Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T16:52:57.244Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotional Intensity of Trauma Memory as Moderator of the Relationship Between Posttraumatic Cognitions and PTSD Symptoms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2016

Ma. Jenina N. Nalipay*
Affiliation:
Psychotrauma Clinic, The Graduate School, University of Santo Tomas, Manila, Philippines College of Graduate Studies and Teacher Education Research, Philippine Normal University, Manila, Philippines
Imelu G. Mordeno
Affiliation:
College of Education, Mindanao State University — Iligan Institute of Technology, Iligan City, Philippines
*
Address for correspondence: Ma. Jenina N. Nalipay, College of Graduate Studies and Teacher Education Research, Philippine Normal University, Taft Avenue, Manila, Philippines. Email: jnalipay@yahoo.com

Abstract

Individuals develop three types of cognitions in the aftermath of a traumatic experience: negative cognitions about the self, negative cognitions about the world, and self-blame (Foa et al., 1999). Although the relationship of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms and posttraumatic cognitions has been supported in literature, memory-related responses affecting this relationship need further exploration. It was the intention of the present study to address this gap by examining the moderating role of emotional intensity of trauma memory in the relationship between posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD symptoms. In a sample of survivors of typhoon Haiyan (N = 632), one of the strongest typhoons ever recorded, it was found that in general, negative cognitions about the self and the world, but not self-blame, predict PTSD symptoms; and emotional intensity of trauma memory generally moderates the relationship between posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD. The findings of the study would be useful in the development and enhancement of interventions to help the survivors of natural disasters in maintaining their mental health and wellbeing.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2016 

Typhoon Haiyan was one of the strongest typhoons to hit Southeast Asia. With its violent rains and winds of up to 170 mph when it made landfall, it caused massive casualties of lives and properties among Filipinos (Inquirer.net, 2013). A total of 6,300 individuals died and 28,689 were injured; 1,084,762 houses were damaged, leading to homelessness and forced displacement. Moreover, billions of pesos worth of properties were damaged. Due to the massive destruction caused by typhoon Haiyan, the Philippines was declared to be a national state of calamity (Del Rosario, Reference Del Rosario2014). Aside from the deaths of loved ones and loss of properties, other problems encountered by survivors included lack of food and drinking water, absence of electricity, and the lack of means of communication and transportation, as well as poor access to basic medical care (Dash, Viswanathan, & Amin-Hanjani, Reference Dash, Viswanathan and Amin-Hanjani2014). These experiences were likely to have a significant impact on the mental health and wellbeing of survivors.

Although numerous studies have examined factors influencing posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) among natural disaster survivors, such as sociodemographic characteristics (Adams et al., Reference Adams, Sumner, Danielson, McCauley, Resnick, Grös and Ruggiero2014; Ehring, Razik, & Emmelkamp, Reference Ehring, Razik and Emmelkamp2011; Kun et al., Reference Kun, Chen, Han, Gong, Chen, Zhang and Yao2009; Xu & Song, Reference Xu and Song2011), loss of family members, damage to properties (Chan et al., Reference Chan, Wang, Qu, Lu, Ran, Ho and Zhang2011; Kun et al., Reference Kun, Chen, Han, Gong, Chen, Zhang and Yao2009), and lack of social support (Ehring et al., Reference Ehring, Razik and Emmelkamp2011), there is limited information on the internal processes involved in the development and maintenance of PTSD symptoms among samples with this type of trauma (Constans et al., Reference Constans, Vasterling, Deitch, Han, Tharp, Davis and Sullivan2012). Two of the factors internal to the individual that have consistently been found to be associated with PTSD are emotional intensity of trauma memory and cognition related to traumatic event.

Posttraumatic Cognitions and PTSD

The role of cognition in the development and maintenance of PTSD symptoms has been highlighted in a number of theories. Horowitz's (Reference Horowitz1986) stress response model suggests that exposure to stressful events initially gives rise to an emotional response. This is then followed by an active cognitive processing, in which the individual tries to resolve the difference between his or her traumatic experience and pre-existing beliefs. During this stage, alternating PTSD symptoms of intrusion and denial are experienced. These continue until discrepancies are resolved. This process takes longer for individuals with more rigid pre-existing beliefs.

The stress response model has since been expounded in other theories. McCann and Pearlman (Reference McCann and Pearlman1990) hypothesised that disruptions in beliefs influenced by trauma experience may cover such themes as that of safety, trust, power, esteem, and intimacy. In addition, Resick, Schnicke, and Markway (Reference Resick, Schnicke and Markway1991) included cognitions of self-blame and undoing as a result of traumatic experience. Epstein (Reference Epstein, Lipsitt and Mitick1991) proposed four core beliefs that change when a person experiences trauma: that the world is benign, that the world is meaningful, that the self is worthy, and that people are trustworthy. Similarly, Janoff-Bulman (Reference Janoff-Bulman1992) postulated that victimisation shatters an individual's fundamental assumptions about the benevolence of the world, meaning of life, and self-worth. These theories emphasise how the experience of trauma may alter an individual's cognition.

Consistent with the other cognitive theories of trauma, Foa and colleagues’ (Foa & Kozak, Reference Foa and Kozak1986; Foa & Riggs, Reference Foa, Riggs, Oldham, Riba and Tasman1993; Foa, Steketee, & Rothbaum, Reference Foa, Steketee and Rothbaum1989) emotional processing theory posits that when basic assumptions of safety are violated, this could lead to cognitive distortions that the world is completely dangerous and the self is entirely incompetent, which then increases the risk for the development of PTSD (Foa & Riggs, Reference Foa, Riggs, Oldham, Riba and Tasman1993; Foa & Rothbaum, Reference Foa and Rothbaum1998). They further contend that as the traumatic event disrupts normal beliefs, survivors with more rigid pre-trauma beliefs are at risk to develop PTSD (Beck, Jacobs-Lentz, McNiff, Olsen, & Clapp, Reference Beck, Jacobs-Lentz, McNiff, Olsen, Clapp, Zoellner and Feeny2011).

Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) cognitive model of PTSD likewise focused on the central role played by cognitions in the development and maintenance of PTSD symptoms. This theory suggests that in contrast to individuals who are able to effectively resolve their trauma, those who develop PTSD experience a persistent sense of ongoing threat. Negative cognitive appraisals of the events and its consequences contribute to the perpetuation of PTSD symptoms as they trigger negative emotions and maladaptive coping mechanisms that temporarily reduce stress but prevent cognitive change, thus maintaining PTSD.

Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin, and Orsillo (Reference Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin and Orsillo1999), using principal components analysis, identified three factors of posttraumatic cognitions: negative cognitions about the self, negative cognitions about the world, and self-blame. The three factors were found to be related to the severity of PTSD and were able to differentiate between individuals with and without PTSD (Foa et al., Reference Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin and Orsillo1999; Su & Chen Reference Su and Chen2008; Van Emmerik, Schoorl, Emmelkamp, & Kamphuis Reference Van Emmerik, Schoorl, Emmelkamp and Kamphuis2006).

The relationships of cognitions about the self and the world with PTSD have been well established in the literature. However, the association between self-blame and PTSD failed to garner support in some studies (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Coffey, Palyo, Gudmundsdottir, Miller and Colder2004; Cieslak, Benight, & Caden Lehman, Reference Cieslak, Benight and Caden Lehman2008; Kolts, Robinson, & Tracy, Reference Kolts, Robinson and Tracy2004; Moser, Hajcak, Simons, & Foa, Reference Moser, Hajcak, Simons and Foa2007; Startup, Makgekgenene, & Webster, Reference Startup, Makgekgenene and Webster2007). A number of explanations have been proposed to account for the differences in findings with regard to the relationship between self-blame cognitions and PTSD. Beck and colleagues (Reference Beck, Coffey, Palyo, Gudmundsdottir, Miller and Colder2004) suggested that this could be due to the nature of participants’ trauma experience. It was noted that the participants in Foa et al.’s (Reference Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin and Orsillo1999) study were mostly survivors of sexual assault, in which self-blame reactions are more likely to be observed compared to the motor vehicle accident survivors in Beck et al.’s (2004) study. This assertion finds support from Müller et al. (Reference Müller, Wessa, Rabe, Dörfel, Knaevelsrud, Flor and Karl2010), indicating that self-blame correlated with PTSD samples who had interpersonal trauma, but not with accident-related trauma. Conversely, Cieslak et al.’s (Reference Cieslak, Benight and Caden Lehman2008) study on survivors of child sexual abuse failed to show a significant association between self-blame and PTSD symptoms.

In contrast to the findings of Foa et al. (Reference Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin and Orsillo1999), Startup et al. (Reference Startup, Makgekgenene and Webster2007) found a negative relationship between self-blame and the risk of PTSD diagnosis and severity of symptoms. This was interpreted in light of the protective role of behavioural self-blame, as well as possible statistical suppression effect, since when self-blame was analysed independently, its association with PTSD was positive; however, it became negative when the contribution of negative cognitions about the self was taken into account. This was supported by the findings of Blain, Galovski, Elwood, and Meriac (Reference Blain, Galovski, Elwood and Meriac2013), in which they found that higher self-blame cognitions indicate lower levels of numbing symptoms of PTSD. These inconsistencies in findings about the relationship between posttraumatic cognitions, especially self-blame, and PTSD symptoms underscore the need to investigate factors that possibly moderate this relationship.

Emotional Intensity of Trauma Memory and PTSD

The occurrence of an emotional response is inevitable when an individual is exposed to a traumatic situation, and thus may play an important part in the development of PTSD. A number of cognitive models of PTSD has acknowledged the role of emotional response as well. In Horowitz's (Reference Horowitz1976) conceptualisation of the stress response model, the initial phase of a response to stress is a state of outcry, which is characterised by an upsurge of intense, unpleasant emotions (e.g., sadness, fear). The occurrence of waves of intense emotions gives rise to symptoms of intrusion, while the numbing or blunting of these emotions leads to denial symptoms.

In Foa and Kozak's (Reference Foa and Kozak1986) emotional response theory, fear structures develop following exposure to trauma, which may be either normal or pathological. Normal fear structure is adaptive, while pathological fear structure is characterised by associations among feared stimuli, meaning representations that distort reality, and excessive physiological and behavioural responses that lead to trauma symptoms.

The interaction between cognitions and emotions are apparent as well in Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) cognitive model. This model postulates that PTSD symptoms accompanied by negative emotions such as anxiety, depression, or anger are experienced when an individual appraises their memory of the traumatic event in a way that it produces current threat. The here-and-now quality of emotions regarding the trauma memory may lead to problematic appraisals and dysfunctional coping strategies, which further aggravate PTSD symptoms and continue the PTSD cycle.

A wide range of negative emotions that accompany PTSD symptoms have been reported in literature. These include fear, hopelessness, horror, anger, disgust, sadness, guilt, and shame (Hathaway, Boals, & Banks, Reference Hathaway, Boals and Banks2010). Among the various properties of emotions, its intensity was found to have the greatest influence on the phenomenology of autobiographical memory. Emotional intensity was found to be a consistent predictor of recollection, belief in the memory's accuracy, vividness, narrative form, rehearsal, and visceral responses of autobiographical memory experience (Talarico, LaBar, & Rubin, Reference Talarico, LaBar and Rubin2004). Moreover, associations between emotional intensity and PTSD symptoms were also found in previous studies (Rubin, Boals, & Berntsen, Reference Rubin, Boals and Berntsen2008; St. Jacques, Botzung, Miles, & Rubin, Reference St Jacques, Botzung, Miles and Rubin2011). In line with this information, we hypothesise that emotional intensity of trauma memory moderates the relationship between posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD symptoms. We consider that as trauma cognitions become more distorted, the more PTSD symptoms occur, especially if the emotional intensity of the trauma memory is high.

The Present Study

The aim of the present study was to understand the moderating role of the emotional intensity of trauma memory on the relationship between posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD symptoms. By looking at the interactions, we address a number of issues that may help understand cognitive processes explaining the symptom development and maintenance of PTSD.

For instance, most studies on cognition and trauma treat PTSD in its totality rather than focusing on its specific clusters. This is unfortunate, considering vast studies have shown differential relationships of PTSD symptoms to other constructs such as anxiety (Gutner, Nillni, Suvak, Wiltsey-Stirman, & Resick, Reference Gutner, Nillni, Suvak, Wiltsey-Stirman and Resick2013), depression (Asmundson, Stapleton, & Taylor, Reference Asmundson, Stapleton and Taylor2004), and suicidal behaviours (Zuromski, Davis, Witte, Weathers, & Blevins, Reference Zuromski, Davis, Witte, Weathers and Blevins2014). So far, only the study of Blain et al. (Reference Blain, Galovski, Elwood and Meriac2013) related posttraumatic cognitions with specific clusters of PTSD symptoms. However, the symptoms they analysed were based on the numbing model (King, Leskin, King, & Weathers, Reference King, Leskin, King and Weathers1998) instead of the those from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

Furthermore, results pertaining to the relationships between posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD have been inconsistent, particularly on the influence of self-blame to posttraumatic stress. Thus, we would like to know if the relationship between posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD is predicated on the moderating role of emotional intensity of trauma memory. To the authors’ knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the effect of emotional intensity of trauma memory as a moderating variable in the relationship between posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD. In addition, the investigation on the dynamics of internal processes, specifically on cognition and memory, is essential, considering the extensive amount of support cognitive behaviour therapies have received from literature in terms of its effectiveness in the treatment of PTSD (Diehle, Schmitt, Daams, Boer, & Lindauer, Reference Diehle, Schmitt, Daams, Boer and Lindauer2014; Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Grey, Wild, Stott, Liness, Deale and Clark2013).

Methods

Participants and Procedures

The respondents of the study included 632 college student survivors of the disastrous typhoon Haiyan from a university in Tacloban, Philippines, one of the areas that was badly hit by the typhoon. The sample consisted of 20% males (n = 127) and 80% females (n = 505). The respondents’ ages ranged from 15 to 31, with a mean of 18.03 (SD = 1.783).

A letter requesting permission for test administration was sent to the university. Upon receipt of approval, testing sessions were conducted in association with the university guidance and counselling office. All the respondents of the study gave their written informed consent prior to the data gathering. Respondents were assured of anonymity and full confidentiality when the instructions and aims of the study were explained. The study followed all applicable ethical guidelines, and the procedures have been reviewed and approved by a university ethics review committee.

Measures

The Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI; Foa et al., Reference Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin and Orsillo1999) was used to measure posttraumatic cognitions. The questionnaire, composed of 33 items, assesses cognitions following a traumatic event. The items consisting of statements reflecting thoughts a person may have after a traumatic experience are rated on a scale of 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree). It has three subscales: negative cognitions about the self (21 items), negative cognitions about the world (7 items), and self-blame (5 items). Higher scores indicate more negative cognitions. The reliability coefficients of the scales are Cronbach's alpha of .904 for negative self-cognitions, .711 for negative world-cognitions, and .725 for self-blame.

To measure emotional intensity of trauma memory, the emotional intensity subscale of the Memory Experiences Questionnaire (MEQ; Sutin & Robins, Reference Sutin and Robins2007) was used. It is composed of six items rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), which assesses the intensity of the emotions that were felt at the time of encoding and retrieval of the memory experience (e.g., ‘As I am remembering the experience now, my feelings are very intense’; ‘My emotions are very intense concerning this event’; ‘The memory of this event evokes powerful emotions’). The participants were specifically instructed to answer the items based on their experience of typhoon Haiyan. The scale demonstrated adequate reliability and strong evidence of validity (Sutin & Robins, Reference Sutin and Robins2007). The reliability coefficient of the data from the current sample is Cronbach's alpha of .70.

The PTSD Checklist 5 (PCL-5; Weathers et al., Reference Weathers, Litz, Keane, Palmieri, Marx and Schnurr2013) was utilised to measure PTSD symptom severity. It is a 20-item self-report measure based on the DSM-5 criteria of PTSD, which includes symptoms of intrusion, avoidance, negative alterations in cognitions and mood, and alterations in arousal and reactivity. The experience of each symptom is rated on a scale of 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). Higher scores indicate more severe experience of PTSD symptoms. The PCL-5 has an internal consistency coefficient of Cronbach's alpha of .928; the reliability coefficients of the subscales intrusion, avoidance, negative alterations in cognitions and mood, and alterations in arousal and reactivity are .829, .814, .858, and .795, respectively.

The original English forms of the measures were used in the study. The participants were bilingual speakers who use English as their secondary language. Furthermore, all of them had passed the English proficiency test required for their admission to the university, which uses English as the medium of instruction. Thus, translation of the materials was deemed unnecessary.

Data Analysis

Three moderation models predicting each symptom cluster of PTSD were analysed using Hayes and Matthes’ (Reference Hayes and Matthes2009) Probing Interactions Procedure (MODPROBE), an add-on command in SPSS specifically designed for probing moderation effects in regression models. The focal predictor in model 1 was negative cognitions about the self; in model 2, negative cognitions about the world; and in model 3, self-blame cognitions. In all three models, age, sex, and the other posttraumatic cognitions were controlled for. All focal predictors and moderator were mean centred.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

The means and standard deviations of posttraumatic cognition factors, emotional intensity of trauma memory, and PTSD symptom clusters are shown in Table 1. The bivariate correlations are shown as well. All factors of posttraumatic cognitions, emotional intensity, and PTSD symptom clusters were found to be correlated with each other at p < .01 level.

Table 1 Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations (N = 632)

Note: Self = negative cognitions about self; World = negative cognitions about the world; Blame = self-blame; Emotional = emotional intensity; Intrusion = intrusion; Avoidance = avoidance; CogMood = negative alterations in cognitions and mood; Arousal = alterations in arousal and reactivity; PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder total score. All correlations are significant at α = .01.

Moderation Analyses

The results of moderation analyses are shown in Table 2. The moderation effect of emotional intensity of trauma memory was found in the relationships of negative cognitions about the self, negative cognitions about the world, and self-blame with PTSD symptom severity. In model 1, negative cognitions about the self predicted all PTSD symptom clusters. However, its interaction with emotional intensity significantly increased the explained variance in total PTSD severity (B = 2.266, p < .01; ∆R 2 = .008, p < .01) and in specific symptom clusters of alterations in cognitions and mood (B = 1.006, p < .01; ∆R 2 = .010, p < .01) and alterations in arousal and reactivity (B = .617, p < .05; ∆R2 = .006, p < .05).

Table 2 Results of Moderation Analysis (N = 632)

Note: Self = negative cognitions about self; World = negative cognitions about the world; Blame = self-blame; Emotional = emotional intensity; Intrusion = intrusion; Avoidance = avoidance; CogMood = negative alterations in cognitions and mood; Arousal = alterations in arousal and reactivity; Covariates for all models include age, sex, and other posttraumatic cognitions; B = unstandardised coefficient.

*p < .05; **p < .01.

Likewise, in model 2, emotional intensity was a moderator in the relationship between negative cognitions about the world and PTSD. Whereas negative world-cognitions predicted overall PTSD symptom severity and alterations in arousal and reactivity, the explained variance increased significantly with its interaction with emotional intensity (B = 1.693, p < .01; ∆R 2 = .009, p < .01 and B = .440, p < .05; ∆R2 = .007, p < .05, respectively). Furthermore, negative world cognitions predicted negative alterations in cognitions and mood only when emotional intensity was high (B = .736, p < .01; ∆R 2 = .011, p < .01).

Similarly, emotional intensity's moderation effect was demonstrated in model 3. Self-blame alone predicted neither the total severity of PTSD, nor its specific symptoms. However, it was found that when emotional intensity is high, self-blame could lead to more severe overall PTSD (B = 1.982, p < .01; ∆R 2 = .011, p < .01), as well as specific symptoms of intrusion (B = .553, p < .05; ∆R2 = .008, p < .05), avoidance (B = .038, p < .05; ∆R 2 = .007, p < .05), negative alterations in cognitions and mood (B = .670, p < .01; ∆R 2 = .008, p < .01), and alterations in arousal and reactivity (B = .520, p < .01; ∆R 2 = .008, p < .01).

Discussion

The study aimed to examine the relationship of posttraumatic cognitions with each PTSD symptom cluster in a sample of natural disaster survivors, as well as find out if emotional intensity of trauma memory moderates the relationship between posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD symptom severity. The results revealed that in general, posttraumatic cognitions about the self and the world, but not self-blame, predict PTSD symptom severity among natural disaster survivors; that each posttraumatic cognition factor relates differently with specific PTSD symptom clusters; and that emotional intensity generally moderates the relationship between posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD.

Posttraumatic Cognitions as Predictors of PTSD Symptoms

The finding that self-blame did not predict PTSD complements Beck et al.’s (Reference Beck, Coffey, Palyo, Gudmundsdottir, Miller and Colder2004) study among vehicle accident survivors. One explanation of this result is the inference among survivors that they could not be the cause of the disaster; thus, they are likely to experience less self-blame. On the other hand, the relationship between self-blame and PTSD symptoms is more likely to be observed among victims of interpersonal trauma (Müller et al., Reference Müller, Wessa, Rabe, Dörfel, Knaevelsrud, Flor and Karl2010).

Negative cognitions about the self appeared to be the strongest predictor of PTSD symptom severity, as it predicted all PTSD symptom clusters. These findings support Moser et al.’s (Reference Moser, Hajcak, Simons and Foa2007) assertion that negative self-cognitions relate more to PTSD symptom severity than other types of trauma-related cognitions after finding out that self-cognitions uniquely predicted PTSD severity when variances contributed by other trauma-related cognitions, gender, and other symptoms such as depression and anxiety have been accounted for.

The result is also in accordance with the findings of Foa and Rauch (Reference Foa and Rauch2004) that decreased negative cognitions about the self directly led to reduced PTSD symptoms, while decreased cognitions about the world were associated with reduced PTSD through its association with cognitions about the self. Similarly, Constans et al. (Reference Constans, Vasterling, Deitch, Han, Tharp, Davis and Sullivan2012) found that among survivors of natural disaster, negative beliefs about the self related strongly with PTSD symptom severity, even after controlling for stress exposures, perceived social support, and damage to property.

The current study's finding that negative cognitions about the world predict increased PTSD is consistent with some studies (e.g., Beck et al., Reference Beck, Coffey, Palyo, Gudmundsdottir, Miller and Colder2004; Cieslak et al., Reference Cieslak, Benight and Caden Lehman2008) but is not supported by others (e.g., Shahar, Noyman, Schnidel-Allon, & Gilboa-Schechtman, Reference Shahar, Noyman, Schnidel-Allon and Gilboa-Schechtman2013; Schumm, Dickstein, Walter, Owens, & Chard, Reference Schumm, Dickstein, Walter, Owens and Chard2015). However, we contend that the findings could be understood more in the context of culture. Filipinos, whose culture is more collectivist in orientation, value personalised social relationships and social harmony (Triandis, McCusker, & Hui, Reference Triandis, McCusker and Hui1990). They embed others in settings, situations, or contexts in which their selves are connected (Markus & Kitayama, Reference Markus and Kitayama1991). Therefore, whatever difficulties they may experience, they expect others, particularly family and in-group members, to help and support them (Mordeno, Nalipay & Cue, Reference Mordeno, Nalipay and Cue2015). Thus, the negative world cognitions brought about by a traumatic event run contrary to their pre-existing beliefs. These dissonant cognitions expectedly lead to psychological distress, which likely contribute to the increase of their PTSD level.

Emotional Intensity of Trauma Memory as Moderator

Emotional intensity was consistently found to be a moderator of the relationship between posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD symptoms. These results suggest that the relationship of PTSD symptom severity is contingent on one's emotional intensity of trauma memory. Particularly, emotional intensity interacts with negative self- and world cognitions, leading to increased alterations in cognitions and mood, and alterations in arousal and reactivity. These findings could be explained by the reciprocal relationship of the trauma memory and the appraisal of the trauma and its consequences, as postulated in the cognitive model of PTSD (Ehlers & Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000). The intense emotions an individual feels regarding the memory of the traumatic event could influence their appraisal of such memories to become more negative and problematic, thus aggravating the PTSD symptoms further. Therefore, when the intensity of emotion of the trauma memory is high, it will amplify the negative thoughts about oneself and the world. Consequently, this will naturally lead to an increased occurrence of PTSD symptoms.

Perhaps one of the most interesting findings in this study was that emotional intensity of trauma memory was found to be a moderator of the relationship between self-blame and PTSD symptoms. Additionally, self-blame's interaction with emotional intensity predicts all four clusters of PTSD symptoms. It is interesting to note that self-blame alone did not significantly predict PTSD symptoms. However, the interaction of self-blame and emotional intensity significantly increased symptoms of PTSD. Further scrutiny of the results yield that as emotional intensity of the memory increases, the relationship between self-blame and PTSD strengthens. It could be explicated therefore that the inconsistency of the relationship between self-blame and PTSD is contingent on the intensity of survivors’ trauma memory. Blain et al. (Reference Blain, Galovski, Elwood and Meriac2013) suggested that the intensity of emotional reactions of guilt, shame, despair, anger, and sadness is associated with heightened self-blame reactions that could ultimately result in elevated levels of PTSD symptoms.

Limitations of the Study

Notwithstanding the contributions of this study, some limitations must be considered in the interpretation of its results. First, the study utilised self-report measures, which are prone to biases such as social desirability. Moreover, while the measures of posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD symptoms have previously been used among Filipino natural disaster survivors (Nalipay, Mordeno, & Saavedra, Reference Nalipay, Mordeno and Saavedra2015), the instruments used in this study were not specifically developed for this sample and may not fully capture these constructs in the context of the Filipino culture. Thus, further validation is recommended. Second, the participants of the study were predominantly young and female, and thus the results may not generalise to other samples. Third, although moderation analysis allows for testing of intrinsically causal relationships (Wu & Zumbo, Reference Wu and Zumbo2008), caution must be taken in interpreting causal relationships among variables because the study employed a cross-sectional design. Fourth, only the overall emotional intensity of the participants was considered. It is possible that the participants may experience a wide range of emotions of varying intensity. It is thus suggested that specific emotions be considered in future research. Fifth, although it is evident that emotional intensity is a consistent moderator between posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD, the increment in R² is relatively small. This suggests that while emotional intensity moderates consistently between posttraumatic cognitions and components of PTSD, there could be other factors that may also explain the links. Future studies are needed to look into other possible moderators. Lastly, the study focused only on internal processes (i.e., cognitions and emotional intensity) involved in PTSD. Nevertheless, we acknowledge that other factors such as the social and physical aftermath of the disaster and the availability of personal, social, and material support are important as well in having a full understanding of PTSD.

Conclusion

Despite its limitations, the study was able to impart some important insights. It investigated the role of posttraumatic cognitions in the development of PTSD symptoms in a sample of natural disaster survivors; examined how each type of posttraumatic cognitions relate with each symptom cluster of PTSD; and demonstrated the moderation effect of emotional intensity of trauma memory in the relationship between posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD symptoms. Although the moderating effect sizes may be construed as minimal, the results showed that emotional intensity as a moderator is important in the context of designing and implementing appropriate interventions. In this case, for example, we contend that focusing on lowering emotional intensity (i.e., arousal reduction) will likely contribute to decreasing PTSD brought about by lowered posttraumatic cognitions.

Declaration of Interest

None.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the assistance provided by the Guidance and Counseling Office of Leyte Normal University for this study. This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit.

References

Adams, Z.W., Sumner, J.A., Danielson, C.K., McCauley, J.L., Resnick, H.S., Grös, K.,. . . Ruggiero, K.J. (2014). Prevalence and predictors of PTSD and depression among adolescent victims of the Spring 2011 tornado outbreak. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 10471055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asmundson, G.J., Stapleton, J.A., & Taylor, S. (2004). Are avoidance and numbing distinct PTSD symptom clusters? Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17, 467475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author.Google Scholar
Asmundson, G.J., Stapleton, J.A., & Taylor, S. (2004). Are avoidance and numbing distinct PTSD symptom clusters?.Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17, 467475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, J.G., Coffey, S.F., Palyo, S.A., Gudmundsdottir, B., Miller, L.M., & Colder, C.R. (2004). Psychometric Properties of the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): A replication with motor vehicle accident survivors. Psychological Assessment, 16, 289298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, J.G., Jacobs-Lentz, J., McNiff, J., Olsen, S.A., & Clapp, J.D. (2011). Understanding post-trauma cognitions and beliefs. In Zoellner, L.A., & Feeny, N.C. (Eds.), Facilitating resilience and recovery following traumatic events (pp. 167190). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Blain, L.M., Galovski, T.E., Elwood, L.S., & Meriac, J.P. (2013). How does the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory fit in a four-factor posttraumatic stress disorder world? An initial analysis. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 5, 513520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, C.L.W., Wang, C.W., Qu, Z., Lu, B.Q., Ran, M.S., Ho, A.H.Y.,. . . Zhang, X. (2011). Posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms among adult survivors of the 2008 Sichuan earthquake in China. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 24, 295302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cieslak, R., Benight, C.C., & Caden Lehman, V. (2008). Coping self-efficacy mediates the effects of negative cognitions on posttraumatic distress. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 788798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Constans, J.I., Vasterling, J.J., Deitch, E., Han, X., Tharp, A.L.T., Davis, T.D., & Sullivan, G. (2012). Pre-Katrina mental illness, postdisaster negative cognitions, and PTSD symptoms in male veterans following Hurricane Katrina. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 4, 568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dash, D., Viswanathan, V., & Amin-Hanjani, S. (2014). Typhoon Haiyan: any role for neurosurgery in natural disasters? World Neurosurgery, 5, 660661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Del Rosario, E.D. (2014). NDRRMC updates: Updates regarding the effects of typhoon ‘Yolanda’ (Haiyan). National Disaster Risk Reduction and Management Council (2014, April 17). Retrieved February 8, 2016, from http://www.ndrrmc.gov.ph/attachments/article/1329/Update_on_Effects_Typhoon_YOLANDA_(Haiyan)_17APR2014.pdf.Google Scholar
Diehle, J., Schmitt, K., Daams, J.G., Boer, F., & Lindauer, R.J. (2014). Effects of psychotherapy on trauma-related cognitions in posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 27, 257264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., & Clark, D.M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 319345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Grey, N., Wild, J., Stott, R., Liness, S., Deale, A.,. . . Clark, D.M. (2013). Implementation of cognitive therapy for PTSD in routine clinical care: Effectiveness and moderators of outcome in a consecutive sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51, 742752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehring, T., Razik, S., & Emmelkamp, P.M. (2011). Prevalence and predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder, anxiety, depression, and burnout in Pakistani earthquake recovery workers. Psychiatry Research, 185, 161166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, S. (1991). Impulse control and self-destructive behavior. In Lipsitt, L.P. & Mitick, L.L. (Eds.), Self-regulatory behavior and risk-taking: Causes and consequences (pp. 273284). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Foa, E.B., Ehlers, A., Clark, D.M., Tolin, D.F., & Orsillo, S.M. (1999). The Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): Development and validation. Psychological Assessment, 11, 303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foa, E.B., & Kozak, M.J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 2035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, E.B., & Rauch, S.A.M. (2004). Cognitive changes during prolonged exposure versus prolonged exposure plus cognitive restructuring in female assault survivors with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 879884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, E.B., & Riggs, D.S. (1993). Post-traumatic stress disorder in rape victims. In Oldham, J., Riba, M.B., & Tasman, A. (Eds.), Review of psychiatry (vol. 12, pp. 273303). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Foa, E.B., & Rothbaum, B.O. (1998). Treating the trauma of rape: Cognitive behavioral therapy for PTSD. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Foa, E.B., Steketee, G., & Rothbaum, B.O. (1989). Behavioral/cognitive conceptualization of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behavior Therapy, 20, 155176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutner, C.A., Nillni, Y.I., Suvak, M., Wiltsey-Stirman, S., & Resick, P.A. (2013). Longitudinal course of anxiety sensitivity and PTSD symptoms in cognitive-behavioral therapies for PTSD. Journal Of Anxiety Disorders, 27, 728734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hathaway, L.M., Boals, A., & Banks, J.B. (2010). PTSD symptoms and dominant emotional response to a traumatic event: An examination of DSM-IV Criterion A2. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 23, 119126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, A.F., & Matthes, J. (2009). Computational procedures for probing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 924936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horowitz, M. (1976). Stress response syndromes. New York, NY: Jason Aronson.Google Scholar
Horowitz, M.J. (1986). Stress-response syndromes: A review of posttraumatic and adjustment disorders. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 37, 241249.Google ScholarPubMed
Inquirer.net. (2013, November 9). One of world's strongest typhoons lashes Philippines. Inquirer.net. Retrieved January 2, 2015, from http://newsinfo.inquirer.net/523635/yolanda-one-of-worlds-strongest-typhoons-blasts-philippines Google Scholar
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Towards a new psychology of trauma. New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
King, D.W., Leskin, G.A., King, L.A., & Weathers, F.W. (1998). Confirmatory factor analysis of the clinician-administered PTSD Scale: Evidence for the dimensionality of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Assessment, 10, 9096.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kun, P., Chen, X., Han, S., Gong, X., Chen, M., Zhang, W., & Yao, L. (2009). Prevalence of post-traumatic stress disorder in Sichuan Province, China after the 2008 Wenchuan earthquake. Public Health, 123, 703707.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolts, R.L., Robinson, A.M., & Tracy, J.J. (2004). The relationship of sociotropy and autonomy to posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD symptomatology in trauma survivors. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 60, 5363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markus, H.R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCann, I.L., & Pearlman, L.A. (1990). Psychological trauma and the adult survivor: Theory, therapy, and transformation. New York, NY: Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
Mordeno, I.G., Nalipay, M.J.N., & Cue, M.P. (2015). The role of functionality in the latent structure of posttraumatic growth among survivors of a flash flood disaster. Psychological Studies, 18.Google Scholar
Nalipay, M.J.N., Mordeno, I.G., & Saavedra, R.L.J. (2015). Cognitive processing, PTSD symptoms, and the mediating role of posttraumatic cognitions. Philippine Journal of Psychology, 48, 326.Google Scholar
Moser, J.S., Hajcak, G., Simons, R.F., & Foa, E.B. (2007). Posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in trauma-exposed college students: The role of trauma-related cognitions, gender, and negative affect. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 10391049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller, J., Wessa, M., Rabe, S., Dörfel, D., Knaevelsrud, C., Flor, H.,. . . Karl, A. (2010). Psychometric properties of the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI) in a German sample of individuals with a history of trauma. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 2, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Resick, P.A., Schnicke, M.K., & Markway, B.G. (1991, November). Personal beliefs and reactions scale: The relation between cognitive content and posttraumatic stress disorder. Paper presented at the 25th Annual Convention of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, New York.Google Scholar
Rubin, D.C., Boals, A., & Berntsen, D. (2008). Memory in posttraumatic stress disorder: Properties of voluntary and involuntary, traumatic and nontraumatic autobiographical memories in people with and without posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 591614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schumm, J.A., Dickstein, B.D., Walter, K.H., Owens, G.P., & Chard, K.M. (2015). Changes in posttraumatic cognitions predict changes in posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms during cognitive processing therapy.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shahar, G., Noyman, G., Schnidel-Allon, I., & Gilboa-Schechtman, E. (2013). Do PTSD symptoms and trauma-related cognitions about the self constitute a vicious cycle? Evidence for both cognitive vulnerability and scarring models. Psychiatry Research, 205, 7984.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Startup, M., Makgekgenene, L., & Webster, R. (2007). The role of self-blame for trauma as assessed by the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): A self-protective cognition? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 395403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
St Jacques, P.L., Botzung, A., Miles, A., & Rubin, D.C. (2011). Functional neuroimaging of emotionally intense autobiographical memories in post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45, 630637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Su, Y.J., & Chen, S.H. (2008). The Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory — Chinese Revised: Validation and refinement with a traumatized college sample in Taiwan. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 11101119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutin, A.R., & Robins, R.W. (2007). Phenomenology of autobiographical memories: The memory experiences questionnaire. Memory, 15, 390411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talarico, J.M., LaBar, K.S., & Rubin, D.C. (2004). Emotional intensity predicts autobiographical memory experience. Memory & Cognition, 32, 11181132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Triandis, H.C., McCusker, C., & Hui, C.H. (1990). Multimethod probes of individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 10061020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Emmerik, A.A.P., Schoorl, M., Emmelkamp, P.M.G., & Kamphuis, J.H. (2006). Psychometric evaluation of the Dutch version of the posttraumatic cognitions inventory (PTCI). Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 10531065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weathers, F.W., Litz, B.T., Keane, T.M., Palmieri, P.A., Marx, B.P., & Schnurr, P.P. (2013). The PTSD checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5). White River Junction, VT: National Center for PTSD, U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs. Retrieved from www.ptsd.va.gov Google Scholar
Wu, A.D., & Zumbo, B.D. (2008). Understanding and using mediators and moderators. Social Indicators Research, 87, 367392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, J., & Song, X. (2011). Posttraumatic stress disorder among survivors of the Wenchuan earthquake 1 year after: Prevalence and risk factors. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 52, 431437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuromski, K.L., Davis, M.T., Witte, T.K., Weathers, F., & Blevins, C. (2014). PTSD symptom clusters are differentially associated with components of the acquired capability for suicide. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 44, 682697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations (N = 632)

Figure 1

Table 2 Results of Moderation Analysis (N = 632)