Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T05:22:41.041Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The basal ganglia and semantic engagement: Potential insights from semantic priming in individuals with subcortical vascular lesions, Parkinson's disease, and cortical lesions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 February 2004

DAVID COPLAND
Affiliation:
Centre for Research in Language Processing and Linguistics, Department of Speech Pathology and Audiology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia

Abstract

The impact of basal ganglia dysfunction on semantic processing was investigated by comparing the performance of individuals with nonthalamic subcortical (NS) vascular lesions, Parkinson's disease (PD), cortical lesions, and matched controls on a semantic priming task. Unequibiased lexical ambiguity primes were used in auditory prime-target pairs comprising 4 critical conditions; dominant related (e.g., bank–money), subordinate related (e.g., bank–river), dominant unrelated (e.g., foot–money) and subordinate unrelated (e.g., bat–river). Participants made speeded lexical decisions (word/nonword) on targets using a go–no-go response. When a short prime–target interstimulus interval (ISI) of 200 ms was employed, all groups demonstrated priming for dominant and subordinate conditions, indicating nonselective meaning facilitation and intact automatic lexical processing. Differences emerged at the long ISI (1250 ms), where control and cortical lesion participants evidenced selective facilitation of the dominant meaning, whereas NS and PD groups demonstrated a protracted period of nonselective meaning facilitation. This finding suggests a circumscribed deficit in the selective attentional engagement of the semantic network on the basis of meaning frequency, possibly implicating a disturbance of frontal–subcortical systems influencing inhibitory semantic mechanisms. (JINS, 2003, 9, 1041–1052.)

Type
THEMATIC ARTICLES
Copyright
© 2003 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abdullaev, Y.G., Bechtereva, N.P., & Melnichuk, K.V. (1998). Neuronal activity of human caudate nucleus and prefrontal cortex in cognitive tasks. Behavioral Brain Research, 97, 159177.Google Scholar
Alexander, M.P., Naeser, M.A., & Palumbo, C.L. (1987). Correlations of subcortical CT lesion sites and aphasia profiles. Brain, 110, 961991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnott, W.L., Chenery, H.J., Murdoch, B.E., & Silburn, P. (2001). Semantic priming in Parkinson's disease: Evidence for delayed spreading activation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 502519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balota, D.A. & Paul, S.T. (1996). Summation of activation: Evidence from multiple primes that converge and diverge within semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 827845.Google Scholar
Balota, D.A., Watson, J.M., Duchek, J.M., & Ferraro, F.R. (1999). Cross-modal semantic and homograph priming in healthy young, healthy old, and in Alzheimer's disease individuals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5, 626640.Google Scholar
Bedard, M.A., Pillon, B., Dubois, B., Duchesne, N., Masson, H., & Agid, Y. (1999). Acute and long-term administration of anticholinergics in Parkinson's disease: Specific effects on the subcortico-frontal syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 40, 289313.Google Scholar
Binder, J.R., Frost, J.A., Hammeke, T.A., Cox, R.W., Rao, S.M., & Prieto, T. (1997). Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 353362.Google Scholar
Blumstein, S.E. (1997). A perspective on the neurobiology of language. Brain and Language, 60, 335346.Google Scholar
Brown, P. & Marsden, C.D. (1998). What do the basal ganglia do? Lancet, 351, 18011804.Google Scholar
Brown, L., Schneider, J., & Lidsky, T. (1997). Sensory and cognitive functions of the basal ganglia. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 157163.Google Scholar
Cabeza, R. & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 147.Google Scholar
Cappa, S.F. & Vallar, G. (1992). Neuropsychological disorders after subcortical lesions: implications for neural models of language and spatial attention. In G. Vallar, S.F. Cappa, & C.-W. Wallesch (Eds.), Neuropsychological disorders associated with subcortical lesions (pp. 741). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Coakes, S.J. & Steed, L.G. (1997). SPSS: Analysis without anguish. Brisbane, Australia: John Wiley & Sons.
Copland, D.A., Chenery, H.J., & Murdoch, B.E. (2000a). Understanding ambiguous words in biased sentences: Evidence of transient contextual effects in individuals with nonthalamic subcortical lesions and Parkinson's disease. Cortex, 36, 601622.Google Scholar
Copland, D.A., Chenery, H.J., & Murdoch, B.E. (2000b). Processing lexical ambiguities in word triplets: Evidence of lexical-semantic deficits following dominant nonthalamic subcortical lesions. Neuropsychology, 14, 370390.Google Scholar
Copland, D.A., Chenery, H.J., & Murdoch, B.E. (2001). Discourse priming of homophones in individuals with dominant subcortical lesions, cortical lesions, and Parkinson's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 338356.Google Scholar
Cottrell, G.W. (1989). A connectionist approach to word sense disambiguation. London: Pitman.
Crosson, B. (1985). Subcortical functions in language: A working model. Brain and Language, 25, 257292.Google Scholar
Crosson, B. (1992a). Subcortical functions in language and memory. New York: Guilford Press.
Crosson, B. (1992b). Is the striatum involved in language? In G. Vallar, S.F. Cappa, & C.-W. Wallesch (Eds.), Neuropsychological disorders associated with subcortical lesions (pp. 268293). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Crosson, B., Moberg, P., Boone, J., Gonzalez Rothi, L., & Raymer, A. (1997). Category-specific naming deficit for medical terms after fominant thalamic/capsular hemorrhage. Brain and Language, 60, 407442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crosson, B. (1999). Subcortical mechanisms in language: Lexical–semantic mechanisms and the thalamus. Brain and Cognition, 40, 414438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crosson, B., Rao, S.M., Woodley, S.J., Rosen, A.C., Bobholz, J.A., Mayer, A., Cunningham, J.M., Hammeke, T.A., Fuller, S.A., Binder, J.R., Cox, R.W., & Stein, E.A. (1999). Mapping of semantic, phonological, and orthographic verbal working memory in normal adults with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychology, 13, 117.Google Scholar
Cummings, J.L. (1993). Frontal–subcortical circuits and human behavior. Archives of Neurology, 50, 873880.Google Scholar
D'Esposito, M. (2000). Functional neuroimaging of cognition. Seminars in Neurology, 20, 487498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Divac, I., Rosvold, H.E., & Swarcbart, M. (1967). Behavioral effects of selective ablation of the caudate nucleus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63, 184190.Google Scholar
Early, T.S., Posner, M.I., Reiman, E.M., & Raichle, M.E. (1989). Left striato–pallidal hyperactivity in schizophrenia part II: Phenomenology and thought disorder. Psychiatric Developments, 2, 109121.Google Scholar
Fiebach, C., Friederici, A.D., Muller, K., & von Cramon, D.Y. (2002). fMRI evidence for dual routes to the mental lexicon in visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 1123.Google Scholar
Frith, C.D., Friston, K., Liddle, P.F., & Frackowiak, S.J. (1991). A PET study of word finding. Neuropsychologia, 29, 11371148.Google Scholar
Gerfen, C.R. (1992). The neostriatal mosaic: Multiple levels of compartmental organization in the basal ganglia. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 285320.Google Scholar
Gold, M., Nadeau, S.E., Jacobs, D.H., Adair, J.C., Gonzalez Rothi, L.J., & Heilman, K.M. (1997). Adynamic aphasia: A transcortical motor aphasia with defective semantic strategy formation. Brain and Language, 57, 374393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonzalez Rothi, L. (1990). Transcortical aphasias. In L.L. LaPointe (Ed.), Aphasia and related neurogenic language disorders (pp. 7895). New York: Thieme Press.
Grossman, M. (1999). Sentence processing in Parkinson's disease. Brain and Cognition, 40, 387413.Google Scholar
Gurd, J. & Oliveira, R. (1996). Competitive inhibition models of lexical–semantic processing: Experimental evidence. Brain and Language, 54, 414433.Google Scholar
Hoehn, M.M. & Yahr, M.D. (1967). Parkinsonism: Onset, progression and mortality. Neurology, 17, 427442.Google Scholar
Hu, M.T., Taylor-Robinson, S.D., Ray Chaudhuri, K., Bell, J.D., Labbe, C., Cunningham, V.J., Koepp, M.J., Hammers, A., Morris, R.G., Turjanski, N., & Brooks, D.J. (2000). Cortical dysfunction in non-demented Parkinson's disease patients: A combined P-MRS and FDG-PET study. Brain, 123, 340352.Google Scholar
Javoy-Agid, F. & Agid, Y. (1980). Is the mesocortical dopaminergic system involved in Parkinson's disease. Neurology, 30, 13261330.Google Scholar
Katz, W.F. (1988). An investigation of lexical ambiguity in Broca's aphasics using an auditory lexical priming technique. Neuropsychologia, 26, 747752.Google Scholar
Kirk, A. & Kertesz, A. (1994). Cortical and subcortical aphasias compared. Aphasiology, 8, 6582.Google Scholar
Kischka, U., Kammer, T., Maier, S., Weisbrod, M., Thimm, M., & Spitzer, M. (1996). Dopaminergic modulation of semantic network activation. Neuropsychologia, 34, 11071113.Google Scholar
Kotz, S., Cappa, S., von Cramon, D., & Friederici, A. (2002). Modulation of the lexical–semantic network by auditory semantic priming: An event-related functional MRI study. Neuroimage, 17, 17611772.Google Scholar
Kucera, H. & Francis, W.N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.
Kuperberg, G.R., McGuire, P.K., Bullmore, E.T., Brammer, M.J., Rabe-Hesketh, S., Wright, I.C., Lythgoe, D.J., Williams, S.C.R., & David, A.S. (2000). Common and distinct neural substrates for pragmatic, semantic, and syntactic processing of spoken sentences: An fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 321341.Google Scholar
Levin, B., Llabre, M., & Weiner, W. (1989). Cognitive impairments associated with early Parkinson's disease. Neurology, 39, 557561.Google Scholar
Mattis, S. (1988). DRS: Dementia Rating scale: Professional manual. Odessa, Florida: PAR, Inc.
McDonald, C., Brown, G.G., & Gorell, J.M. (1996). Impaired set-shifting in Parkinson's Disease: New evidence from a lexical decision task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18, 793809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendez, M.F., Adams, N.L., & Lewandowski, K.S. (1989). Neurobehavioral changes associated with caudate lesions. Neurology, 39, 349354.Google Scholar
Milberg, W., Blumstein, S., & Dworetzky, B. (1987). Processing of lexical ambiguities in aphasia. Brain and Language, 31, 138150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mink, J.W. (1996). The basal ganglia: Focused selection and inhibition of competing motor programs. Progress in Neurobiology, 50, 381425.Google Scholar
Mummery, C.J., Patterson, K., Hodges, J.R., & Price, C.J. (1998). Functional neuroanatomy of the semantic system: Divisible by what? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 766786.Google Scholar
Mummery, C.J., Shallice, T., & Price, C.J. (1999). Dual-Process model in semantic priming: A functional imaging perspective. NeuroImage, 9, 516525.Google Scholar
Nadeau, S.E. & Crosson, B. (1997). Subcortical aphasia. Brain and Language, 58, 355402.Google Scholar
Neely, J.H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G.W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264336). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Nelson, D.L., McEvoy, C.L., Walling, J.R., & Wheeler, J.W. (1980). The University of South Florida homograph norms. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 12, 1637.Google Scholar
Ni, W., Constable, R.T., Mencl, W.E., Pugh, K.R., Fulbright, R.K., Shaywitz, S.E., Shaywitz, B.A., Gore, J.C., & Shankweiler, D. (2000). An event-related neuroimaging study distinguishing form and content in sentence processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 120133.Google Scholar
Posner, M.I. & DiGirolamo, G.J. (1998). Executive attention: Conflict, target detection, and cognitive control. In R. Parasuraman (Ed.), The attentive brain (pp. 401423). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Pilgrim, L., Fadili, J., Fletcher, P., & Tyler, L. (2002). Overcoming confounds of stimulus blocking: An event-related fMRI design of semantic processing. Neuroimage, 16, 713723.Google Scholar
Price, C.J., Moore, C.J., Humphreys, G.W., & Wise, R.J.S. (1997). Segregating semantic from phonological processes during reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 727734.Google Scholar
Rossell, S.L., Bullmore, E.T., Williams, S.C.R., & David, A.S. (2001). Brain activation during automatic and controlled processing of semantic relations: A priming experiment using lexical decision. Neuropsychologia, 39, 11671176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scatton, B., Rouquier, L., Javoy-Agid, F., & Agid, Y. (1982). Dopamine deficiency in the cerebral cortex in Parkinson's disease. Neurology, 32, 10391040.Google Scholar
Simpson, G.B. & Burgess, C. (1985). Activation and selection processes in the recognition of ambiguous words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 2839.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M.K., Dell, G.S., & Carlson, G. (1987). Context effects and lexical processing: A connectionist approach to modularity. In J.L. Garfield (Ed.), Modularity in knowledge representation and natural language understanding (pp. 83108). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Taylor, A.E., Saint-Cyr, J.A., & Lang, A.E. (1986). Frontal lobe dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. The cortical focus of neostriatal outflow. Brain, 109, 845883.Google Scholar
Twilley, L.C., Dixon, P., Taylor, D., & Clark, K. (1994). University of Alberta norms of relative meaning frequency for 566 homographs. Memory and Cognition, 22, 111126.Google Scholar
Ullman, M.T., Corkin, S., Coppola, M., Hickok, G., Growdon, J.H., Koroshetz, W.J., & Pinker, S. (1997). A neural dissociation within language: Evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that grammatical rules are processed by the procedural system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 266276.Google Scholar
Wallesch, C.W. & Papagno, C. (1988). Subcortical aphasia. In F.C. Rose, R. Whurr, & M.A. Wyke (Eds.), Aphasia. London: Whurr Publishers.
Wallesch, C.-W. (1997). Symptomatology of subcortical aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 10, 267275.Google Scholar
Watters, P. & Patel, M. (2002). Competition, inhibition, and semantic judgment errors in Parkinson's disease. Brain and Language, 80, 328339.Google Scholar