Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T09:02:48.700Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

48 Psychometric properties of DCTclock™ with commonly used neuropsychological tests and their combined ability to predict Beta-Amyloid Positron Emission Tomography Status

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 December 2023

Sheina Emrani*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School, Brown University, Providence RI, USA, Providence, RI, USA
Jennifer Strenger
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School, Brown University, Providence RI, USA, Providence, RI, USA
Zachary Kunicki
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School, Brown University, Providence RI, USA, Providence, RI, USA
Alyssa De Vito
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School, Brown University, Providence RI, USA, Providence, RI, USA
Stephen Salloway
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School, Brown University, Providence RI, USA, Providence, RI, USA
Louisa Thompson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School, Brown University, Providence RI, USA, Providence, RI, USA
*
Correspondence: Sheina Emrani, Dept. of Psychiatry & Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School, Brown University, Providence RI, USA, Sheina_emrani@brown.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.
Objective:

Sensitive and non-invasive methods of screening for early-stage Alzheimer’s disease (AD) are urgently needed. The digital clock drawing test (DCTclockTM) is an established and well-researched neuropsychological tool that can aid in early detection of dementia. Other simple, yet sensitive, neuropsychological measures able to detect early stages of AD include Trail Making Tests (TMT). We investigated the psychometric properties of DCTclockTM with TMT-A and TMT-B. We then sought to understand the degree to which neuropsychological tools (i.e., DCTclockTM, TMT-A, and B) versus the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) predict beta-amyloid (Aß) positron emission tomography (PET) status (positive or negative) in cognitively normal individuals.

Participants and Methods:

Participants included a sample of cognitively normal older adults (n= 59, M age = 69.2, F = 64%) recruited from the Butler Memory and Aging Program. The Linus Health DCTclockTM uses a digital pen to capture traditional clock drawing test performance and advanced analytics to evaluate the drawing process for indicators of cognitive difficulty. DCTclockTM may have overlapping cognitive properties with TMT measures, like efficiency, processing speed, and spatial reasoning. We compared latency measures (i.e., process efficiency, clock face speed, average latency, and processing speed) and spatial reasoning of the DCTclockTM to z-scores of TMT-A and TMT-B to detect any overlapping psychometric properties. Verbal fluency was included for discriminant validity. We then ran logistic regressions on a subset of the sample to compare neuropsychological tests (DCTclockTM total score [score that captures overall performance], TMT-A/B, and verbal fluency) to the MoCA, a commonly used cognitive screening tool, in determining PET status.

Results:

Highly correlated (r > .7) DCTclockTM variables were excluded. We found statistically significant correlations between some DCTclockTM measures and TMT-A/B, like DCTclockTM drawing process efficiency and TMT-A and TMT-B (r= .45, p< .001, r=.29, p< .026, respectively), and DCTclockTM average latency and TMT-A and TMT-B (r=.3, p< .024, r= .26, p< .044, respectively). No statistically significant associations were found between any DCTclockTM measures and verbal fluency, or between DCTclockTM spatial reasoning and TMT-A/B. We then investigated the effect of these neuropsychological tests (DCTclockTM total score, TMT-A/B, verbal fluency) and age on the likelihood of PET positivity (subset of sample, total PET, n=31). The model was statistically significant (x2 (5) = 15.35, p< .01). The model explained 53% (Nagelkerke R2) of the variance in PET status and correctly classified 74.2% of cases. DCTclockTM was the only significant predictor (p< .02), after controlling for TMT-A, TMT-B, verbal fluency, and age. Comparatively, there was no effect of MoCA and age (total PET, n= 29) on the likelihood of PET positivity.

Conclusions:

Overall, these results suggest psychometric convergence on elements of DCTclockTM and TMT-A/B, while there was no association in spatial operations between DCTclockTM and TMT measures. Further, when compared to the MoCA, DCTclockTM and these commonly used neuropsychological tests (verbal fluency and TMT-A/B) were better predictors of PET status, primarily driven by the DCTclockTM. Digitized neuropsychological tools may provide additional metrics not captured by pen-and-paper tests that can detect AD-associated pathology.

Type
Poster Session 03: Dementia | Amnesia | Memory | Language | Executive Functions
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2023