philosophique de la France et de l'Etranger, 14, 136.Google Scholar
Visalberghi, E. & Alleva, E. (1979). Magnetic influences on pigeons homing. Biological Bulletin, 125, 246256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walcott, C. (1978). Anomalies in the Earth's magnetic field increase the scatter of pigeons vanishing bearings. In Animal Migration, Navigation, and Homing (ed. Schmidt-Koenig, K. & Keeton, W. T.), pp. 143151. Springer, Heidelberg, Berlin, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walcott, C. (1988). Homing in pigeons: are differences in experimental results due to different home-loft environments? In Acta XIX Congressus Internationalis Ornithologicus (ed. Ouellet, H.), pp. 305308. University of Ottawa Press, Ottawa.Google Scholar
Walcott, C. & Green, R. P. (1974). Orientation of homing pigeons altered by a change in the direction of an applied magnetic field. Science, 184, 180182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
1Wallraff, H. G. (1970). Über die Flugrichtung verfrachteter Brieftauben in Abhängigkeit vom Heimatort und vom Ort der Freilassung. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 27, 303351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2Wallraff, H. G. (1974). Das Navigationssystem der Vögel. Ein theoretischer Beitrag zur Analyse ungeklärter Orientierungsleistungen. R. Oldenbourg, München, Wien.Google Scholar
3Wallraff, H. G., Foà, A. & Ioalè, P. (1980). Does pigeon homing depend on stimuli perceived during displacement? Experiments in Italy. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 139, 203208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4Weintraub, J. D. (1970). Homing in the lizard Sceloporus orcutti. Animal Behaviour, 18, 132137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5Wiltschko, R. (1980). Die Sonnenorientierung der Vögel. I. Die Rolle der Sonne im Orientierungssystem und die Funktionsweise des Sonnenkompaβ. Journal für Ornithologie, 121, 121143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
6Wiltschko, R. (1983). The ontogeny of orientation in young pigeons. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, 76A, 701708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7Wiltschko, R. (1991). The role of experience in avian navigation and homing. In Orientation in Birds (ed.Berthold, P.), pp. 250269. Birkhäuser Verlag, Basel.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8Wiltschko, R. (1992). Das Verhalten verfrachteter Vögel. Vogelwarte, 36, 249310.Google Scholar
9Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (1978). Evidence for the use of magnetic outward-journey information in homing pigeons. Naturwissenschaften, 65, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
10Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (1985). Pigeon homing: change in navigational strategy during ontogeny. Animal Behaviour, 33, 583590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (1989). Pigeon homing: olfactory orientation – a paradox. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 24, 163173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
12Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (1990). Zur Entwicklung des Sonnenkompaβ bei jungen Brieftauben. Journal für Ornithologie, 131, 120.Google Scholar
13Wiltschko, W. & Wiltschko, R. (1982). The role of outward journey information in the orientation of homing pigeons. In Avian Navigation (ed. Papi, F. & Wallraff, H. G.), pp. 239252. Springer Verlag, Heidelberg, Berlin, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14Wiltschko, W. & Wiltschko, R. (1987). Cognitive maps and navigation in homing pigeons. In Cognitive Processes and Spatial Orientation in Animal and Man (ed. Ellen, P. & Thinus-Blanc, C.), pp. 201216. Martinus Nijhoff, Dordrecht.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15Wiltschko, W. & Wiltschko, R. (1988). Magnetic orientation in birds. Current Ornithology, 5, 67121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16Wiltschko, W. & Wiltschko, R. (1991). Der Magnetkompaβ als Komponente eines komplexen Richtungsorientierungssystems. Zoologische Jahrbücher (Physiologie), 95, 437446.Google Scholar
17Wiltschko, W., Wiltschko, R., Keeton, W. T. & Madden, R. (1983). Growing up in an altered magnetic field affects the initial orientation of young homing pigeons. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 12, 135142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
18Wiltschko, W., Wiltschko, R. & Keeton, W. T. (1984). Effect of a ‘permanent' clock-shift on the orientation of experienced homing pigeons. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 15, 263272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19Wiltschko, W., Wiltschko, R., Keeton, W. T. & Brown, A. I. (1987a). Pigeon homing: the orientation of young birds that had been prevented from seeing the Sun. Ethology, 76, 2732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
20Wiltschko, W., Wiltschko, R. & Walcott, C. (1987b). Pigeon homing: different effects of olfactory deprivation in different countries. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 21, 333342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21Wiltschko, W., Daum-Benz, P., Munro, U. & Wiltschko, R. (1989). Interaction of magnetic and stellar cues in migratory orientation. This Journal, 42, 355366.Google Scholar
22Wiltschko, W., Beason, R. C. & Wiltschko, R. (1991). Concluding remarks: Sensory basis of orientation. In Acta XIX Congressus Internationalis Ornithologicus Christchurch (ed. Bell, B. D., Cossee, R. O., Flux, J. E. C., Heather, B. D., Hithmough, R. A., Robertson, C. J. R. & Williams, M. S.), pp. 18451850. Ornithological Congress Trust Board, Christchurch.Google Scholar
23Wodzicki, K. & Wojtusiak, R. J. (1934). Untersuchungen über die Orientation und Geschwindigkeit des Fluges bei Vögeln. I. Experimente an Schwalben (Hirudo rustica L.). Acta Ornithologica Musei Zoologici Polonici (Werk 1), 8, 253274.Google Scholar
24Wojtusiak, R. J. & Ferens, B. (1938). Untersuchungen über die Orientierung und Geschwindigkeit des Fluges bei Vögeln. IV. Heimkehrgeschwindigkeit und Orientierungsart beiden Rauchschwalben (Hirundo rustica L.). Bulletin de 1'Académie polonaise Cracovie (Sér. B), 2, 173201.Google Scholar
25Wojtusiak, R. J., Wodzicki, K. & Ferens, B. (1937). Untersuchungen über die Orientation und die Geschwindigkeit des Fluges der Vögel. II. Weitere Untersuchungen an Schwalben: Beeinflussung durch Nachtzeit und Gebirge. Acta Ornithologica Musei Zoologici Polonici (Werk 2), 4, 3961.Google Scholar
26Yeagley, H. L. (1947). A preliminary study of a physical basis of bird navigation. Journal of Applied Physics, 18, 10351063.CrossRefGoogle Scholar