Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T16:35:49.718Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A systematic view of human memory processes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2009

John D. E. Gabrieli
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Dialogue Section
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Blaxton, T.A. (1989). Investigating dissociations among memory measures: Support for a transfer-appropriate processing framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 657668.Google Scholar
Blaxton, T.A. (1992). Dissociations among memory measures in memory-impaired subjects: Evidence for a processing account of memory. Memory and Cognition, 20, 549562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blaxton, T.A., Gabrieli, J.D.E., Park, S., Figlozzi, C.., DeCarli, C. & Theodore, W.H. (1994). A PET activation study of implicit priming in picture naming. Proceedings of Cognitive Neuroscience Society Inaugural Meeting, 1, 43.Google Scholar
Carroll, M., Byrne, B., & Kirsner, K. (1985). Autobiographical memory and perceptual learning: A developmental study using picture recognition, naming latency, and perceptual identification. Memory and Cognition, 13, 273279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cave, C.B. & Squire, L.R. (1992). Intact and long-lasting repetition priming in amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 509520.Google ScholarPubMed
Cermak, L.S., Bleich, R.P., & Blackford, S.P. (1988). Deficits in the implicit retention of new associations by alcoholic Kor-sakoff patients. Brain and Language, 7, 312323.Google ScholarPubMed
Cermak, L.S., Verfaellie, M., Letourneau, L., & Jacoby, L.L. (1993). Episodic effects of picture identification for alcoholic Korsakoff patients. Brain and Cognition, 22, 8597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, N.J. & Squire, L.R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corkin, S. (1968). Acquisition of motor skills after bilateral medial temporal-lobe excision. Neuropsychologia, 6, 255265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daum, I., Channon, S., & Canavan, A.G.M. (1989). Classical conditioning in patients with severe memory problems. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 52, 4751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fleischman, et al. (1995). Neuropsychology (in press).Google Scholar
Gabrieli, J.D.E. (1991). Differential effects of aging and age-related neurological diseases on memory subsystems of the human brain. In Boller, F. & Grafman, J. (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North Holland.Google Scholar
Gabrieli, J.D.E., Cohen, N.J., & Corkin, S. (1988). The impaired learning of semantic knowledge following bilateral medial temporal-lobe resection. Brain and Cognition, 7, 157177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabrieli, J.D.E., Corkin, S., Mickel, S.F., & Growdon, J.H. (1993). Intact acquisition and long-term retention of mirror-tracing skill in Alzheimer’s disease and in global amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 899910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabrieli, J.D.E., Fleischman, D.A., Keane, M.M., Reminger, S.L., & Morrell, F. (in press). Double dissociation between memory systems underlying explicit and implicit memory in the human brain. Psychological Science.Google Scholar
Gabrieli, J.D.E., Keane, M.M., Stanger, B.Z., Kjelgaard, M.M., Corkin, S., & Growdon, J.H. (1994). Dissociations among structural-perceptual, lexical-semantic, and even-fact memory systems in amnesic, Alzheimer’s, and normal subjects. Cortex, 30, 75103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabrieli, J.D.E., Milberg, W., Keane, M.M., & Corkin, S. (1990). Intact priming of patterns despite impaired memory. Neuropsychologia, 28, 417427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graf, P., Mandler, G., & Haden, P.E. (1982). Simulating amnesic symptoms in normal subjects. Science, 218,12431244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graf, P. & Schacter, D.L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 501518.Google ScholarPubMed
Graf, P., Shimamura, A.P., & Squire, L.R. (1985). Priming across modalities and priming across category levels: Extending the domain of preserved function in amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 77 386396.Google Scholar
Graf, P., Squire, L.R., & Mandler, G. (1984). The information that amnesic patients do not forget. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 164178.Google Scholar
Heindel, W.C., Butters, N., & Salmon, D.P. (1988). Impaired learning of a motor skill in patients with Huntington's disease. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102, 141147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heindel, W.C., Salmon, D.P., Shults, C.W., Walicke, P.A., & Butters, N. (1989). Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: A comparison of Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's disease patients. The Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 582587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacoby, L.L. (1983). Remembering the data: Analyzing interactive processes in reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 485508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacoby, L.L. & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jetter, W., Poser, U., Freeman, R.B.J., & Markowitsch, H.J. (1986). A verbal long term memory deficit in frontal lobe damaged patients. Cortex, 22, 229242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keane, M.M., Gabrieli, J.D.E., Fennema, A.C., Growdon, J.H., & Corkin, S. (1991). Evidence for a dissociation between perceptual and conceptual priming in Alzheimer's disease. Behavioral Neuroscience, 105, 326342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keane, M.M., Gabrieli, J.D.E., Monti, L.A., Cantor, J.M., & Noland, J.S. (1993). Amnesic patients show normal priming and a normal depth-of-processing effect in a conceptually driven implicit memory task. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 19, 1079.Google Scholar
McGlinchey-Berroth, R., Carrillo, M.C., Armfield, S., Saver, J.L., Gabrieli, J.D.E., Cermak, L.S., & Disterhoft, J.F. (in press). Intact delay eyeblink conditioning in medial temporal amnesia, but not in alcoholic Korsakoff's amnesia. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 20, 430.Google Scholar
Milner, B. (1962). Les troubles de la memoire accompagnant des lesions hippocampiques bilaterales. Psychologie de l’hippo-campe. Paris: Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique.Google Scholar
Milner, B., Corkin, S., & Teuber, H.L. (1968). Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesia syndrome: 14 year follow-up study of H. M. Neuropsychologia, 6, 215234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, D.C., Bransford, J.D., & Franks, J.J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 519533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Musen, G. & Squire, L.R. (1992). Nonverbal priming in amnesia. Memory and Cognition, 20, 441448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roediger, H.L. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remembering. American Psychologist, 45(9), 10431056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roediger, H.L. & Blaxton, T.A. (1987). Retrieval modes produce dissociations in memory for surface information. In Gorfein, D.S. & Hoffman, R.R. (Eds.), Memory and Cognitive Processes: The Ebbinghaus Centennial Conference (pp. 349379). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Roediger, H.L., Weldon, M.S., & Challis, B.H. (1989). Explain-ing dissociations between implicit and explicit measures of retention: A processing account. In Roediger, H.L. & Craik, F.I.M. (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour ofEndel Tulving (pp. 314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Roediger, H.L., Weldon, M.S., Stadler, M.L., & Riegler, G.L. (1992). Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: Word fragment and word stem completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 12511269.Google ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D.L. (1990). Perceptual representation systems and implicit memory: Toward a resolution of the multiple memory systems debate. In Diamond, A. (Ed.), Development and Neural Bases of Higher Cognitive Function, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences (Vol. 608, pp. 543571).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schacter, D.L., Cooper, L.A., Tharan, M., & Rubens, A.B. (1991). Preserved priming of novel objects in patients with memory disorders. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 117130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D.L. & Graf, P. (1986). Preserved learning in amnesic patients: Perspectives from research on direct priming. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8, 727743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherry, D.F. & Schacter, D.L. (1987). The evolution of multiple memory systems. Psychological Review, 94, 439454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimamura, A.P. (1986). Priming effects in amnesia: Evidence for a dissociable memory function. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 619644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shimamura, A.P. & Squire, L.R. (1984). Paired-associate learning and priming effects in amnesia: A neuropsychological study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 556570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shimamura, A.P. & Squire, L.R. (1989). Impaired priming of new associations in amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 721728.Google ScholarPubMed
Squire, L.R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L.R., Shimamura, A.P., & Graf, P. (1987). Strength and duration of priming effects in normal subjects and amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 25, 195210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Srinivas, K. & Roediger, H.L. (1990). Classifying implicit memory tests: Category association and anagram solution. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 389412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there? American Psychologist, 40, 385398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E. & Schacter, D.L. (1990). Priming and human memory systems. Science, 247, 301306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaidya, C.J., Demb, J.B., Keane, M.M., Monti, L.A., Cohen, N.J., Poldrack, R.A. & Gabrieli, J.D.E. (1994). The core deficit in amnesia is neither one of conceptual processing nor one of explicit retrieval. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 20, 1289.Google Scholar
Warrington, E.K. & Weiskrantz, L. (1968). A new method of testing long-term retention with special reference to amnesic patients. Nature, 217, 972974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E.K. & Weiskrantz, L. (1970). The amnesic syndrome: Consolidation or retrieval? Nature, 228, 628630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskrantz, L. & Warrington, E.K. (1979). Conditioning in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 17, 187194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed