Students who use various cognitive and meta-cognitive strategies are more likely to learn the material, have higher academic performance, and acquire more positive learning outcomes compared to their counterparts who are not as proficient in using these strategies (for reviews see Winne, Reference Winne, Zimmerman and Schunk2011; Zimmerman, Reference Zimmerman, Zimmerman and Schunk2011).
Numerous studies have investigated the predictors of cognitive and meta-cognitive strategy use. In general, antecedents identified in previous research have focused on cognitive-motivational constructs such as epistemological beliefs (e.g., Bromme, Pieschl, & Stahl, Reference Bromme, Pieschl and Stahl2010; Richter & Schmid, Reference Richter and Schmid2010), self-related beliefs (e.g., Garcia & Pintrich, Reference Garcia, Pintrich, Schunk and Zimmerman1994; Schunk & Usher, Reference Schunk, Usher, Zimmerman and Schunk2011; Zimmerman, Reference Zimmerman2000), achievement goals (e.g., Wolters, Reference Wolters2004), task values (Battle & Wigfield, Reference Battle and Wigfield2003), and interest (e.g., Hidi & Ainley, Reference Hidi, Ainley, Schunk and Zimmerman2008) among others (for an overview see also Hodis, Meyer, McClure, Weir, & Walkey, Reference Hodis, Meyer, McClure, Weir and Walkey2011; Walkey, McClure, Meyer, & Weir, Reference Walkey, McClure, Meyer and Weir2013; Wigfield, Klauda, & Cambria, Reference Wigfield, Klauda, Cambria, Zimmerman and Schunk2011). To date, scant attention has been paid to the role of emotions in facilitating or inhibiting the use of these strategies.
An exception would be the case of test anxiety, which has been well-studied in the literature (Zeidner, Reference Zeidner1998, Reference Zeidner, Schutz and Pekrun2007). It is important to examine how a wider range of emotions influence cognitive and meta-cognitive strategy use given that students experience a whole gamut of emotions related to school aside from anxiety (Boekaerts, Reference Boekaerts, Zimmerman and Schunk2011; Efklides, Reference Efklides2006; Meyer & Turner, Reference Meyer and Turner2002; Pekrun, Reference Pekrun, Wosnitza, Karabenick, Efklides and Nenninger2009). For example, students may enjoy school, be hopeful about succeeding in a test, or experience pride in their schoolwork. Alternatively, they may also feel bored in class or hopeless about the prospect of passing an exam. How do these distinct emotions influence the use of various cognitive and metacognitive strategies? The aim of this study was to answer this question by examining whether academic emotions could predict the use of various cognitive and metacognitive strategies.
Emotions
Early studies in educational psychology have mostly treated emotions as outcome variables, such as in the attribution-affect link specified in attribution theory (Weiner, Reference Weiner1985). However, there is also some support for its role as an antecedent of motivated behaviour. For example, research on test anxiety has shown that anxious students worry about how well they will do in school, thus exerting a negative influence on memory and test performance (see Wigfield & Eccles, Reference Wigfield and Eccles1989 for a review).
The role of emotions on learning is still hotly debated. Some researchers argue that positive emotions might be detrimental to the learning process. As Aspinwall (Reference Aspinwall1998, p. 7) noted, ‘our primary goal is to feel good, and feeling good makes us lazy thinkers who are oblivious to potentially useful negative information and unresponsive to meaningful variations in information and situation’. These researchers claim that positive emotions can lead to unrealistic appraisals, facilitate superficial information processing, and decrease motivation to pursue challenging goals (Aspinwall, Reference Aspinwall1998).
Some laboratory-based studies support this contention. For example, experimental studies have shown that the induction of positive moods can lead people to: (a) illusively overestimate the probability of attaining positive outcomes and underestimate the probability of attaining negative outcomes; (b) undermine effortful exertion by letting them think that everything is already going well; (c) neglect the careful prevention of future obstacles because of the motivation to maintain the pleasant mood and ignore negative thoughts; and (d) reduce the cognitive resources available for the task on hand (Aspinwall, Reference Aspinwall1998).
However, other researchers disagree with this stance and argue that positive emotions can indeed be beneficial for learning and performance (e.g., Pekrun et al., Reference Pekrun, Goetz, Titz and Perry2002). Experimental research has also shown that positive mood can enhance divergent thinking, facilitate flexible problem-solving, and improve cognitive performance (Isen, Daubman, & Nowicki, Reference Isen, Daubman and Nowicki1987). There is also some evidence showing the positive mood can facilitate elaborate information processing (Titz, Reference Titz2001).
More recent research provides support for the latter argument (Pekrun et al., Reference Pekrun, Goetz, Titz and Perry2002). Correlational evidence has illustrated the benefits of positive affect especially when these emotions help the people focus on the task at hand. A problem with the former argument — which construes positive emotions as non-beneficial for task outcomes — is that the experiments these researchers cite may lack external validity. Usually, the mood-induction procedures focused a participant's attention on emotion-arousing stimuli such as pictures or recall of significant life events, thus less attention was available for the task at hand. This may be the case for test anxiety, where the student worries too much about the upcoming exam, thereby limiting the cognitive resources available for studying. However, if the emotion is focused on the learning task itself, the situation becomes different. Rather than being distracted, the attention can be directed towards the task on hand. An example would be the experience of enjoyment in studying, which may help students become so immersed in the task that even the perception of time and borders between the self and the external environment is diminished in consciousness (i.e., flow experiences; Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi, Reference Csikszentmihalyi and Csikszentmihalyi1988).
Academic Emotions
Emotions are multifaceted phenomena consisting of multiple component processes that include affective, cognitive, physiological, and behavioral elements (Damasio, Reference Damasio, Manstead, Frijda and Fischer2004; Scherer, 2000). However, for this study the focus was on achievement emotions in general or academic emotions in particular.
Pekrun and colleagues (Reference Pekrun, Goetz, Titz and Perry2002) developed the control-value theory of emotions as a framework to understand achievement emotions, which refer to emotions linked to achievement activities or achievement outcomes such as success or failure. As the focus was on the academic domain in this research, the term ‘academic emotions’ was used to denote the emotions related to academic learning and achievement.
Pekrun et al. (Reference Pekrun, Goetz, Titz and Perry2002) proposed that emotions could be classified into: (1) positive activating emotions such as enjoyment, hope, and pride; (2) positive deactivating emotions such as relief; (3) negative activating emotions such as anger, anxiety, and shame; and (4) negative deactivating emotions such as boredom and hopelessness (see also Pekrun, Reference Pekrun2006, Reference Pekrun, Wosnitza, Karabenick, Efklides and Nenninger2009). In terms of valence, positive emotions can be differentiated from negative emotions such as pleasant enjoyment experienced during studying versus unpleasant anxiety felt before an upcoming exam. In terms of activation, physiologically activating emotions that facilitate excitement can be distinguished from deactivating emotions that induce relaxation. An example of such a distinction would be between activating anger that makes a student more physiologically aroused versus deactivating hopelessness that makes a student withdraw from the process.
Extant research has begun to map out the relationship of academic emotions to a wide range of educational outcomes. For example, Titz (Reference Titz2001) found that the positive emotions of enjoyment and hope were positively correlated with the use of elaboration strategies. The negative emotions of anger, anxiety, and boredom, on the other hand, were negatively related with elaboration. He also found enjoyment and hope to be positively correlated with effort, while anger, anxiety, and boredom were negatively correlated with them. Turner, Thorpe, and Meyer (Reference Turner, Thorpe and Meyer1998) found that negative emotions were detrimental to the preference for difficult work.
Other researchers have found academic emotions to be linked with performance outcomes. For example, Pekrun, Elliot, and Maier (Reference Pekrun, Elliot and Maier2009) found that positive activating emotions such as enjoyment, hope, and pride were positively related to students’ midterm exam scores. On the other hand, negative emotions — both activating and de-activating (anger, anxiety, shame, hopelessness, and boredom) — were negatively related to midterm exam scores. Villavicencio and Bernardo (Reference Villavicencio and Bernardo2013) found that enjoyment and pride were positive predictors of grades among undergraduate students (see also Villavicencio, Reference Villavicencio2011).
Academic emotions have also been linked to motivational orientations and effort exertion. The positive activating emotions of enjoyment, hope, and pride were found to be positively related to intrinsic motivation and effort in school, while the negative emotions of anger, anxiety, shame, hopelessness, and boredom were negatively related to intrinsic motivation and effort (Pekrun, Goetz, Frenzel, Barchfeld, & Perry, Reference Pekrun, Goetz, Frenzel, Barchfeld and Perry2011). Ouano (Reference Ouano2011) found that intrinsic motivation was positively related to positive academic emotions and that extrinsic motivation was positively related to the negative emotions.
Cultural Context
Most studies on academic emotions have been conducted in the Western setting (e.g., Govaerts & Gregoire, Reference Govaerts and Gregoire2008). Cross-cultural psychologists have argued that the relationships among variables may change as a function of cultural context. Henrich, Heine, and Norenzayan (Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010) criticised psychologists for overly relying on samples from WEIRD (Western, educated, industrialised, rich, democratic) societies and generalising these results to the rest of the world. Therefore, it is important to consider the issue of cross-cultural generalisability in this study.
Research on academic emotions in the Asian context in general and the Philippines in particular has been limited. The few existing studies have focused on validating the Academic Emotions Questionnaire (AEQ) developed by Pekrun and his colleagues. These studies have generally found support for the factor structure of the AEQ in various cultural contexts, including the Philippines (Frenzel, Thrash, Pekrun, & Goetz, Reference Frenzel, Thrash, Pekrun and Goetz2007; King, Reference King2010; see also Tze, Daniels, Klassen, & Li, Reference Tze, Daniels, Klassen and Li2013). Moreover, studies have also shown that the relationship of various academic emotions to outcome variables did not differ significantly from findings in Western cultures (e.g., Ouano, Reference Ouano2011; Villavicencio, Reference Villavicencio2011; Villavicencio & Bernardo, Reference Villavicencio and Bernardo2013).
Despite these cross-cultural similarities, there is also evidence of cultural differences. A qualitative study conducted by Bernardo, Ouano, and Salanga (Reference Bernardo, Ouano and Salanga2009) found support for the generalisability of the common academic emotions examined by Pekrun and his colleagues among Filipino students. However, they also highlighted that Filipino students’ academic emotions were broader than those typically examined in the Western research. In particular, Filipino students considered certain psychological phenomena as a type of academic emotion although Western research has not typically conceived of them as such.
Given the lack of concrete evidence for culture would modify the relationship between academic emotions and learning strategies, an agnostic stance was maintained in this study.
The Present Study
Extant research has suggested that academic emotions may be linked to various academic outcomes. However, a study that looks at how academic emotions are linked to a wide range of cognitive and meta-cognitive strategies is still lacking. The aim of this study is to examine the linkage between academic emotions and cognitive/meta-cognitive strategy use.
Based on previous studies showing the positive effects of positive academic emotions (Frenzel, Thrash, Pekrun, & Goetz, Reference Frenzel, Thrash, Pekrun and Goetz2007; Goetz, Frenzel, Pekrun, Hall, & Lüdtke, Reference Goetz, Frenzel, Pekrun, Hall and Lüdtke2007; Pekrun, Goetz, Titz, & Perry, Reference Pekrun, Goetz, Titz and Perry2002), it was hypothesised that positive academic emotions will have a positive association with various cognitive and meta-cognitive strategies (H1).
Given the negative association between negative academic emotions and a wide range of outcomes, it was hypothesised that negative academic emotions (both activating and deactivating) would be negative predictors of cognitive and metacognitive strategy use (H2).
Methods
Participants
The study involved 1,147 secondary school students (216 first-year students, 387 second-year students, 374 third-year students and 170 fourth-year students) from five different schools in Metro Manila, Philippines.
Four of these were private schools, while one was a public school. The private schools were all Catholic schools, had smaller class sizes, and catered to students from the middle income to upper income bracket. The public school was typical of the several public schools in the Metro Manila area. Compared to the private schools, it had a larger class size, and was non-sectarian. Students in the public school came from the lower to middle income bracket. All these four schools cater to students from first to fourth year and use English as the medium of instruction. There were 623 males and 524 females. The average age was 14.20 years old.
Procedures
The questionnaires were administered by the first author during class hours with the assistance of the classroom teachers. Students were informed that they can withdraw from the study at any time if they wished to do so.
Instruments
Academic emotions
The Short Version of the Academic Emotions Questionnaire for Filipinos (S-AEQ-F; King, Reference King2010), which was based on the Academic Emotions Questionnaire for Learning (AEQ; Pekrun et al., Reference Pekrun, Goetz, Frenzel, Barchfeld and Perry2011) was used. It comprises a total of 16 items, with each emotion being measured by 2 items. It has been previously validated among Filipino students using CFA (King, Reference King2010; King & Watkins, Reference King and Watkins2011). Academic emotions measured included the positive activating emotions of enjoyment (e.g., ‘I look forward to studying’), hope (e.g., ‘I feel optimistic that I will make good progress in studying’), and pride (e.g., ‘I think I can be proud of my accomplishments at studying’); the negative activating emotions of anger (e.g., ‘I get angry when I have to study’), anxiety (e.g., ‘I get tense and nervous while studying’), shame (e.g., ‘I feel ashamed because I am not as good as others in studying’); and the negative de-activating emotions of boredom (e.g., ‘Studying is dull and monotonous’), and hopelessness (e.g., ‘I feel hopeless when I think about studying’).
Cognitive strategies
Three types of cognitive strategies drawn from the cognitive strategies subscale of the Goal Orientation and Learning Strategies Survey (GOALS-S; Dowson & McInerney, Reference Dowson and McInerney2004) were measured: elaboration (e.g., ‘When learning things for school, I try to see how they fit together with other things I already know’), organising (e.g., ‘I try to organise my school notes when I want to learn things for school’), and rehearsal (e.g., ‘I reread my books when I want to learn things for school’). Each of the cognitive strategies were measured by six items.
Meta-cognitive strategies
Three types of meta-cognitive strategies drawn from GOALS-S (Dowson & McInerney, Reference Dowson and McInerney2004) were measured: monitoring (e.g., ‘I often ask myself questions to see if I understand what I am learning’), planning (e.g., ‘I often plan ahead so that I can do well in my schoolwork’), and regulating (e.g., ‘When I don't understand something at school, I try to get someone to help me’). Each of the metacognitive strategies were measured by six items.
Higher scores indicated a greater degree of endorsement. Questionnaires were all administered in English as it was the medium of instruction for Filipino students from the elementary to the tertiary level.
Data Analytic Strategy
Given the hierarchical nature of the current dataset (i.e., students nested within schools), hierarchical linear modelling (HLM; Raudenbush & Bryk, Reference Raudenbush and Bryk2002) analyses were conducted using HLM 6 for Windows (Raudenbush, Bryk, Cheong, Congdon, & du Toit, Reference Raudenbush, Bryk, Cheong, Congdon and du Toit2004).
First, fully unconditional models (i.e., null or intercept-only models) were fitted to partition the total variance in outcome measures into within-school variance (i.e., variance attributable to students) and between-school variance (i.e., variance attributable to schools). A fully unconditional model is analogous to a one-way random effects ANOVA model (Raudenbush & Bryk, Reference Raudenbush and Bryk2002). Finally, a full model that included both student- and school-level variables was estimated. Student-level predictors included the different types of academic emotions — enjoyment, hope, pride, anger, anxiety, shame, hopelessness, and boredom. We also included age and gender as covariates. For the school level, we included type of school (private school or public school) as a school-level predictor.
Restricted maximum likelihood was the method of estimation used. Dichotomous student- (e.g., gender) and school-level (e.g., type of school) variables were kept in their original metric. All continuous student-level variables were group-mean centred. Centring not only helps in the interpretation of the model parameters (Ferron et al., Reference Ferron, Hogarty, Dedrik, Hess, Niles, Kromrey, O’Connell and McCoach2008), but also ‘removes high correlations between random intercepts and slopes, and high correlations between first- and second-level variables and cross-level interactions’ (Kreft & de Leeuw, Reference Kreft and de Leeuw1998, p. 114). The HLM mixed model equations are given in the Appendix.
Results
Preliminary Analyses
Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics and internal consistency reliabilities of the various scales used in this study. Most of the scales had acceptable Cronbach's alpha reliabilities except for pride and anxiety scales had lower than ideal alpha estimates. Given that these constructs were only measured by two items each, it seems reasonable to expect that alphas obtained would be lower than ideal. It is usually the case that shorter versions of questionnaires (which is what was used in this study) have lower alphas compared to the full version of the questionnaire. In this study the pragmatic decision to use the short version of the scale was made given the time constraints imposed by the schools.
Correlations Among the Variables
Table 2 shows the bivariate correlations among the variables. The positive activating emotions of enjoyment, hope, and pride were all positively correlated with the various cognitive and meta-cognitive strategies, which were in line with theoretical expectations. Anger and boredom were both negatively associated with all the cognitive and meta-cognitive strategies. Anxiety had more ambiguous effects. It had weak positive correlations with rehearsal and monitoring. Hopelessness was not significantly associated with most of the cognitive and metacognitive strategies included in this study. For example, the correlations among the positive activating emotions of enjoyment, hope, and pride ranged from r = .48 to r = .65 (p < .001). However, they were not significantly correlated or negatively correlated with the negative activating and negative deactivating emotions. These correlations lend support to the classification of academic emotions proposed by Pekrun et al. (Reference Pekrun, Goetz, Titz and Perry2002). All the cognitive and metacognitive strategies were also moderately positively correlated with each other.
Note: +p < .10, *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
Partitioning of Variance in Cognitive and Meta-Cognitive Strategies
The intercept-only models (see Tables 3–4) indicated that students’ scores on the various cognitive strategies, which included elaboration (γ00 = 8.98), organisation (γ00 = 8.98), and rehearsal (γ00 = 8.87), varied across the five schools: χ2(4, N = 1147) = 67.58, p < .001; χ2(4, N = 1147) = 74.47, p < .001; χ2(4, N = 1147) = 94.19, p < .001 for elaboration, organisation, and rehearsal respectively. Moreover, for elaboration, 4% of the variance was between schools and 96% of the variance was within schools. For both organisation and rehearsal, 6% of the variance was between schools and 94% was within schools.
Note: Unstandardised coefficients are shown; N level 1 = 1,147 students; N level 2 = five schools; *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
Note: Unstandardised coefficients are shown; N level 1 = 1147 students, N level 2 = five schools; *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
Next, we analysed the intercept-only models for students’ meta-cognitive strategies. Results indicated that students’ scores on the metacognitive strategies — planning (γ00 = 8.73), monitoring (γ00 = 9.03), and regulating (γ00 = 8.85) — varied across the five schools: χ2(4, N = 1147) = 112.57, p < .001; χ2(4, N = 1147) = 91.41, p < .001; χ2(4, N = 1147) = 31.42, p < .001 for planning, monitoring, and regulating respectively. For planning, 8% of the variance was between schools and 92% was within schools. For monitoring, 6% of the variance was between schools and 94% was within schools; while for regulating, 2% of the variance was between schools and 98% was within schools.
Tables 3 and 4 also present the results of the full model with both student-level and school-level predictors. Student-level predictors included the various types of academic emotions (enjoyment, hope, pride, anger, anxiety, shame, hopelessness, and boredom). Gender and age were also entered as predictors at the student level to control for their effects. For the school level, we included school type (private vs. public) as a predictor.
Results indicated that the positive academic emotions of enjoyment, hope, and pride were positive predictors of all the cognitive and meta-cognitive strategies. In general, the negative academic emotions of anger, shame, hopelessness, and boredom did not significantly predict the various strategies. An exception was anxiety, which was a weak positive predictor for some types of strategies such as elaboration, rehearsal, monitoring, and regulating.
Discussion
The aim of this study was to explore whether academic emotions were systematically related to students’ cognitive and meta-cognitive strategy use. It was found that students who frequently experienced the positive emotions of enjoyment, hope, and pride were more likely to use various cognitive and meta-cognitive strategies, thus confirming the first hypothesis. These findings dovetail with previous research on academic emotions, which showed how positive activating emotions are related to optimal educational outcomes.
Recent studies in the field of positive psychology also corroborate the current findings about the benefits of experiencing positive emotions. Although not focusing specifically on emotions felt in school but on emotions in general, Fredrickson's (Reference Fredrickson1998, Reference Fredrickson2001) broaden and build theory argues that the frequent experience of positive emotions can broaden people's thoughts and behaviours and facilitate more adaptive responses to the environment. The experience of the positive activating emotions of enjoyment, hope, and pride in school may have helped the students broaden their cognitive repertoires, as evinced in their use of various cognitive and metacognitive strategies.
The second hypothesis was not confirmed. In general, most of the negative academic emotions were not significant predictors of cognitive/metacognitive strategy used in the regression model. There were also a certain degree of difference between the different types of academic emotions and how they related to strategy use. For example, anxiety was found to be a positive predictor of certain types of strategies. On the other hand, boredom and anger were negatively correlated with all the strategies.
It seems surprising that anxiety can positively predict the use of several types of cognitive and metacognitive strategies. However, it is possible that anxiety can also motivate certain types of students to use different strategies in their learning. For example, certain types of students may be highly anxious and yet also get high GPAs. Their anxiety might actually impel them to study harder and use various types of strategies so that they can cope with the exams. It is possible that a certain degree of anxiety might actually be helpful and it is the more severe types of anxiety that could be debilitating.
This study also showed the negative association of boredom and anger to all the strategies examined. Teachers and educators may need to pay special attention to curbing the onset of these two types of emotions in the school context. Boredom is especially harmful because it might make students tune out of school. Anger might also lead to other externalising aggressive behaviours, which may have detrimental consequences for learning.
The regression analysis showed that when all the emotions are included in the model, only positive activating emotions are highly significant. The other negative emotions mostly become non-significant. This shows the greater predictive power of positive emotions. This has important implications for practitioners who want to harness the power of positive emotions to facilitate student learning. While this study was strictly correlational and could not shed light on causal relations, studies using experimental designs lend support to the current conclusions. These experimental studies have generally shown positive emotions to be related to better learning (Bryan & Bryan, Reference Bryan and Bryan1991; Bryan, Mathur, & Sullivan, Reference Bryan, Mathur and Sullivan1996). For example, Bryan and Bryan (Reference Bryan and Bryan1991) induced positive emotions among students and asked them to solve math problems. Compared to those in the control condition, those who were in the positive emotions condition solved more math problems and had higher math self-efficacy. The current study adds to this growing literature by showing that positive emotions are also positively related to a broad range of cognitive and metacognitive strategies.
Limitations and Directions for Future Research
A limitation of this study was its cross-sectional design, which cannot reveal causal connections. Future research using longitudinal or experimental designs could be adopted. It is also likely that the relationships among academic emotions and the various cognitive and metacognitive strategies may be linked by reciprocal causation (Pekrun & Stephens, Reference Pekrun and Stephens2009). Thus, studies using cross-lagged designs that look at the reciprocal links between strategies and emotions across time may be helpful in revealing their dynamic relationships.
Another weakness is the exclusive reliance on self-report data, which is known to be affected by common method variance and social desirability issues (Fulmer & Frijters, Reference Fulmer and Frijters2009; Samueltstuen & Braten, 2007). While some researchers criticise the use of self-report in measuring cognitive and metacognitive strategy use (e.g., Winne & Jamieson-Noel, Reference Winne and Jamieson-Noel2002), in this article the tradition of Pintrich and his colleagues, who used self-report questionnaires to measure strategy use, was followed (e.g., Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & McKeachie, Reference Pintrich, Smith, Garcia and Mckeachie1993). Despite their weaknesses, self-report questionnaires are easy to use, suitable for large-scale surveys, and have the ability to tap into respondents’ beliefs and self-perceptions about their own learning processes (Perry & Winne, Reference Perry and Winne2006; Pintrich, Reference Pintrich2004; Zimmerman, Reference Zimmerman2008). However, it is acknowledged that future research should consider complementing self-reports with other types of data collection methods, such as the use of online measures of learning strategy use (Vandevelde, van Keer, & Rosseel, Reference Vandevelde, van Keer and Rosseel2013).
Third, only emotions tied to the school context were measured. Future studies may consider measuring more general types of emotions that may have a spillover effect on the more specific academic emotions. Fourth, students’ tendencies to give socially desirable responses were not measured. Social desirability may influence the results obtained and it is recommended that future studies measure social desirability responding. Fourth, we only conducted a quantitative study. It might be useful in future research to conduct a mixed-methods study that includes a qualitative component to investigate whether Filipino students’ understanding of learning strategies and academic emotions are concordant with Western theorising. It is also recommended that future studies consider the inclusion of possible moderators.
Conclusion
In general, this study showed the importance of taking academic emotions into account when investigating student learning. With few exceptions, such as test anxiety research (Zeidner, Reference Zeidner1998; Reference Zeidner, Schutz and Pekrun2007), the study of emotions in school has been relatively neglected, although this has been changing in recent years (e.g., Boekaerts, Reference Boekaerts, Zimmerman and Schunk2011; Efklides, Reference Efklides2006; Meyer & Turner, Reference Meyer and Turner2002; Pekrun et al., Reference Pekrun, Goetz, Titz and Perry2002). This study found that academic emotions are closely related to students’ cognitive and metacognitive strategy use. As the use of these strategies are hallmarks of successful learning, researchers and practitioners need to take into account their students’ emotional experiences in school in order to harness the power of these emotions to facilitate better learning.
Appendix HLM Mixed Model Equations
Null Models
ELABORATIONij = γ00 + u0j+ rij
ORGANIZINGij = γ00 + u0j+ rij
REHEARSALij = γ00 + u0j+ rij
PLANNINGij = γ00 + u0j+ rij
MONITORINGij = γ00 + u0j+ rij
REGULATINGij = γ00 + u0j+ rij
Full Models
ELABORATIONij = γ00 + γ01*PRIVATEPUBLICj + γ10*ENJOYMENTij + γ20*HOPEij + γ30*PRIDEij + γ40*ANGERij + γ50*ANXIETYij + γ60*SHAMEij + γ70*HOPELESSij + γ80*BOREDOMij + γ90*AGEij + γ100*GENDERij + u0j+ rij
ORGANIZINGij = γ00 + γ01*PRIVATEPUBLICj + γ10*ENJOYMENTij + γ20*HOPEij + γ30*PRIDEij + γ40*ANGERij + γ50*ANXIETYij + γ60*SHAMEij + γ70*HOPELESSij + γ80*BOREDOMij + γ90*AGEij + γ100*GENDERij + u0j+ rij
REHEARSALij = γ00 + γ01*PRIVATEPUBLICj + γ10*ENJOYMENTij + γ20*HOPEij + γ30*PRIDEij + γ40*ANGERij + γ50*ANXIETYij + γ60*SHAMEij + γ70*HOPELESSij + γ80*BOREDOMij + γ90*AGEij + γ100*GENDERij + u0j+ rij
PLANNINGij = γ00 + γ01*PRIVATEPUBLICj + γ10*ENJOYMENTij + γ20*HOPEij + γ30*PRIDEij + γ40*ANGERij + γ50*ANXIETYij + γ60*SHAMEij + γ70*HOPELESSij + γ80*BOREDOMij + γ90*AGEij + γ100*GENDERij + u0j+ rij
MONITORINGij = γ00 + γ01*PRIVATEPUBLICj + γ10*ENJOYMENTij + γ20*HOPEij + γ30*PRIDEij + γ40*ANGERij + γ50*ANXIETYij + γ60*SHAMEij + γ70*HOPELESSij + γ80*BOREDOMij + γ90*AGEij + γ100*GENDERij + u0j+ rij
REGULATINGij = γ00 + γ01*PRIVATEPUBLICj + γ10*ENJOYMENTij + γ20*HOPEij + γ30*PRIDEij + γ40*ANGERij + γ50*ANXIETYij + γ60*SHAMEij + γ70*HOPELESSij + γ80*BOREDOMij + γ90*AGEij + γ100*GENDERij + u0j+ rij