Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T19:24:30.053Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neurocognitive Functions in 3- to 15-Year-Old Children: An International Comparison

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 February 2017

Johanna Rosenqvist*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Finland Department of Phoniatrics, Helsinki University Hospital, Finland
Pekka Lahti-Nuuttila
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Finland Department of Phoniatrics, Helsinki University Hospital, Finland
Cosimo Urgesi
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Languages and Literatures, Communication, Education and Society, University of Udine and Scientific Institute (IRCCS) Eugenio Medea, Italy
James Holdnack
Affiliation:
Pearson, San Antonio, Texas
Sally L. Kemp
Affiliation:
School of Health Professions, University of Missouri - Columbia, Columbia, Missouri
Marja Laasonen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Finland Department of Phoniatrics, Helsinki University Hospital, Finland Department of Psychology and Speech-Language Pathology, University of Turku, Finland
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Johanna Rosenqvist, PO 9, 00014 University of Helsinki, Finland. E-mail: johanna.rosenqvist@helsinki.fi

Abstract

Objectives: Performance on neurocognitive tasks develops with age, but it is still unknown whether this performance differs between children from different cultures. We compared cross-sectionally the development of neurocognitive functions in 3- to 15-year-old children from three countries: Finland, Italy, and the United States (N=2745). Methods: Language, face memory, emotion recognition, theory of mind, and visuospatial processing subtests from the NEPSY-II standardizations in Finland, Italy, and the United States were used to evaluate if children and adolescents from different linguistic and cultural backgrounds differ in performance on these measures. Results: We found significant differences in performance on the tasks between the countries. Generally, the differences were more pronounced in the younger age groups. Some subtests showed greater country effects than others, performance on these subtests being higher, in general, in one country over the others, or showed different patterns of age associated changes in test performance. Conclusions: Significant differences in neurocognitive performance between children from Finland, Italy, and the United States were found. These findings may be due to cultural or educational differences that impact test performance, or due to factors associated with the adaptation of measures from one culture to another. The finding of performance differences across countries on similar tasks indicate that cross-cultural and background variables impact performance on neuropsychological measures. Therefore, clinicians need to consider a child’s cultural background when evaluating performance on neuropsychological assessments. The results also indicate that future cross-cultural studies are needed to further examine the underlying cultural factors that influence neurocognitive performance. (JINS, 2017, 23, 367–380)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Agranovich, A.V., Panter, A.T., Puente, A.E., & Touradji, P. (2011). The culture of time in neuropsychological assessment: Exploring the effects of culture-specific time attitudes on timed test performance in Russian and American samples. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(04), 692701. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000592 Google Scholar
Ardila, A. (2005). Cultural values underlying psychometric cognitive testing. Neuropsychology Review, 15(4), 185195. doi: 10.1007/s11065-005-9180-y Google Scholar
Ardila, A., & Rosselli, M. (1994). Development of language, memory, and visuospatial abilities in 5‐ to 12‐year‐old children using a neuropsychological battery. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10(2), 97120. doi: 10.1080/87565649409540571 Google Scholar
Bentin, S., Hammer, R., & Cahan, S. (1991). The effects of aging and first grade schooling on the development of phonological awareness. Psychological Science, 2(4), 271274. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00148.x Google Scholar
Boone, K.B., Victor, T.L., Wen, J., Razani, J., & Pontón, M. (2007). The association between neuropsychological scores and ethnicity, language, and acculturation variables in a large patient population. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(3), 355365. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.010 Google Scholar
Brickman, A.M., Cabo, R., & Manly, J.J. (2006). Ethical issues in cross-cultural neuropsychology. Applied Neuropsychology, 13(2), 91100. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1302_4 Google Scholar
Burger, K. (2010). How does early childhood care and education affect cognitive development? An international review of the effects of early interventions for children from different social backgrounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(2), 140165. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.11.001 Google Scholar
Byrd, D., Arentoft, A., Scheiner, D., Westerveld, M., & Baron, I.S. (2008). State of multicultural neuropsychological assessment in children: Current research issues. Neuropsychology Review, 18(3), 214222. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9065-y Google Scholar
Callaghan, T., Rochat, P., Lillard, A., Claux, M.L., Odden, H., Itakura, S., & Singh, S. (2005). Synchrony in the onset of mental-state reasoning: Evidence from five cultures. Psychological Science, 16(5), 378384. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01544.x Google Scholar
Cheie, L., Veraksa, A., Zinchenko, Y., Gorovaya, A., & Visu-Petra, L. (2015). A cross-cultural investigation of inhibitory control, generative fluency, and anxiety symptoms in Romanian and Russian preschoolers. Child Neuropsychology, 21(2), 121149. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2013.879111 Google Scholar
Dalen, K., Jellestad, F., & Kamaloodien, K. (2007). The translation of the NEPSY-II to Afrikaans, some ethical reflections. Cognitie, Creier, Comportament/Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 11(3), 609. Retrieved from http://web.b.ebscohost.com/ehost/pdfviewer/pdfviewer?sid=a2da3f10-0967-4a21-9624-3429302f409b%40sessionmgr105&vid=1&hid=118 Google Scholar
De Heering, A., De Liedekerke, C., Deboni, M., & Rossion, B. (2010). The role of experience during childhood in shaping the other‐race effect. Developmental Science, 13(1), 181187. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00876.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devine, R.T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 85(5), 17771794. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12237 Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H.A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 203235. doi: 10.1037/0033–2909.128.2.203 Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H.A., & Ambady, N. (2003). Universals and cultural differences in recognizing emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(5), 159164. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01252 Google Scholar
Fernández, A.L., & Marcopulos, B.A. (2008). A comparison of normative data for the trail making test from several countries: Equivalence of norms and considerations for interpretation. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49(3), 239246. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00637.x Google Scholar
Garratt, L.C., & Kelly, T.P. (2008). To what extent does bilingualism affect children’s performance on the NEPSY? Child Neuropsychology, 14(1), 7181. doi: 10.1080/09297040701218405 Google Scholar
Gavrilov, Y., Rotem, S., Ofek, R., & Geva, R. (2012). Socio-cultural effects on children’s initiation of joint attention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(286), 110. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00286 Google Scholar
Grissmer, D., Grimm, K.J., Aiyer, S.M., Murrah, W.M., & Steele, J.S. (2010). Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: Two new school readiness indicators. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 10081017. doi: 10.1037/a0020104 Google Scholar
Karlsson, L.C., Soveri, A., Räsänen, P., Kärnä, A., Delatte, S., Lagerström, E., & Laine, M. (2015). Bilingualism and performance on two widely used developmental neuropsychological test batteries. PLoS One, 10(4), e0125867. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0125867 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korkman, M., Barron-Linnankoski, S., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (1999). Effects of age and duration of reading instruction on the development of phonological awareness, rapid naming, and verbal memory span. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16(3), 415431. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1603_24 Google Scholar
Korkman, M., Kemp, S.L., & Kirk, U. (2001). Effects of age on neurocognitive measures of children ages 5 to 12: A cross-sectional study on 800 children from the United States. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 331354. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2001_2 Google Scholar
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S.L. (1997). NEPSY. Lasten neuropsykologinen tutkimus. [NEPSY. A child neuropsychological assessment]. Helsinki, Finland: Psykologien kustannus.Google Scholar
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S.L. (1998). NEPSY—A developmental neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S.L. (2007a). NEPSY II. Second edition. Administration manual. San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp / Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S.L. (2007b). NEPSY II. Second edition. Clinical and interpretive manual. San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp / Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S.L. (2008a). Nepsy-II. Käsikirja I. Testin esitys- ja pisteytysohjeet. [Nepsy-II. Manual I. Instructions for assessment administration and scoring]. Helsinki: Psykologien kustannus.Google Scholar
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S.L. (2008b). Nepsy-II. Käsikirja II. Kehittely, käyttö ja psykometriset tiedot. [NEPSY-II. Manual II. Development, application, and psychometric information]. Helsinki: Psykologien kustannus.Google Scholar
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S.L. (2011). NEPSY-II. Second edition. Contributo alla taratura italiana (Italian edition, C. Urgesi & F. Fabbro [Eds.]). Firenze, Italia: Giunti O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali.Google Scholar
Korkman, M., Lahti-Nuuttila, P., Laasonen, M., Kemp, S.L., & Holdnack, J. (2013). Neurocognitive development in 5- to 16-year-old North American children: A cross-sectional study. Child Neuropsychology, 19(5), 516539. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2012.705822 Google Scholar
Korkman, M., Stenroos, M., Mickos, A., Westman, M., Ekholm, P., & Byring, R. (2012). Does simultaneous bilingualism aggravate children’s specific language problems? Acta Paediatrica, 101(9), 946952. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2012.02733.x Google Scholar
Landerl, K., Ramus, F., Moll, K., Lyytinen, H., Leppänen, P.H.T., Lohvansuu, K., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2012). Predictors of developmental dyslexia in European orthographies with varying complexity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 54(6), 686694. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12029 Google Scholar
Manly, J.J. (2008). Critical issues in cultural neuropsychology: Profit from diversity. Neuropsychology Review, 18(3), 179183. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9068-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manly, J.J., Jacobs, D.M., Sano, M., Bell, K., Merchant, C.A., Small, S.A., & Stern, Y. (1998). Cognitive test performance among nondemented elderly African Americans and whites. Neurology, 50(5), 12381245. doi: 10.1212/WNL.50.5.1238 Google Scholar
McLoyd, V.C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. The American Psychologist, 53(2), 185204. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.185 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moll, K., Ramus, F., Bartling, J., Bruder, J., Kunze, S., Neuhoff, N., & Landerl, K. (2014). Cognitive mechanisms underlying reading and spelling development in five European orthographies. Learning & Instruction, 29, 6577. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.09.003 Google Scholar
Morrison, F.J., Smith, L., & Dow-Ehrensberger, M. (1995). Education and cognitive development: A natural experiment. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 789799. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.789 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulenga, K., Ahonen, T., & Aro, M. (2001). Performance of Zambian children on the NEPSY: A pilot study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 375383. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2001_4 Google Scholar
Oh, S., & Lewis, C. (2008). Korean preschoolers’ advanced inhibitory control and its relation to other executive skills and mental state understanding. Child Development, 79(1), 8099. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01112.x Google Scholar
Ojeda, N., Aretouli, E., Peña, J., & Schretlen, D.J. (2016). Age differences in cognitive performance: A study of cultural differences in historical context. Journal of Neuropsychology, 10, 104115. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12059 Google Scholar
Olson, K., & Jacobson, K. (2015). Cross-cultural considerations in pediatric neuropsychology: A review and call to attention. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 4(3), 166177. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2013.830258 Google Scholar
Restrepo, M.A., Schwanenflugel, P.J., Blake, J., Neuharth-Pritchett, S., Cramer, S.E., & Ruston, H.P. (2006). Performance on the PPVT–III and the EVT: Applicability of the measures with African American and European American preschool children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 37(1), 1727. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2006/003) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosselli, M., & Ardila, A. (2003). The impact of culture and education on non-verbal neuropsychological measurements: A critical review. Brain and Cognition, 52(3), 326333. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00170-2 Google Scholar
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Bateman, J.R., & Guzmán, M. (2001). Neuropsychological test scores, academic performance, and developmental disorders in Spanish-speaking children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 355373. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2001_3 Google Scholar
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Navarrete, M.G., & Matute, E. (2010). Performance of Spanish/English bilingual children on a Spanish-language neuropsychological battery: Preliminary normative data. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 25(3), 218235. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acq012 Google Scholar
Sabbagh, M.A., Xu, F., Carlson, S.M., Moses, L.J., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17(1), 7481. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01667.x Google Scholar
Shahaeian, A., Henry, J.D., Razmjoee, M., Teymoori, A., & Wang, C. (2015). Towards a better understanding of the relationship between executive control and theory of mind: An intra-cultural comparison of three diverse samples. Developmental Science, 18(5), 671685. doi: 10.1111/desc.12243 Google Scholar
Shahaeian, A., Nielsen, M., Peterson, C.C., & Slaughter, V. (2014). Cultural and family influences on children’s theory of mind development a comparison of Australian and Iranian school-age children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45(4), 555568. doi: 10.1177/0022022113513921 Google Scholar
Shahaeian, A., Peterson, C.C., Slaughter, V., & Wellman, H.M. (2011). Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 12391247. doi: 10.1037/a0023899 Google Scholar
Shuttleworth-Edwards, A.B., Kemp, R.D., Rust, A.L., Muirhead, J.G., Hartman, N.P., & Radloff, S.E. (2004). Cross-cultural effects on IQ test performance: A review and preliminary normative indications on WAIS-III test performance. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26(7), 903920. doi: 10.1080/13803390490510824 Google Scholar
Urgesi, C., Campanella, F., & Fabbro, F. (2011). NEPSY–II: Second Edition. Contributo alla taratura italiana [NEPSY–II: Second edition. Italian Standardization]. Firenze: Giunti O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali.Google Scholar
Veliu, B., & Leathem, J. (2016). Neuropsychological assessment of refugees: Methodological and cross-cultural barriers. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2016.1201483 Google Scholar
Waber, D.P., De Moor, C., Forbes, P.W., Almli, C.R., Botteron, K.N., Leonard, G., & Rumsey, J. (2007). The NIH MRI study of normal brain development: Performance of a population based sample of healthy children aged 6 to 18 years on a neuropsychological battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(05), 729746. doi: 10.10170S1355617707070841 Google Scholar
Westman, M., Korkman, M., Mickos, A., & Byring, R. (2008). Language profiles of monolingual and bilingual Finnish preschool children at risk for language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43(6), 699711. doi: 10.1080/13682820701839200 Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Rosenqvist supplementary material

Table

Download Rosenqvist supplementary material(File)
File 55.1 KB