Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T10:08:09.392Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Assessing the Relationship between Semantic Processing and Thought Disorder Symptoms in Schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 August 2015

Eric Josiah Tan*
Affiliation:
Monash Alfred Psychiatry Research Center, Monash University Central Clinical School, and The Alfred Hospital, Melbourne, VIC, Australia Brain and Psychological Sciences Research Center, Swinburne University of Technology, Hawthorn, VIC, Australia
Erica Neill
Affiliation:
Monash Alfred Psychiatry Research Center, Monash University Central Clinical School, and The Alfred Hospital, Melbourne, VIC, Australia Brain and Psychological Sciences Research Center, Swinburne University of Technology, Hawthorn, VIC, Australia
Susan Lee Rossell
Affiliation:
Monash Alfred Psychiatry Research Center, Monash University Central Clinical School, and The Alfred Hospital, Melbourne, VIC, Australia Brain and Psychological Sciences Research Center, Swinburne University of Technology, Hawthorn, VIC, Australia
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Eric Josiah Tan, Monash Alfred Psychiatry Research Center, Level 4 607 St Kilda Road, Melbourne, VIC 3004 Australia E-mail: eric.tan@monash.edu

Abstract

Aberrant semantic processing has been linked to the etiology of formal thought disorder (TD) symptoms in schizophrenia. In this cross-sectional study, two prominent theories, overactivation and disorganized structure of semantic memory (SM), were examined in relation to TD symptoms using the continuum approach across two established semantic tasks (direct/indirect semantic priming and categorical fluency). The aim was to examine the validity of the two TD theories in relation to TD symptoms in schizophrenia. Greater direct and indirect priming, fluency productivity and category errors were expected if the data supported the overactivation theory. Reduced fluency productivity and increased category errors would be characteristic of disorganized storage. Fifty-seven schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder patients and 48 controls completed a clinical assessment and the semantic tasks. There was significantly reduced direct priming in patients compared to controls (p<.05), while indirect priming was not significantly different; there was no association between TD and degree of priming. Patients produced more category-inappropriate words (p<.005) than controls, which was related to increasing severity of circumstantiality. The pattern of results was more indicative of a disorganized SM storage problem in this sample. This phenomenon may underlie some TD symptoms in general schizophrenia. The findings strengthen the relationship between SM deficits and TD symptoms, though this appears to differ between individual symptoms. The authors discuss the value of the continuum approach in addressing research questions in TD etiology. Given low levels of TD in this study, replication of these findings in a sample with greater TD is desirable. (JINS, 2015, 21, 629–638)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andreasen, N.C. (1979). Thought, language, and communication disorders II. Diagnostic significance. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36(12), 13251330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, N.C. (1986). Scale for the assessment of thought, language, and communication (TLC). Schizophrenia Bulletin, 12(3), 473482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D.M., Servan-Schreiber, D., Steingard, S., Cohen, J.D., Steinhauer, S.S., & Van Kammen, D.P. (1996). Semantic priming in schizophrenia: An examination of spreading activation using word pronunciation and multiple SOAs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105(4), 592601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrera, A., McKenna, P.J., & Berrios, G.E. (2005). Formal thought disorder in schizophrenia: An executive or a semantic deficit? Psychological Medicine, 35(1), 121132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrera, A., McKenna, P.J., & Berrios, G.E. (2009). Formal thought disorder, neuropsychology and insight in schizophrenia. Psychopathology, 42(4), 264269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, A.L., & Hamsher, K.d.S. (1976). Multilingual aphasia examination. Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa Press.Google Scholar
Besche-Richard, C., & Passerieux, C. (2003). Semantic context-processing deficit in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients: Evidence from new semantic priming paradigms. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 8(3), 173189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bleuler, E. (1950). Dementia praecox of the group of schizophrenias. J. Zinkin (Trans.). New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Blum, N.A., & Freides, D. (1995). Investigating thought disorder in schizophrenia with the lexical decision task. Schizophrenia Research, 16(3), 217224. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0920-9964(94)00078-MCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bokat, C.E., & Goldberg, T.E. (2003). Letter and category fluency in schizophrenic patients: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 64(1), 7378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brébion, G., Stephan-Otto, C., Huerta-Ramos, E., Usall, J., Ochoa, S., Roca, M., & Haro, J.-M. (2013). Abnormal functioning of the semantic network in schizophrenia patients with thought disorganization. An exemplar production task. Psychiatry Research, 205(1-2), 16. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.08.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, L.J., Chapman, J.P., Curran, T.E., & Miller, M.B. (1994). Do children and the eldery show heightened semantic priming? How to answer the question. Developmental Review, 14(2), 159185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, R.Y.L., Chen, E.Y.H., Chan, C.K.Y., Lam, L.C.W., & Lieh-Mak, F. (2000). Verbal fluency in schizophrenia: Reduction in semantic store. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 34(1), 4348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chenery, H.J., Copland, D.A., McGrath, J., & Savage, G. (2004). Maintaining and updating semantic context in schizophrenia: An investigation of the effects of multiple remote primes. Psychiatry Research, 126(3), 241252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coccia, M., Bartolini, M., Luzzi, S., Provinciali, L., & Ralph, M.A. (2004). Semantic memory is an amodal, dynamic system: Evidence from the interaction of naming and object use in semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21(5), 513527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, A.M., & Loftus, E.F. (1975). A spreading activation of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Condray, R., Siegle, G.J., Keshavan, M.S., & Steinhauer, S.R (2010). Effects of word frequency on semantic memory in schizophrenia: Electrophysiological evidence for a deficit in linguistic access. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 75(2), 141156. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.10.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeFreitas, C.M., Dunaway, L.A., & Torres, I.J. (2009). Preferential semantic fluency impairment is related to hallucinations, but not formal thought disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 107(2-3), 307312. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2008.08.020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doughty, O.J., & Done, D.J. (2009). Is semantic memory impaired in schizophrenia? A systematic review and meta-analysis of 91 studies. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 14(6), 473509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dwyer, K., David, A., McCarthy, R., McKenna, P., & Peters, E. (2014). Higher-order semantic processing in formal thought disorder in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 216(2), 168176. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.02.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giffard, B., Desgranges, B., Nore‐Mary, F., Lalevée, C., Beaunieux, H., de la Sayette, V., & Eustache, F. (2002). The dynamic time course of semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer’s disease: Clues from hyperpriming and hypopriming effects. Brain, 125(9), 20442057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gladsjo, J.A., Schuman, C.C., Evans, J.D., Peavy, G.M., Miller, S.W., & Heaton, R.K. (1999). Norms for letter and category fluency: Demographic corrections for age, education and ethnicity. Assessment, 6(2), 147178. doi:10.1177/107319119900600204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, T.E., Aloia, M.S., Gourovitch, M.L., Missar, D., Pickar, D., & Weinberger, D.R. (1998). Cognitive substrates of thought disorder, I: The semantic system. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 155(12), 16711676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gouzoulis-Mayfrank, E., Voss, T., Morth, D., Thelen, B., Spitzer, M., & Meincke, U. (2003). Semantic hyperpriming in thought-disordered patients with schizophrenia: State or trait?—A longitudinal investigation. Schizophrenia Research, 65, 6573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henik, A., Nissimov, E., Priel, B., & Umansky, R. (1995). Effects of cognitive load on semantic priming in patients with schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(4), 576584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henik, A., Priel, B., & Umansky, R. (1992). Attention and automaticity in semantic processing of schizophrenic patients. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 5(3), 161169.Google Scholar
Hogben, G.L., & Jacobs, I.A. (1972). Two verbal aspects of language and thought in schizophrenia. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 38(2), 296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, H., Federspiel, A., Wirth, M., Muller, T.J., Wiest, R., Wang, J.J., & Strik, W. (2009). Structural and metabolic changes in language areas linked to formal thought disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 194(2), 130138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jamadar, S., O’Neil, K.M., Pearlson, G.D., Ansari, M., Gill, A., Jagannathan, K., & Assaf, M. (2013). Impairment in semantic retrieval is associated with symptoms in schizophrenia but not bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 73(6), 555564. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.07.027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, S.R., Fiszbein, A., & Opler, L.A. (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13(2), 261276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiefer, M., Martens, U., Weisbrod, M., Hermle, L., & Spitzer, M. (2009). Increased unconscious semantic activation in schizophrenia patients with formal thought disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 114(1-3), 7983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiss, G.R., Armstrong, C., Milroy, R., & Piper, J. (1973). An associative thesaurus of English and its computer analysis. In J.A. Aitkin, R. W. Bailey & N. Hamilton-Smith (Eds.), The computer and literary studies. Edinburgh, UK: University Press.Google Scholar
Kleiger, J.H. (1999). Disordered thinking and the Rorschach: Theory, research and differential diagnosis. Mahwah, NJ: Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Kuperberg, G.R. (2010). Language in schizophrenia Part 1: An introduction. Language and Linguistics Compass, 4(8), 576589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laver, G.D., & Burke, D.M. (1993). Why do semantic priming effects increase in old age? A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 8(1), 3443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laws, K.R., Al-Uzri, M., & Mortimer, A. (2000). Lexical knowledge degradation in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 45(1-2), 123131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laws, K.R., Mortimer, A.M., & Al‐Uzri, M.M. (2004). An early marker for semantic memory impairment in patients with schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 9(4), 267279. doi:10.1080/13546800344000255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leeson, V.C., Laws, K.R., & McKenna, P.J. (2006). Formal thought disorder is characterised by impaired lexical access. Schizophrenia Research, 88(1-3), 161168. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.07.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leeson, V.C., McKenna, P.J., Murray, G., Kondel, T.K., & Laws, K.R. (2005). What happens to semantic memory when formal thought disorder remits? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10(1), 5771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liddle, P.F., & Morris, D.L. (1991). Schizophrenic syndromes and frontal lobe performance. British Journal of Psychiatry, 158(3), 340345. doi:10.1192/bjp.158.3.340CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maher, B.A. (1983). A tentative theory of schizophrenic utterance. In B.A. Maher & W.B. Maher (Eds.), Progress in experimental personality research, vol 12: Personality. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Maher, B.A., Manschreck, T.C., Redmond, D., & Beaudette, S. (1996). Length of illness and the gradient from positive to negative semantic priming in schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research, 22(2), 127132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, S. (2006). Semantic space model demo. Retrieved from http://www.iccs.inf.ed.ac.uk/~smcdonal/semantic_space_model.html (accessed April 2009).Google Scholar
McKenna, P.J., & Oh, T.M. (2005). Schizophrenic speech: Making sense of bathroots and ponds that fall in doorways. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Minzenberg, M.J., Ober, B.A., & Vinogradov, S. (2002). Semantic priming in schizophrenia: A review and synthesis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(5), 699720. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617702801357CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minzenberg, M.J., Poole, J.H., Vinogradov, S., Shenaut, G.K., & Ober, B.A. (2003). Slowed lexical access is uniquely associated with positive and disorganised symptoms of schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 8(2), 107127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S., Mersmann, K., Kloss, M., Jacobsen, D., Wikle, U., Andresen, B., & Pawlik, K. (2001). “Hyper-priming” in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients. Psychological Medicine, 31(2), 221229. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291701003105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moritz, S., Woodward, T.S., Küppers, D., Lausen, A., & Schickel, M. (2003). Increased automatic spreading of activation in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research, 59(2-3), 181186. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00337-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neill, E., Gurvich, C., & Rossell, S.L. (2013). Category fluency in schizophrenia research: Is it an executive or semantic measure? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 19(1), 8195. doi:10.1080/13546805.2013.807233CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neill, E., & Rossell, S.L. (2013). Comparing implicit and explicit semantic access of direct and indirect word pairs in schizophrenia to evaluate models of semantic memory. Psychiatry Research, 205(3), 199204. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.11.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ober, B.A., Vinogradov, S., & Shenaut, G.K. (1997). Automatic versus controlled semantic priming in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 11(4), 506513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Passerieux, C., Hardy-Bayle, M.-C., & Widlocher, D. (1995). Semantic inhibition in schizophrenic patients assessed by a lexical decision task. European Psychiatry, 10(1), 3643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulsen, J.S., Romero, R., Chan, A., Davis, A.V., Heaton, R.K., & Jeste, D.V. (1996). Impairment of the semantic network in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 63(2-3), 109121. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0165-1781%2896%2902901-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pomarol-Clotet, E., Oh, T., Laws, K.R., & McKenna, P. (2008). Semantic priming in schizophrenia: Systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 192(2), 9297. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.106.032102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rastle, K., Harrington, J., & Coltheart, M. (2002). 358,534 nonwords: The ARC nonword database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(4), 13391362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rossell, S.L., & David, A.S. (2006). Are semantic deficits in schizophrenia due to problems with access or storage? [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Schizophrenia Research, 82(2-3), 121134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rossell, S.L., Rabe-Hesketh, S., Shapleske, J., & David, A.S. (1999). Is semantic fluency differentially impaired in schizophrenic patients with delusions? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21(5), 629642. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/jcen.21.5.629.865CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rossell, S.L., Shapleske, J., & David, A.S. (2000). Direct and indirect semantic priming with neutral and emotional words in schizophrenia: Relationship to delusions. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 5(4), 271292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossell, S.L., & Stefanovic, A. (2007). Semantic priming effects in schizophrenia. Current Psychiatry Reviews, 3(2), 137145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Safadi, Z., Lichtenstein-Vidne, L., Dobrusin, M., & Henik, A (2013). Investigating Thought Disorder in Schizophrenia: Evidence for Pathological Activation. PLoS One, 8(12), e82882. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0082882CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, D.V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K.H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., & Dunbar, G.C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl. 20), 2233.Google ScholarPubMed
Soriano, M., Jimenez, J., Roman, P., & Bajo, M. (2008). Cognitive substrates in semantic memory of formal thought disorder in schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30(1), 7082. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803390701220011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, M., Braun, U., Hermle, L., & Maier, S. (1993). Associative semantic network dysfunction in thought disordered schizophrenic patients: Direct evidence for indirect semantic priming. Biological Psychiatry, 34(12), 864877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, M., Braun, U., Maier, S., Hermle, L., & Maher, B.A. (1993). Indirect semantic priming in schizophrenic patients. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't]. Schizophrenia Research, 11(1), 7180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (2008). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms and commentary (2nd ed, New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stirling, J., Hellewell, J., Blakey, A., & Deakin, W. (2006). Thought disorder in schizophrenia is associated with both executive dysfunction and circumscribed impairments in semantic function. Psychological Medicine, 36(4), 475484. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291705006884CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Subotnik, K.L., Nuechterlein, K.H., Green, M.F., Horan, W.P., Nienow, T.M., Ventura, J., & Nguyen, A.T. (2006). Neurocognitive and social cognitive correlates of formal thought disorder in schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Research, 85(1-3), 8495. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.03.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sweeney, J.A., Keilp, J.G., Haas, G.L., Hill, J., & Weiden, P.J. (1991). Relationships between medication treatments and neuropsychological test performance in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 37(3), 297308. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0165-1781(91)90065-WCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, E.J., & Rossell, S.L. (2014). Building a neurocognitive profile of thought disorder in schizophrenia using a standardized test battery. Schizophrenia Research, 152(1), 242245. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, E.J., & Rossell, S.L. (2015). On the nosology of formal thought disorder. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 49(8), 758759. doi:10.1177/0004867415577439CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381403). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Warrington, E.K., & Shallice, T. (1979). Semantic access dyslexia. Brain, 102(1), 4363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2001). Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
World Medical Association. (2008). WMA Declaration of Helsinki - Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. Retrieved from http://www.wma.net/en/30publications/10policies/b3/.Google Scholar