Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T13:03:43.807Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Characteristics of Healthy Older Adults that Influence Self-rated Cognitive Function

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 July 2017

Bryce P. Mulligan*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada
Colette M. Smart
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada Institute on Aging and Lifelong Health, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada
Sidney J. Segalowitz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada Jack & Nora Walker Centre for Lifespan Development Research, Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada
Stuart W.S. MacDonald
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada Institute on Aging and Lifelong Health, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Bryce P. Mulligan, Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, PO Box 1700 STN CSC, Victoria, BC V8W 2Y2, Canada. E-mail: bpm@uvic.ca

Abstract

Objectives: We sought to clarify the nature of self-reported cognitive function among healthy older adults by considering the short-term, within-person association (coupling) of subjective cognitive function with objective cognitive performance. We expected this within-person coupling to differ between persons as a function of self-perceived global cognitive decline and depression, anxiety, or neuroticism. Methods: This was an intensive measurement (short-term longitudinal) study of 29 older adult volunteers between the ages of 65 and 80 years without an existing diagnosis of dementia or mild cognitive impairment. Baseline assessment included neuropsychological testing and self-reported depression, anxiety, and neuroticism, as well as self- and informant-reported cognitive decline (relative to 10 years previously). Intensive within-person measurement occasions included subjective ratings of cognitive function paired with performance on a computerized working memory (n-back) task; each participant attended four or five assessments separated by intervals of at least one day. Statistical analysis was comprised of multilevel linear regression. Results: Comparison of models suggested that both neuroticism and self-rated cognitive decline explained unique variance in the within-person, across-occasion coupling of subjective cognitive function with objective working memory performance. Conclusions: Self-ratings of cognition may accurately reflect day-to-day variations in objective cognitive performance among older adults, especially for individuals lower in neuroticism and higher in self-reported cognitive decline. Clinicians should consider these individual differences when determining the validity of complaints about perceived cognitive declines in the context of otherwise healthy aging. (JINS, 2018, 24, 57–66)

Type
Special Section: Lifespan Neuropsychology
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Amariglio, R.E., Townsend, M.K., Grodstein, F., Sperling, R.A., & Rentz, D.M. (2011). Specific subjective memory complaints in older persons may indicate poor cognitive function. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 59(9), 16121617. doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03543.x Google Scholar
Binder, L.M., Iverson, G.L., & Brooks, B.L. (2009). To err is human: “abnormal” neuropsychological scores and variability are common in healthy adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24(1), 3146. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acn001 Google Scholar
Boone, K.B. (2009). Fixed belief in cognitive dysfunction despite normal neuropsychological scores: Neurocognitive hypochondriasis? The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23(6), 10161036. doi: 10.1080/13854040802441135 Google Scholar
Borsboom, D., Mellenbergh, G.J., & van Heerden, J. (2003). The theoretical status of latent variables. Psychological Review, 110(2), 203219. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.203 Google Scholar
Brandmaier, A.M., von Oertzen, T., Ghisletta, P., Hertzog, C., & Lindenberger, U. (2015). LIFESPAN: A tool for the computer-aided design of longitudinal studies. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 110. doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00272 Google Scholar
Brose, A., Schmiedek, F., Lövdén, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2012). Daily variability in working memory is coupled with negative affect: The role of attention and motivation. Emotion, 12(3), 605617. doi: 10.1037/a0024436 Google Scholar
Brose, A., Schmiedek, F., & Molenaar, P.C.M. (2010). Adult age differences in covariation of motivation and working memory performance: Contrasting between-person and within-person findings. Research in Human Development, 7(1), 6178. doi: 10.1080/15427600903578177 Google Scholar
Button, K.S., Ioannidis, J.P.A., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B.A., Flint, J., Robinson, E.S.J., &&Munafò, M.R. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 14(5), 365376. doi.org/10.1038/nrn3475 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charles, S.T., & Carstensen, L.L. (2010). Social and emotional aging. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 383409. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100448 Google Scholar
Comijs, H., Deeg, D., & Dik, M. (2002). Memory complaints: The association with psycho-affective and health problems and the role of personality characteristics: A 6-year follow-up study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 72, 157165.Google Scholar
Cooper, C., Bebbington, P., Lindesay, J., Meltzer, H., McManus, S., Jenkins, R., &&Livingston, G. (2011). The meaning of reporting forgetfulness: A cross-sectional study of adults in the English 2007 Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey. Age and Ageing, 40(6), 711717. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afr121 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crum, R., Anthony, J., Bassett, S., & Folstein, M. (1993). Population-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational level. Journal of the American Medical Association, 269(18), 23862391. doi: 10.1001/jama.269.18.2386 Google Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test-2nd Edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Derouesné, C., & Lacomblez, L. (1999). Memory complaints in young and elderly subjects. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 291301.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dux, M.C., Woodard, J.L., Calamari, J.E., Messina, M., Arora, S., Chik, H., && Pontarelli, N. (2008). The moderating role of negative affect on objective verbal memory performance and subjective memory complaints in healthy older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(2), 327336. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080363 Google Scholar
Farias, S.T., Mungas, D., Reed, B.R., Cahn-Weiner, D., Jagust, W., Baynes, K., && Decarli, C. (2008). The measurement of everyday cognition (ECog): Scale development and psychometric properties. Neuropsychology, 22(4), 531544. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.4.531 Google Scholar
Folstein, M.F., & Folstein, S.E. (2001). Mini-mental state examination (2nd ed.). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Glodzik-Sobanska, L., Reisberg, B., De Santi, S., Babb, J.S., Pirraglia, E., Rich, K.E., & de Leon, M.J. (2007). Subjective memory complaints: Presence, severity and future outcome in normal older subjects. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 24(3), 177184. doi: 10.1159/000105604 Google Scholar
Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E., & Barresi, B. (2001). Boston diagnostic aphasia examination (3rd ed.). Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
Hofer, S.M., & Sliwinski, M.J. (2001). Understanding ageing: An evaluation of research designs for assessing the interdependence of ageing-related changes. Gerontology, 47(6), 341352. doi: 10.1159/000052825 Google Scholar
Hoffman, L. (2015). Longitudinal analysis: Modeling within-person fluctuation and change. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hoffman, L., & Stawski, R.S. (2009). Persons as contexts: Evaluating between-person and within-person effects in longitudinal analysis. Research in Human Development, 6(2-3), 97120. doi: 10.1080/15427600902911189 Google Scholar
Hooker, K., Monahan, D., Shifren, K., & Hutchinson, C. (1992). Mental and physical health of spouse caregivers: The role of personality. Psychology and Aging, 7(3), 367375.Google Scholar
Ivnik, R.J., Malec, J.F., Smith, G.E., Tangalos, E.G., & Petersen, R.C. (1996). Neuropsychological tests’ norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE token, WRAT-R reading, AMNART, STROOP, TMT, and JLO. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10(3), 262278. doi: 10.1080/13854049608406689 Google Scholar
Ivnik, R.J., Malec, J.F., Smith, G.E., Tangalos, E.G., Petersen, R.C., Kokmen, E., &&Kurland, L.T. (1992). Mayo’s older Americans normative studies: WAIS-R norms for ages 56 to 97. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6(S1), 130. doi: 10.1080/13854049208401877 Google Scholar
Jessen, F., Amariglio, R.E., van Boxtel, M., Breteler, M., Ceccaldi, M., … Chételat, G., SCD-I Working Group. (2014). A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 10(6), 844852. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001 Google Scholar
John, O.P., Donahue, E.M., & Kentle, R.L. (1991). The big five inventory-versions 4a and 54. Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and Social Research.Google Scholar
Jonker, C., Geerlings, M.I., & Schmand, B. (2000). Are memory complaints predictive for dementia? A review of clinical and population-based studies. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15(11), 983991.Google Scholar
Jorm, A., & Butterworth, P. (2004). Memory complaints in a community sample aged 60-64 years: Associations with cognitive functioning, psychiatric symptoms, medical conditions, APOE genotype. Psychological Medicine, 34(8), 14951506. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704003162 Google Scholar
Kliegel, M., & Zimprich, D. (2005). Predictors of cognitive complaints in older adults: A mixture regression approach. European Journal of Ageing, 2(1), 1323. doi: 10.1007/s10433-005-0017-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ladouceur, C.D., Silk, J.S., Dahl, R.E., Ostapenko, L., Kronhaus, D.M., & Phillips, M.L. (2009). Fearful faces influence attentional control processes in anxious youth and adults. Emotion, 9(6), 855864. doi: 10.1037/a0017747 Google Scholar
Lawton, M.P., & Brody, E.M. (1969). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. The Gerontologist, 9(3 Pt 1), 179186. doi: 10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179 Google Scholar
Lucas, J.A., Ivnik, R.J., Smith, G.E., Bohac, D.L., Tangalos, E.G., Graff-Radford, N.R., &&Petersen, R.C. (1998). Mayo’s older Americans normative studies: Category fluency norms. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20(2), 194200. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.2.194.1173 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, B. (1975). Comparison of the predicted and observed secondary structure of T4 phage lysozyme. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta (BBA)-Protein Structure, 405(2), 442451.Google Scholar
Mitchell, A.J., Beaumont, H., Ferguson, D., Yadegarfar, M., & Stubbs, B. (2014). Risk of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older people with subjective memory complaints: Meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 130(6), 439451. doi.org/10.1111/acps.12336 Google Scholar
Mistridis, P., Egli, S.C., Iverson, G.L., Berres, M., Willmes, K., Welsh-Bohmer, K.A., &&Monsch, A.U. (2015). Considering the base rates of low performance in cognitively healthy older adults improves the accuracy to identify neurocognitive impairment with the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease-Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (CERAD). European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 265(5), 407417. doi: 10.1007/s00406-014-0571-z Google Scholar
Molenaar, P. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: Bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Measurement, 2(4), 3741. doi: 10.1207/s15366359mea0204 Google Scholar
Mroczek, D.K., Spiro, A., Griffin, P.W., & Neupert, S. (2006). Social influences on adult personality, self-regulation, and health. In Social structures, aging and self-regulation (pp. 6983). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Mroczek, D.K., & Almeida, D.M. (2004). The effect of daily stress, personality, and age on daily negative affect. Journal of Personality, 72(2), 355378.Google Scholar
Neupert, S.D., Mroczek, D.K., & Spiro, A. (2008). Neuroticism moderates the daily relation between stressors and memory failures. Psychology and Aging, 23(2), 287296. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.2.287 Google Scholar
Rabin, L.A., Wang, C., Katz, M.J., Derby, C.A., Buschke, H., & Lipton, R.B. (2012). Predicting Alzheimer’s disease: Neuropsychological tests, self-reports, and informant reports of cognitive difficulties. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 60(6), 11281134. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.03956.x Google Scholar
Rabin, L.A., Saykin, A.J., Wishart, H.A., Nutter-Upham, K.E., Flashman, L.A., Pare, N., & Santulli, R.B. (2007). The Memory and Aging Telephone Screen: Development and preliminary validation. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 3(2), 109121. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.02.002 Google Scholar
Rabin, L.A., Smart, C.M., Crane, P.K., Amariglio, R.E., Berman, L.M., Boada, M., & Sikkes, S.A. (2015). Subjective cognitive decline in older adults: An overview of self-report measures used across 19 international research studies. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 48(Suppl 1), S63S86. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150154 Google Scholar
Reisberg, B., & Gauthier, S. (2008). Current evidence for subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) as the pre-mild cognitive impairment (MCI) stage of subsequently manifest Alzheimer’s disease. International Psychogeriatrics, 20(1), 116. doi: 10.1017/S1041610207006412 Google Scholar
Reisberg, B., Prichep, L., Mosconi, L., John, E.R., Glodzik-Sobanska, L., Boksay, I., & de Leon, M.J. (2008). The pre-mild cognitive impairment, subjective cognitive impairment stage of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 4, S98S108. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.11.017 Google Scholar
Reynolds, C.R., Richmond, B.O., & Lowe, P.A. (2003). The Adult Manifest Anxiety Scale–Elderly Version (AMAS-E). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Robinson, W. (1950). Ecological correlations and the behavior of individuals. American Sociological Review, 15(3), 351357.Google Scholar
Royall, D.R., Cordes, J.A., & Polk, M. (1998). CLOX: An executive clock drawing task. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 64(5), 588594.Google Scholar
Schretlen, D.J., Munro, C.A., Anthony, J.C., & Pearlson, G.D. (2003). Examining the range of normal intraindividual variability in neuropsychological test performance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9(6), 864870. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703960061 Google Scholar
Shieh, Y.-Y., & Fouladi, R.T. (2003). The effect of multicollinearity on multilevel modeling parameter estimates and standard errors. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 63(6), 951985. doi: 10.1177/0013164403258402 Google Scholar
Slavin, M.J., Brodaty, H., Kochan, N.A., Crawford, J.D., Trollor, J.N., Draper, B., &&Sachdev, P.S. (2010). Prevalence and predictors of “subjective cognitive complaints” in the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(8), 701710. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181df49fb Google Scholar
Singer, J.D., & Willett, J.B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Weaver-Cargin, J., Collie, A., Masters, C.L., & Maruff, P. (2008). The nature of cognitive complaints in healthy older adults with and without objective memory decline. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30(2), 245257. doi: 10.1080/13803390701377829 Google Scholar
Wechsler, D.A. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D.A. (1987). Manual for the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Yesavage, J., Brink, T.L., Rose, T.L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., &&Leirer, V.O. (1983). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17(I), 3749. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4 Google Scholar