Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T21:55:35.741Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of Marathi-Hindi Bilingualism on Neuropsychological Performance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 December 2011

Rujvi Kamat*
Affiliation:
San Diego University/ University of California, San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology; San Diego, California
Manisha Ghate
Affiliation:
National AIDS Research Institute, Pune, Maharashtra
Tamar H. Gollan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, School of Medicine; San Diego, California
Rachel Meyer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, School of Medicine; San Diego, California
Florin Vaida
Affiliation:
Department of Family and Preventative Medicine, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California
Robert K. Heaton
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, School of Medicine; San Diego, California
Scott Letendre
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, University of California, San Diego, School of Medicine; San Diego, California
Donald Franklin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, School of Medicine; San Diego, California
Terry Alexander
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, School of Medicine; San Diego, California
Igor Grant
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, School of Medicine; San Diego, California
Sanjay Mehendale
Affiliation:
National AIDS Research Institute, Pune, Maharashtra
Thomas D. Marcotte
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, School of Medicine; San Diego, California
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Rujvi Kamat. 220 Dickenson Street, Suite B, San Diego, CA 92103. E-mail: rkamat@ucsd.edu

Abstract

The present study aimed to examine if bilingualism affects executive functions and verbal fluency in Marathi and Hindi, two major languages in India, with a considerable cognate (e.g., activity is actividad in Spanish) overlap. A total of 174 native Marathi speakers from Pune, India, with varying levels of Hindi proficiency were administered tests of executive functioning and verbal performance in Marathi. A bilingualism index was generated using self-reported Hindi and Marathi proficiency. After controlling for demographic variables, the association between bilingualism and cognitive performance was examined. Degree of bilingualism predicted better performance on the switching (Color Trails-2) and inhibition (Stroop Color-Word) components of executive functioning; but not for the abstraction component (Halstead Category Test). In the verbal domain, bilingualism was more closely associated with noun generation (where the languages share many cognates) than verb generation (which are more disparate across these languages), as predicted. However, contrary to our hypothesis that the bilingualism “disadvantage” would be attenuated on noun generation, bilingualism was associated with an advantage on these measures. These findings suggest distinct patterns of bilingualism effects on cognition for this previously unexamined language pair, and that the rate of cognates may modulate the association between bilingualism and verbal performance on neuropsychological tests. (JINS, 2012, 18, 305–313)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Annamalai, E. (1990). Linguistic dominance and cultural dominance: A study of tribal bilingualism in India. Multilingualism in India, 2536.Google Scholar
Army Individual Test Battery (1944). Manual of directions and scoring. Washington, DC: War Department, Adjutant General's Office.Google Scholar
Benedikter, T. (2011). Language policy and linguistic minorities in India: An appraisal of the linguistic rights of minorities in India (Vol. 3). Berlin: LIT Verlag Munster.Google Scholar
Benton, L., Hamsher, K., Sivan, A. (1994). Multilingual aphasia examination. Iowa City: AJA Associates.Google Scholar
Bhatia, T.K., Ritchie, W.C. (2004). The handbook of bilingualism. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I., Grady, C., Chau, W., Ishii, R., Gunji, A., Pantev, C. (2005). Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: Evidence from MEG. Neuroimage, 24(1), 4049. doi:S1053-8119(04)00585-3 [pii]10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., Green, D.W., Gollan, T.H. (2009). Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10(3), 89129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I., Klein, R., Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290303. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290 2004-14948-005 [pii].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., Luk, G. (2008). Lexical access in bilinguals: Effects of vocabulary size and executive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(6), 522538. doi:Doi 10.1016/J.Jneuroling.2007.07.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Feng, X. (2009). Language proficiency and executive control in proactive interference: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual children and adults. Brain and Language, 109(2–3), 93100. doi:S0093-934X(08)00112-0 [pii] 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.09.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Majumder, S. (1998). The relationship between bilingualism and the development of cognitive processes in problem solving. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 6986.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bunge, S., Dudukovic, N., Thomason, M., Vaidya, C., Gabrieli, J. (2002). Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: Evidence from fMRI. Neuron, 33(2), 301311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choca, J., Laatsch, L., Wetzel, L., Agresti, A. (1997). The Halstead Category Test: A fifty year perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 7(2), 6175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colzato, L.S., Bajo, M.T., van den Wildenberg, W., Paolieri, D., Nieuwenhuis, S., La Heij, W., Hommel, B. (2008). How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(2), 302.Google ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., Caramazza, A., Sebastian-Galles, N. (2000). The cognate facilitation effect: Implications for models of lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 12831296.Google ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., Hernandez, M., Sebastian-Galles, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106(1), 5986. doi:S0010-0277(06)00268-X [pii] 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., Santesteban, M., Ivanova, I. (2006). How do highly proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process? Inhibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms are both functional. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 32(5), 10571074. doi:2006-10980-008 [pii] 10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Elia, L., Satz, P., Uchiyama, C., White, T. (1996). Color trail test: Professional manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Deoskar, T. (2006). Marathi light verbs. Proceedings from the Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, 42(2), 183198.Google Scholar
Dugbartey, A.T., Townes, B.D., Mahurin, R.K. (2000). Equivalence of the Color Trails Test and Trail Making Test in nonnative English-speakers. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15(5), 425431. doi:S0887-6177(99)00034-7 [pii].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golden, C. (1978). Stroop color and word test. Chicago: Stoelting.Google Scholar
Gollan, T.H., Acenas, L.A.R. (2004). What is a TOT? Cognate and translation effects on tip-of-the-tongue states in Spanish-English and Tagalog-English bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 30(1), 246269. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.1.246.Google ScholarPubMed
Gollan, T.H., Bonanni, M.P., Montoya, R.I. (2005). Proper names get stuck on bilingual and monolingual speakers’ tip of the tongue equally often. Neuropsychology, 19(3), 278287. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.19.3.278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T.H., Montoya, R.I., Cera, C., Sandoval, T.C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(3), 787814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T.H., Montoya, R.I., Fennema-Notestine, C., Morris, S.K. (2005). Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory and Cognition, 33(7), 12201234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T.H., Montoya, R.I., Werner, G.A. (2002). Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuropsychology, 16(4), 562576. doi:10.1037//0894-4105.16.4.562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D.W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(02), 6781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heaton, R., Grant, I., Matthews, C. (1991). Comprehensive Norms for an Expanded Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Battery: Demographic corrections, research findings, and clinical applications. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Heaton, R.K., Ryan, L., Grant, I. (2009). Demographic influences and use of demographically corrected norms in neuropsychological assessment. Neuropsychological Assessment of Neuropsychiatric and Neuromedical Disorders, 3, 127155.Google Scholar
Hernandez, M., Costa, A., Fuentes, L.J., Vivas, A.B., Sebastian-Galles, N. (2010). The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(03), 315325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ivanova, I., Costa, A. (2008). Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production? Acta Psychologica, 127(2), 277288. doi:S0001-6918(07)00050-9 [pii] 10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.06.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kroll, J.F., Bobb, S.C., Misra, M., Guo, T. (2008). Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica, 128(3), 416430. doi:10.1016/J.Actpsy.2008.02.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lansdell, H., Donnelly, E.F. (1977). Factor analysis of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale subtests and the Halstead-Reitan Category and Tapping tests. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45(3), 412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, L., Luk, G., Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 114(1), 2941. doi:S0010-0277(09)00209-1 [pii].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maj, M., Delia, L., Satz, P., Janssen, R., Zaudig, M., Uchiyama, C., Chervinsky, A. (1993). Evaluation of 2 new neuropsychological tests designed to minimize cultural-bias in the assessment of HIV-1 seropositive persons – A WHO study. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 8(2), 123135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Makin, R., Pandey, N., Pingali, P., Varma, V. (2007). Approximate string matching techniques for effective CLIR among Indian languages. Applications of Fuzzy Sets Theory, 430437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H.K., Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940967. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meuter, R.F.I., Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(1), 2540. doi:10.1006/jmla.1998.2602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pandharipande, R. (1986). Language contact and language variation: Nagpuri Marathi. South Asian Languages: Structure, Convergence, and Diglossia, 3, 219.Google Scholar
Pendleton, M., Heaton, R. (1982). A comparison of the Wisconsin card sorting test and the category test. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38(2), 392396.3.0.CO;2-W>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perani, D., Paulesu, E., Galles, N.S., Dupoux, E., Dehaene, S., Bettinardi, V., Mehler, J. (1998). The bilingual brain. Proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. Brain, 121(10), 1841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piatt, A., Fields, J., Paolo, A., Troster, A. (1999). Action verbal fluency: Normative data for the elderly. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14(1), 6262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Portocarrero, J., Burright, R., Donovick, P. (2007). Vocabulary and verbal fluency of bilingual and monolingual college students. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(3), 415422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, A., Gollan, T.H. (2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, A., MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(02), 253262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramanand, J., Ukey, A., Singh, B., Bhattacharyya, P. (2007). Mapping and structural analysis of multi-lingual wordnets. IEEE Data Engineering Bulletin, 30(1).Google Scholar
Ratcliff, G., Ganguli, M., Chandra, V., Sharma, S., Belle, S., Seaberg, E., Pandav, R. (1998). Effects of literacy and education on measures of word fluency. Brain and Language, 61(1), 115122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitan, R.M., Wolfson, D. (1993). The Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery: Theory and clinical interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
Rivera Mindt, M., Arentoft, A., Kubo Germano, K., DíAquila, E., Scheiner, D., Pizzirusso, M., Gollan, T.H. (2008). Neuropsychological, cognitive, and theoretical considerations for evaluation of bilingual individuals. Neuropsychology Review, 18(3), 255268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Araujo, K., Weekes, V.A., Caracciolo, V., Padilla, M., Ostrosky-Solis, F. (2000). Verbal fluency and repetition skills in healthy older Spanish-English bilinguals. Applied Neuropsychology, 7(1), 1724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandoval, T.C., Gollan, T.H., Ferreira, V.S., Salmon, D.P. (2010). What causes the bilingual disadvantage in verbal fluency? The dual-task analogy. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(2), 231252. doi:10.1017/s1366728909990514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaywitz, B.A., Pugh, K.R., Todd Constable, R., Shaywitz, S.E., Bronen, R.A., Fulbright, R.K., Skudlarski, P. (1994). Localization of semantic processing using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Human Brain Mapping, 2(3), 149158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, A., Surana, H. (2007a). Can corpus based measures be used for comparative study of languages? Paper presented at the ACL Special Interest Group on Computational Phonology and Morphology, Prague, Czech Republic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, A.K., Surana, H. (2007b). Study of cognates among south asian languages for the purpose of building lexical resources. Journal of Language Technology, 8, 7882.Google Scholar