Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T16:22:40.322Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of bilingualism on verbal learning and memory in Hispanic adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2009

Josette G. Harris
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Denver, CO 80262
C. Munro Cullum
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and Neurology, The University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, TX 75235-8898
Antonio E. Puente
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Wilmington, Wilmington, NC 28403

Abstract

The effect of bilingualism on qualitative aspects of verbal learning and memory was investigated. Equivalent list learning tests in English and Spanish were carefully constructed, and compared across two bilingual Hispanic groups of Mexican origin that differed in their level of English proficiency (“balanced” and “nonbalanced” bilinguals) and a group of monolingual English-speaking non-Hispanic subjects. Groups were matched for age, education, and gender composition. Nonbalanced bilinguals assessed in English utilized semantic clustering to the same extent as monolinguals, but learned fewer words overall, and demonstrated lower retention scores compared to monolinguals. Comparisons of groups assessed in their dominant languages, however, revealed no significant differences on any of the learning and memory indices examined. In addition to comparisons with standard clinical memory indices, assessment issues concerning bilingual individuals are addressed. (JINS, 1995, I, 10–16.)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., & Puente, A.E. (1994). Neuropsycho-logical evaluation of the Spanish speaker. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albert, M.L. & Obler, L.K. (1978). The bilingual brain. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. (1976). The psychology of memory. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Battig, W.F. & Montague, W.E. (1969). Category norms for verbal items in 56 categories: A replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms. Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph, 80, 146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birkmeyer, F. (1965). The relationship between the Coloured Progressive Matrices and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Psychology in the Schools, 2, 278280.3.0.CO;2-#>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bousfield, W.A. & Cohen, B.H. (1955). The occurrence of clustering in the recall of randomly arranged words of different frequencies-of-usage. The Journal of General Psychology, 52, 8395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchanan, M.A. (1927). Graded Spanish word book. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Carlson, J.S. & Jensen, C.M. (1981). Reliability of the Raven Coloured Progressive Matrices Test: Age and ethnic group comparisons. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 320322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J.B., Davies, P., & Richman, B. (1971). The American Heritage Word Frequency Book. New York: American Heritage Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Champagnol, R. (1973). Organisation se'mantique et linguis-tique dans le rappel libre bilingue. Anne'e Psychologique, 73, 115134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craik, F.I.M. (1981). Encoding and retrieval effects in human memory: A partial review. In Long, J. & Baddeley, A.D. (Eds.), Attention and performance, IX. (pp. 383402). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Delis, D., Freeland, J., Kramer, J., & Kaplan, E. (1988). Integrating clinical assessment with cognitive neuroscience: Construct validation of the California Verbal Learning Test. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 123130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delis, D., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (1987). The California Verbal Learning Test-Research edition. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Durgunoglu, A.Y. & Roediger, H.L. (1987). Test differences in accessing bilingual memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 377391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eaton, H.S. (1967). An English-French-German-Spanish word frequency dictionary. New York: Dover Publications.Google Scholar
Genesee, F., Hamers, J., Lambert, W.E., Mononen, L., Seitz, M., & Starck, R. (1978). Language processing in bilinguals. Brain and Language, 5, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gernsbacher, M.A. (1984). Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(2), 256281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen, A.R. (1974). How biased are culture-loaded tests? Genetic Psychology Monographs, 90, 185244.Google Scholar
Juilland, A. & Chang-Rodriguez, E. (1964). Frequency dictionary of Spanish words. The Hague: Mouton & Co.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatzky, R.L. (1980). Human memory: Structures and processes. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman and Company.Google Scholar
Kolers, P.A. (1963). Interlingual word associations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 291300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, J.H., Delis, D.C., & Daniel, M. (1988). Sex differences in verbal learning. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(6), 907915.3.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambert, W.E., Ignatow, M., & Krauthamer, M. (1968). Bilingual organization in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, 207214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, G. (1967). Organization and memory. In Spence, K.W. & Spence, J.A. (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 1, pp. 327372). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manuel-DuPont, S., Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., & Puente, A.E. (1992). Bilingualism. In Puente, A.E. & McCaffrey, R.J. (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychological assessment: A bio-psychosocial perspective, (pp. 193–210). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Nott, C.R. & Lambert, W.E. (1965). Free recall of bilinguals. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, 10651071.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paivio, A., Clark, J.M., & Lambert, W.E. (1988). Bilingual dual-coding theory and semantic repetition effects on recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(1), 163172.Google Scholar
Palmer, M.B. (1972). Effects of categorization, degree of bilingualism, and language upon recall of select monolinguals and bilinguals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 160164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, M. (1977). Bilingualism and aphasia. In Whitaker, H. & Whitaker, H. (Eds.), Studies in neurolinguistics (Vol. 3, pp. 65121). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Population Reference Bureau. (1991). World Population Data Sheet (1986–1990). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Raven, J.C. (1956). The Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: H.K. Lewis & Co.Google Scholar
Raven, J.C., Court, J.H., & Raven, J. (1986). A manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary tests (Research Supplement No. 3). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P.J. & Rey, M. (1986). The relationship between category typicality and concept familiarity: Evidence from Spanish- and English-speaking monolinguals. Memory and Cognition, 14(2), 150163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, M.F., Marin, O.S.M., & Saffran, E.M. (1979). Dissociations of language function in dementia: A case study. Brain and Language, 7, 277306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. (1968). The Symbol Digit Modalities Test: A neuro-psychologic test for economic screening of learning and other cerebral disorders. Learning Disorders, 3, 8391.Google Scholar
Smith, A. (1973). Symbol Digit Modalities Test Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E.L. & Lorge, I. (1944). The teacher's word book of 30,000 words. New York: Teachers College.Google Scholar
United States Bureau of the Census (1986). Projections of the Hispanic population: 1983–2080. (U.S. Dept. of Commerce, Population Estimates and Projections, Series P-25, No. 995). Washington DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.Google Scholar
Valencia, R.R. (1979). Comparison of intellectual performance of Chicano and Anglo third-grade boys on the Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices. Psychology in the Schools, 16, 448453.3.0.CO;2-Z>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valencia, R.R. (1984). Reliability of the Raven Coloured Progressive Matrices for Anglo and for Mexican-American children. Psychology in the Schools, 21, 4952.3.0.CO;2-H>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1967). Escala de Inteligencia Wechsler para Adul-tos. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R.W. (1980). Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery, English Form. Hingham, MA: Teaching Resources Corp.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R.W. (1981). Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery, Spanish Form. Hingham, MA: Teaching Resources Corp.Google Scholar