Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T11:01:34.511Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of fixed- and varied-context repetition on associative recognition in amnesia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 April 2010

DANIEL L. GREENBERG*
Affiliation:
Memory Disorders Research Center, VA Boston Healthcare System and Boston University School of Medicine
MIEKE VERFAELLIE
Affiliation:
Memory Disorders Research Center, VA Boston Healthcare System and Boston University School of Medicine
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Daniel L. Greenberg, Memory Disorders Research Center, 150 S. Huntington Avenue (151-A), Boston, MA 02130. E-mail: dlg@bu.edu

Abstract

This study compared the effects of fixed- and varied-context repetition on associative recognition in amnesia. Controls and amnesic participants were presented with a set of three-word phrases. Each was presented three times. In the varied-context condition, the verb changed with each presentation; in the fixed-context condition, it remained constant. At test, participants performed an associative-recognition task in which they were shown pairs of words from the study phase and asked to distinguish between intact and recombined pairs. For corrected recognition (hits – false alarms), controls performed better in the varied-context than in the fixed-context repetition condition, whereas amnesic participants’ performance did not differ between conditions. Similarly, controls had lower false-alarm rates in the varied-context condition, but there was no significant effect of condition for the amnesic participants. Thus, varied-context repetition does not improve amnesic participants’ performance on a recollection-dependent associative-recognition task, possibly because the amnesic participants were unable to take advantage of the additional cues that the varied-context encoding condition provided. (JINS, 2010, 16, 596–602.)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baddeley, A.D., & Wilson, B.A. (1994). When implicit learning fails: Amnesia and the problem of error elimination. Neuropsychologia, 32, 5368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjork, R.A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In Metcalfe, J. & Shimamura, A. (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 185205). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjork, R.A., & Bjork, E.L. (2006). Optimizing treatment and instruction: Implications of a new theory of disuse. In Nilsson, L.-G. & Ohta, N. (Eds.), Memory and society: Psychological perspectives. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Cermak, L.S., Verfaellie, M., Lanzoni, S., Mather, M., & Chase, K. (1996). Effect of spaced repetitions on amnesia patients’ recall and recognition performance. Neuropsychology, 10, 219227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, S.E., & Gronlund, S.D. (1996). Global matching models of memory: How the models match the data. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 3760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davachi, L. (2006). Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, 693700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlhardt, L.A., Sohlberg, M.M., Kennedy, M., Coelho, C., Ylvisaker, M., Turkstra, L., et al. . (2008). Evidence-based practice guidelines for instructing individuals with neurogenic memory impairments: What have we learned in the past 20 years? Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 18, 300342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eichenbaum, H., & Cohen, N.J. (2001). From conditioning to conscious recollection: Memory systems of the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gardiner, J.M. (1988). Functional aspects of recollective experience. Memory & Cognition, 24, 523533.Google Scholar
Giovanello, K.S., Verfaellie, M., & Keane, M.M. (2003). Disproportionate deficit in associative recognition relative to item recognition in global amnesia. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 186194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenberg, A.M. (1979). Component-levels theory of the effects of spacing of repetitions on recall and recognition. Memory & Cognition, 7, 95112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, J.J., Hopkins, R.O., & Squire, L.R. (2006). Single-item memory, associative memory, and the human hippocampus. Learning & Memory, 13, 644649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hannula, D.E., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N.J. (2006). The long and the short of it: Relational memory impairments in amnesia, even at short lags. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 83528359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, T., Mahendra, N., Kim, E., Azuma, T., Bayles, K.A., Cleary, S.J., et al. . (2005). Evidence-based practice recommendations for working with individuals with dementia: Spaced-retrieval training. Journal of Medical Speech Language Pathology, 13, 2734.Google Scholar
Huppert, F.A., & Piercy, M. (1978). The role of trace strength in recency and frequency judgements by amnesics and control subjects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30, 346354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kan, I.P., Giovanello, K.S., Schnyer, D.M., Makris, N., & Verfaellie, M. (2007). Role of the medial temporal lobes in relational memory: Neuropsychological evidence from a cued recognition paradigm. Neuropsychologia, 45, 25892597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konkel, A., Warren, D.E., Duff, M.C., Tranel, D.N., & Cohen, N.J. (2008). Hippocampal amnesia impairs all manner of relational memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2, 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leding, J.K., & Lampinen, J.M. (2009). Memory conjunction errors: The effects of presentation duration and study repetition. Memory, 17, 597607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macmillan, N.A., & Creelman, C.D. (2005). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mayes, A.R., Holdstock, J., Isaac, C., Montaldi, D., Grigor, J., Gummer, A., et al. . (2004). Associative recognition in a patient with selective hippocampal lesions and relatively normal item recognition. Hippocampus, 14, 763784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J.L., McNaughton, B.L., & O’Reilly, R.C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102, 419457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, D.L., McEvoy, C.L., & Schreiber, T.A. (1998). The University of South Florida word association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Retrieved August 25, 2009, from http://w3.usf.edu/FreeAssociation/Google Scholar
Paivio, A. (1974). Spacing of repetitions in the incidental and intentional free recall of pictures and words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 497511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Postman, L., & Knecht, K. (1983). Encoding variability and retention. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 133152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rajaram, S., Hamilton, M., & Bolton, A. (2002). Distinguishing states of awareness from confidence during retrieval: Evidence from amnesia. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 227235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S.M., Glenberg, A.M., & Bjork, R.A. (1978). Environmental context and human memory. Memory and Cognition, 6, 342353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soraci, S., Carlin, M.T., Chechile, R.A., Franks, J.J., Wills, T., & Watanbe, T. (1999). Encoding variability and cuing in generative processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 541559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Squire, L.R., Wixted, J.T., & Clark, R.E. (2007). Recognition memory and the medial temporal lobe: A new perspective. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 872883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stark, S., Gordon, B., & Stark, C. (2008). A case study of amnesia: Exploring a paradigm for new semantic learning and generalization. Brain Injury, 22, 283292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stark, C., Stark, S., & Gordon, B. (2005). New semantic learning and generalization in a patient with amnesia. Neuropsychology, 19, 139151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troyer, A.K., Murphy, K.J., Anderson, N.D., Hayman-Abello, B.A., Craik, F.I., & Moscovitch, M. (2008). Item and associative memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology, 22, 1016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 26, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turriziani, P., Fadda, L., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G.A. (2004). Recognition memory for single items and for associations in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 42, 426433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verfaellie, M., Rajaram, S., Fossum, K., & Williams, L. (2008). Not all repetition is alike: Different benefits of repetition in amnesia and normal memory. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 365372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verkoeijen, P.P., Rikers, R.M., & Schmidt, H.G. (2004). Detrimental influence of contextual change on spacing effects in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30, 796800.Google ScholarPubMed
Wilson, B.A. (2009). Memory rehabilitation. New York: Guilford Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wixted, J.T. (2007). Dual-process theory and signal-detection theory of recognition memory. Psychological Review, 114, 152176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, J., & Malmberg, K.J. (2007). Modeling the effects of verbal and nonverbal pair strength on associative recognition. Memory & Cognition, 35, 526544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yonelinas, A.P. (1994). Receiver-operating characteristics in recognition memory: Evidence for a dual-process model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 13411354.Google ScholarPubMed
Yonelinas, A.P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, D.R., & Bellezza, F.S. (1982). Encoding variability, memory organization, and the repetition effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 545559.Google Scholar