Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T09:25:11.997Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotion perception deficits following traumatic brain injury: A review of the evidence and rationale for intervention

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 June 2008

CRISTINA BORNHOFEN*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
SKYE MCDONALD
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Cristina Bornhofen, School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney 2052, Australia. E-mail: cristinab@iinet.net.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

While the cognitive disturbances that frequently follow severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) are relatively well understood, the ways in which these affect the psychosocial functioning of people with TBI are yet to be determined and have thus received little attention in treatment research. Growing evidence indicates that a significant proportion of individuals with TBI demonstrate an inability to recognize affective information from the face, voice, bodily movement, and posture. Because accurate interpretation of emotion in others is critical for the successful negotiation of social interactions, effective treatments are necessary. Until recently, however, there have been no rehabilitation efforts in this area. The present review examines the literature on emotion perception deficits in TBI and presents a theoretical rationale for targeted intervention. Several lines of research relevant to the remediation of emotion perception in people with TBI are considered. These include work on emotion perception remediation with other cognitively impaired populations, current neuropsychological models of emotion perception and underlying neural systems, and recent conceptualizations of remediation processes. The article concludes with a discussion of the importance of carrying out efforts to improve emotion perception within a contextualized framework in which the day-to-day relevance of training is clear to all recipients. (JINS, 2008, 14, 511–525.)

Type
Critical Review
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2008

INTRODUCTION

The effects of traumatic brain injury (TBI) on psychosocial functioning comprise an even greater long-term barrier to adjustment and rehabilitation than the effects on cognitive and physical functioning (Eslinger et al., Reference Eslinger, Grattan and Geder1995; Godfrey et al., Reference Godfrey, Knight and Partridge1996; Grattan & Ghahramanlou, Reference Grattan, Ghahramanlou and Eslinger2002; Hoofien et al., Reference Hoofien, Gilboa, Vakil and Donovick2001; Tate et al., Reference Tate, Lulham, Broe, Strettles and Pfaff1989b; Thomsen, Reference Thomsen1984; Yates, Reference Yates2003). In the months and years following TBI, a significant proportion of individuals experience breakdowns in one or more aspects of social functioning. This includes loss of employment, disruption to intimate relationships, and reduced social networks (Elsass & Kinsella, Reference Elsass and Kinsella1987; Gosling & Oddy, Reference Gosling and Oddy1999; Hallett et al., Reference Hallett, Zasler, Maurer and Cash1994; Kersel et al., Reference Kersel, Marsh, Havill and Sleigh2001; Oddy & Humphrey, Reference Oddy and Humphrey1980; Oddy et al., Reference Oddy, Humphrey and Uttley1978; Peters et al., Reference Peters, Stambrook, Moore and Esses1990; Tate et al., Reference Tate, Lulham, Broe, Strettles and Pfaff1989b; Thomsen, Reference Thomsen1974; Weddell et al., Reference Weddell, Oddy and Jenkins1980; Ylvisaker & Feeney, Reference Ylvisaker and Feeney2000). In many cases, circumstances do not improve with time, but instead tend to worsen (Burleigh et al., Reference Burleigh, Farber and Gillard1998; Dombovy & Olek, Reference Dombovy and Olek1997; Durgin, Reference Durgin2000; Godfrey & Shum, Reference Godfrey and Shum2000; Gomez-Hernandez et al., Reference Gomez-Hernandez, Max, Kosier, Paradiso and Robinson1997; Hammond et al., Reference Hammond, Hart, Bushnik, Corrigan and Sasser2004; Morton & Wehman, Reference Morton and Wehman1995; Oddy et al., Reference Oddy, Coughen, Tyerman and Jenkins1985; Olver et al., Reference Olver, Ponsford and Curran1996; Tate et al., Reference Tate, Lulham, Broe, Strettles and Pfaff1989b).

The principal causes for these social difficulties are likely to be complex, including a variety of internal factors, such as cognitive, emotional, and physical status, and external circumstances, such as reduced social opportunities and limited support (Biddle et al., Reference Biddle, McCabe and Bliss1996; Bond & Godfrey, Reference Bond and Godfrey1997; Chapman, Reference Chapman1997; Chapman et al., Reference Chapman, Levin, Matejka, Harward and Kufera1995; Chapman et al., Reference Chapman, Watkins, Gustafson, Moore, Levin and Kufera1997; Godfrey et al., Reference Godfrey, Knight and Bishara1991; Godfrey & Shum, Reference Godfrey and Shum2000; Marsh, Reference Marsh, McDonald, Togher and Code1999; McDonald, Reference McDonald1992, Reference McDonald1993, Reference McDonald2000; Ylvisaker, Reference Ylvisaker1993). The major focus of this review is the role that impaired emotion perception may play in reducing social competence post TBI, and even more importantly, the extent to which this can be seen as a legitimate target for remediation. The notion that emotion perception deficits are a major problem for many people with TBI has only recently been recognized, with just two papers that reported such deficits (Jackson & Moffat, Reference Jackson and Moffat1987; Prigatano & Pribram, Reference Prigatano and Pribram1982) before the 1990s. There is now, however, an established literature that attests to the prevalence of emotion perception deficits in this population. The emergence of this literature coincides with an increased interest in the efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation for people with brain injuries in general and renewed efforts to provide relevant, theoretically driven approaches to remediation that will lead to real functional gains. In this review, we will address the literature to date that defines the nature of emotion processing, its relevance to social competence and how it is impaired in those with TBI. Next, we will examine evidence for treatment of emotion perception disorders in other populations that may have some relevance to TBI. Finally, we will describe the specific neuropsychological and neuroanatomical underpinnings of emotion perception and relate these to potential remediation approaches.

THE SIGNIFICANCE OF EMOTION PERCEPTION

Emotion perception, that is, the ability to accurately perceive and appreciate affective information from facial expressions, emotional prosody, body posture, and contextual parameters (such as the type of social occasion, the relationship between speakers, etc.), is critical to social competence. Numerous studies of normal adults have demonstrated that those who are poor at reading social cues also experience poor social skills in general (Boice, Reference Boice1983; Morrison & Bellack, Reference Morrison and Bellack1981; Trower, Reference Trower1980). Poor emotion perception has also been shown to be related to social adjustment in school-aged children (Leppanen & Hietanen, Reference Leppanen and Hietanen2001).

Furthermore, disruption to emotion perception appears to play a role in a variety of clinical disorders, such as autism (Bolte & Poustka, Reference Bolte and Poustka2003; Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, Minshew, Leonard and Behrmann2007; Lindner & Rosen, Reference Lindner and Rosen2006; Mazefsky & Oswald, Reference Mazefsky and Oswald2007), schizophrenia (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Jackson and Pattison2002; Mandal et al., Reference Mandal, Pandey and Prasad1998; Tremeau, Reference Tremeau2006), attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children (Kats-Gold et al., Reference Kats-Gold, Besser and Priel2007; Pelc et al., Reference Pelc, Kornreich, Foisy and Dan2006), intellectual disability (Rojahn & Warren, Reference Rojahn and Warren1997; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Wishart, Pitcairn and Willis2005; Wishart et al., Reference Wishart, Cebula, Willis and Pitcairn2007), and anxiety disorders (panic disorder, Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Roth, von Wietersheim, Deighton and Traue2007; obsessive-compulsive disorder, Aigner et al., Reference Aigner, Sachs, Bruckmuller, Winklbaur, Zitterl, Kryspin-Exner, Gur and Katschnig2007; social anxiety, Montagne et al., Reference Montagne, Schutters, Westenberg, van Honk, Kessels and de Haan2006). Indeed, evidence from clinical groups [e.g., TBI (Watts & Douglas, Reference Watts and Douglas2006), schizophrenia (Hooker & Park, Reference Hooker and Park2002; Kee et al., Reference Kee, Green, Mintz and Brekke2003; Morrison & Bellack, Reference Morrison and Bellack1981; Mueser et al., Reference Mueser, Doonan, Penn, Blanchard, Bellack, Nishith and DeLeon1996; Sergi et al., Reference Sergi, Rassovsky, Nuechterlein and Green2006), autism (Boraston et al., Reference Boraston, Blakemore, Chilvers and Skuse2007), and children with ADHD characteristics (Kats-Gold et al., Reference Kats-Gold, Besser and Priel2007)] mirrors studies of normal adults in suggesting that those who are poor at reading affective information demonstrate low levels of social skills and/or social functioning. In the schizophrenia field, it has been shown that emotion perception ability is not only related to level of social functioning (Penn et al., Reference Penn, Combs, Mohamed, Corrigan and Penn2001) but may adversely affect social functioning independently of both cognitive deficits and positive and negative symptoms of the disorder (Kohler et al., Reference Kohler, Bilker, Hagendoorn, Gur and Gur2000). Others within this field suggest that emotion perception may mediate between cognitive and social functioning in psychosis (Addington et al., Reference Addington, Saeedi and Addington2006). Such findings have clear implications for treatment of emotion perception in schizophrenia (Green et al., Reference Green, Olivier, Crawley, Penn and Silverstein2005). They also highlight the potential importance of emotion perception deficits following TBI.

EMOTION PERCEPTION AND THE CASE OF TBI

The first report of impaired emotion perception in a group of people with TBI was made by Prigatano and Pibram in Reference Prigatano and Pribram1982. Since that time, accumulating evidence has indicated that a significant proportion of people with TBI demonstrate impaired perception of emotional information across a range of media. In terms of visual media, these include both relatively simple (black and white line drawings) and more complex (photographs and video scenes) stimuli, as well as input from different bodily sources, that is, facial expression and body posture (Allerdings & Alfano, Reference Allerdings and Alfano2006; Croker & McDonald, Reference Croker and McDonald2005; Green et al., Reference Green, Turner and Thompson2004; Jackson & Moffat, Reference Jackson and Moffat1987; McDonald et al., Reference McDonald, Flanagan, Rollins and Kinch2003; McDonald & Saunders, Reference McDonald and Saunders2005; Milders et al., Reference Milders, Fuchs and Crawford2003). Deficits in judging prosodic affective cues were first reported in 1996 (McDonald & Pearce, Reference McDonald and Pearce1996) and have since then been repeatedly found (McDonald & Saunders, Reference McDonald and Saunders2005; Milders et al., Reference Milders, Fuchs and Crawford2003; Spell & Frank, Reference Spell and Frank2000). Recently, it has been shown that people with TBI also show impairment in interpreting the emotional content of multichannel stimuli in audiovisual format (McDonald & Flanagan, Reference McDonald and Flanagan2004; McDonald et al., Reference McDonald, Flanagan, Rollins and Kinch2003; McDonald & Saunders, Reference McDonald and Saunders2005). Across all modalities, perception of negative emotions (anger, disgust, sadness, and fear) seems to be worse than perception of positive emotions (happiness and surprise; Croker & McDonald, Reference Croker and McDonald2005; Green et al., Reference Green, Turner and Thompson2004; Hopkins et al., Reference Hopkins, Dywan and Segalowitz2002; Jackson & Moffat, Reference Jackson and Moffat1987; McDonald et al., Reference McDonald, Flanagan, Rollins and Kinch2003). Additionally, there is growing evidence of impairment in the ability to make mental state inferences—which rely heavily on the interpretation of affective cues (Bibby & McDonald, Reference Bibby and McDonald2005; McDonald & Flanagan, Reference McDonald and Flanagan2004; McDonald et al., Reference McDonald, Flanagan, Rollins and Kinch2003, Reference McDonald, Flanagan, Martin and Saunders2004).

Deficits in emotion perception vary among individuals with TBI, with some demonstrating little or no impairment on measures used and others performing at near chance levels (Bornhofen & McDonald, in press; Croker & McDonald, Reference Croker and McDonald2005; McDonald & Pearce, Reference McDonald and Pearce1996; McDonald & Saunders, Reference McDonald and Saunders2005). Similarly, there are suggestions that individuals with TBI who have emotion perception deficits may differ with respect to modality of deficit, such that some cases may demonstrate difficulties with recognizing facial expressions but not emotional prosody, and others, vice versa (McDonald & Saunders, Reference McDonald and Saunders2005). These results may reflect heterogeneity with respect to pathology as well as the impact of other cognitive deficits present in this group (Allerdings & Alfano, Reference Allerdings and Alfano2006). Another potentially important factor is the availability of social opportunities for people with TBI who may no longer be able to work or whose mobility has been limited by physical impairments. In such cases, it is possible that sheer isolation from prior social networks and poor community reintegration may maintain or intensify deficits in emotion processing. This possibility has not yet been directly examined in research.

Finally, recent research has raised the possibility that emotional reactivity may be disturbed following TBI (Blair & Cipolotti, Reference Blair and Cipolotti2000; Croker & McDonald, Reference Croker and McDonald2005; Hornak et al., Reference Hornak, Rolls and Wade1996; Saunders et al., Reference Saunders, McDonald and Richardson2006), including reactivity to facial expressions of others (Angrilli et al., Reference Angrilli, Palomba, Cantagallo, Maietti and Stegagno1999; Blair & Cipolotti, Reference Blair and Cipolotti2000; Hopkins et al., Reference Hopkins, Dywan and Segalowitz2002). There is only very limited evidence, to date, that affective reactivity and the ability to accurately recognize emotions in others are related following TBI (Croker & McDonald, Reference Croker and McDonald2005; Hornak et al., Reference Hornak, Rolls and Wade1996). However, work with normal adults (e.g., McHugo & Smith, Reference McHugo and Smith1996; Wild et al., Reference Wild, Erb and Bartels2001) has suggested that the identification of and affective responses to emotional expressions are functionally intertwined and, furthermore, that emotional reactions to faces may provide an important cue to their identification (Wild et al., Reference Wild, Erb and Bartels2001).

In summary, it is apparent that a significant proportion of the TBI population demonstrates specific impairments in the perception of one or more types of nonverbal affective cues used in everyday social encounters. Although the nature of these deficits is still being clarified, evidence to date points to impairments across both visual and auditory modalities, as well as across static and dynamic media, in this population. Perception of negative emotions appears to be relatively more impaired than that of positive emotions. Overall, degrees of impairment across individuals appear to vary widely, as might be expected given the heterogeneous nature of this condition. There is additional evidence that emotional responsivity to facial expressions and other emotionally significant stimuli is also impaired.

REMEDIATION OF DEFICITS IN EMOTION PERCEPTION IN OTHER CLINICAL POPULATIONS

The prevalence of problems related to emotion perception in TBI necessitates effective treatment; however, to date there is little evidence regarding the efficacy of emotion perception training in people with TBI. In contrast, work on remediating emotion perception deficits in other clinical populations with similar cognitive profiles, namely autism, intellectual disability, and schizophrenia, has produced a growing body of evidence in this area. While clear differences exist between these groups and TBI, it is also the case that they share several characteristics that are relevant to treatment such as learning deficits, slowed processing and impaired executive functioning (Anderson, Reference Anderson2001; Calhoun, Reference Calhoun2006; Green et al., Reference Green, Kern, Braff and Mintz2000; Lezak, Reference Lezak1995; Russo et al., Reference Russo, Flanagan, Iarocci, Berringer, Zelazo and Burack2007; Welsh et al., Reference Welsh, Ahn and Placantonakis2005). This suggests that valuable insights might be gained from these remediation efforts. The first such insight is that treatment of this kind of deficit, at least in other populations, is feasible. In autism (Bolte et al., Reference Bolte, Feineis-Matthews, Leber, Dierks, Hubl and Poustka2002, Reference Bolte, Hubl, Feineis-Matthews, Prvulovic, Dierks and Poustka2006; Solomon et al., Reference Solomon, Goodlin-Jones and Anders2004) and intellectually disability (McAlpine et al., Reference McAlpine, Singh, Ellis, Kendall and Hampton1992; McKenzie et al., Reference McKenzie, Matheson, McKaskie, Hamilton and Murray2000; Rydin-Orwin et al., Reference Rydin-Orwin, Drake and Bratt1999), measurable improvement in emotion perception ability has been demonstrated across pre- and posttreatment in all published studies. In some instances, more widespread gains have also been noted. For example, McAlpine and colleagues (Reference McAlpine, Singh, Ellis, Kendall and Hampton1992), working with adults with intellectually disability, reported evidence of generalization from judgments based on photographs to judgments based on videotaped role plays and maintenance of skills for at least 9 months. Bolte and colleagues (Reference Bolte, Hubl, Feineis-Matthews, Prvulovic, Dierks and Poustka2006), working with individuals with autism, observed brain activation changes following treatment in brain regions associated with visuospatial and facial processing. These results are remarkable given the developmental and longstanding nature of these disorders, which are likely to have limited any awareness of emotional material from an early age (Hill et al., Reference Hill, Berthoz and Frith2004; Leonard et al., Reference Leonard, Msall, Bower, Tremont and Leonard2002; Rieffe et al., Reference Rieffe, Meerum Terwogt and Kotronopoulou2007).

The most extensive research in emotion perception remediation has been carried out in schizophrenia (Combs et al., Reference Combs, Tosheva, Wanner and Basso2006, Reference Combs, Adams, Penn, Roberts, Tiegreen and Stem2007; Frommann et al., Reference Frommann, Streit and Wolwer2003; Penn & Combs, Reference Penn and Combs2000; van der Gaag et al., Reference van der Gaag, Kern, van den Bosch and Liberman2002; Wolwer et al., Reference Wolwer, Frommann, Halfmann, Piaszek, Streit and Gaebel2005). Again, significant gains have been reported in all studies. Of particular note is that some of these studies incorporated strategies such as errorless learning, self-instruction, and direct positive reinforcement, which took into account the particular cognitive impairments presented by its target population (Frommann et al., Reference Frommann, Streit and Wolwer2003; van der Gaag et al., Reference van der Gaag, Kern, van den Bosch and Liberman2002; Wolwer et al., Reference Wolwer, Frommann, Halfmann, Piaszek, Streit and Gaebel2005). Several of these impairments, such as poor organization, and reduced learning and attention, are shared with the TBI population, which suggests that the techniques have the potential to be relevant to TBI. Indeed, many of these same techniques mentioned have been applied in the TBI population and shown to be effective for remediation of deficits in attention, memory, strategy use, problem solving, and self-regulation of behavior (Burke et al., Reference Burke, Zencius, Wesolowski and Doubleday1991; Dou et al., Reference Dou, Man, Ou, Zheng and Tam2006; Lawson & Rice, Reference Lawson and Rice1989; Melton & Bourgeois, Reference Melton and Bourgeois2005; Squires et al., Reference Squires, Hunkin and Parkin1997; Turkstra & Bourgeois, Reference Turkstra and Bourgeois2005; Webster & Scott, Reference Webster and Scott1983).

Despite these generally positive findings, a potential shortcoming of many of such studies (e.g., Bolte et al., Reference Bolte, Hubl, Feineis-Matthews, Prvulovic, Dierks and Poustka2006; Frommann et al., Reference Frommann, Streit and Wolwer2003; Penn & Combs, Reference Penn and Combs2000; Wolwer et al., Reference Wolwer, Frommann, Halfmann, Piaszek, Streit and Gaebel2005) is their use of photograph-based stimuli and assessment measures pertaining exclusively to facial affect. Given that emotional information derives from a wide range of sources (posture, gesture, voice, contextual factors, and facial expressions), this creates problems for generalization of learning to naturalistic social situations in which emotions present dynamically and by means of multimodal channels (or in the case of phone communication, by means of voice alone). Indeed, problems with generalization and maintenance of gains after training with only photographs have been found in several studies that have measured these factors (Bolte et al., Reference Bolte, Feineis-Matthews, Leber, Dierks, Hubl and Poustka2002; Penn & Combs, Reference Penn and Combs2000), although not in others (Combs et al., Reference Combs, Tosheva, Wanner and Basso2006; McAlpine et al., Reference McAlpine, Singh, Ellis, Kendall and Hampton1992). A minority of the studies used other stimuli in treatment, such as voice and body language, or videos incorporating these cue modalities (e.g., McKenzie et al., Reference McKenzie, Matheson, McKaskie, Hamilton and Murray2000; Rydin-Orwin et al., Reference Rydin-Orwin, Drake and Bratt1999; Solomon et al., Reference Solomon, Goodlin-Jones and Anders2004; van der Gaag et al., Reference van der Gaag, Kern, van den Bosch and Liberman2002). Of these, only Rydin-Orwin and colleagues used multimodal stimuli and assessment measures, and none assessed durability of treatment gains or generalization to measures of broader social functioning. Clearly, an important goal of remediation is an improvement in actual function, so a model remediation program must address the multimodal nature of emotions and the transfer of treatment gains to everyday social settings.

A second possible weakness of these studies is their focus on recognition of emotional stimuli (primarily facial expressions) without attention to appropriate ways of responding to emotional communication. As noted by McAlpine and colleagues (Reference McAlpine, Singh, Ellis, Kendall and Hampton1992), recognizing the emotional significance of stimuli does not necessarily lead to socially acceptable use of such information in social behavior. This suggests that emotion perception training should incorporate components aimed at improving self-regulation of social behavior in order for maximum benefit to be realized. Some effort in this direction has been made by Combs and colleagues (Reference Combs, Adams, Penn, Roberts, Tiegreen and Stem2007), who have developed a program for individuals with schizophrenia that encompasses both emotion perception training and training in using emotional information effectively within social interactions. Preliminary findings have been promising and included evidence of generalization of treatment gains to social functioning. In TBI, this area remains to be explored.

Despite the similarities among these disorders, it is important to note that individuals who have sustained a TBI in adulthood may differ in important respects from individuals with developmental disorders, who are likely to have had longstanding emotion processing deficits, in many cases, from early childhood. For adults with TBI sustained after normal development, some aspects of emotion perception may remain intact and semantic knowledge of emotions (social cognition) tends to be unaffected (Croker & McDonald, Reference Croker and McDonald2005). These relative strengths in ability, therefore, offer avenues that could be used to support training aimed at areas of deficit. For example, training aimed at improving facial expression, voice tone, and body language recognition may draw rich references from individuals' semantic awareness of emotions and past emotional experiences.

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL BASES OF EMOTION PERCEPTION DEFICITS IN TBI

Recent research on the mechanisms of emotion perception provides the opportunity for a more sophisticated conceptualization of emotion perception training and thus offers insights into how emotion perception remediation might be designed so as to maximize effectiveness. Specifically, current work into the neural underpinnings of emotion perception, combined with recent theorizing regarding the basis of remediation can help configure remediation approaches and treatment goals to effectively address emotion perception disorders following TBI within broader rehabilitation goals.

From the outset, it needs to be emphasized that the neuropsychological profile from which emotion processing deficits appear in TBI is a complicated one, due largely to the multifaceted nature of the injury (Tate, Reference Tate1987). There are few studies that have specifically examined neuropathological correlates of emotion processing disorders in TBI. On the other hand, a body of neuroimaging and lesion studies in animals and humans has accrued, which provides a general picture of the neural architecture of emotion processing. Several interacting subsystems are implicated primarily situated in the frontal and temporal regions. These systems involve the amygdala, anterior cingulate gyrus, insular, parietal and somatosensory cortices, and numerous structures within the frontal lobes. The roles of these separate structures appear to be complex and suggest interactions between interrelated structures and the temporal features of the perceptual processes as well as concomitant cognitive demands. The interested reader should refer to authoritative overviews on this topic (e.g., Adolphs, Reference Adolphs2002b; Adolphs & Damasio, Reference Adolphs, Damasio and Borod2000; Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Drevets, Rauch and Lane2003). To summarize the reported relationships: the amygdala has been associated with negative facial expressions and emotional prosody (Adolphs & Tranel, Reference Adolphs and Tranel2003, Reference Adolphs and Tranel2004; Adolphs et al., Reference Adolphs, Tranel, Damasio and Damasio1995, Reference Adolphs, Tranel and Damasio2001b; Cardinal et al., Reference Cardinal, Parkinson, Hall and Everitt2002; Critchley et al., Reference Critchley, Daly, Phillips, Brammer, Bullmore, Williams, Van Amelsvoort, Robertson, David and Murphy2000b; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Frith, Perrett, Rowland, Young, Calder and Dolan1996, Reference Morris, DeGelder, Weiskrantz and Dolan2001; Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Drevets, Rauch and Lane2003; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Young, Calder, Hellawell, Aggleton and Johnson1997; Stork & Pape, Reference Stork and Pape2002; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Menon, Eliez, Blasey, White, Reid, Gotlib and Reiss2002); the anterior cingulate cortex with facial and prosodic expressions (Holland & Sonderman, Reference Holland and Sonderman1974; Hornak et al., Reference Hornak, O'Doherty, Bramham, Rolls, Morris, Bullock and Polkey2004; Streit et al., Reference Streit, Ioannides, Liu, Wolwer, Dammers, Gross, Gaebel and Muller-Gartner1999); the anterior insula with both facial and prosodic expressions of disgust (Calder et al., Reference Calder, Keane, Manes, Antoun and Young2000, Reference Calder, Lawrence and Young2001, Reference Calder, Keane, Manly, Sprengelmeyer, Scott, Nimmo-Smith and Young2003; Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Drevets, Rauch and Lane2003); the right somatosensory cortex (Adolphs et al., Reference Adolphs, Damasio, Tranel and Damasio1996, Reference Adolphs, Damasio, Tranel, Cooper and Damasio2000) and the temporal visual cortex with facial expressions (Critchley et al., Reference Critchley, Daly, Phillips, Brammer, Bullmore, Williams, Van Amelsvoort, Robertson, David and Murphy2000b; Haxby et al., Reference Haxby, Hoffman and Gobbini2002; Streit et al., Reference Streit, Ioannides, Liu, Wolwer, Dammers, Gross, Gaebel and Muller-Gartner1999; Weddell, Reference Weddell1994); the middle temporal gyrus with bimodal emotional expressions (Pourtois et al., Reference Pourtois, de Gelder, Bol and Crommelinck2005); the right frontoparietal operculum, bilateral frontal pole, and middle right superior temporal sulcus with emotional prosody (Adolphs, Reference Adolphs2002b; Grandjean et al., Reference Grandjean, Sander, Pourtois, Schwartz, Seghier, Scherer and Vuilleumier2005; Pell, Reference Pell2006); and the orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortices, which together comprise the ventromedial prefrontal region, with facial and prosodic expressions (Barrash et al., Reference Barrash, Tranel and Anderson2000; Blair et al., Reference Blair, Morris, Frith, Perrett and Dolan1999; Cicerone & Tanenbaum, Reference Cicerone and Tanenbaum1997; Damasio, Reference Damasio1994; George et al., Reference George, Parekh, Rosinsky, Ketter, Kimbrell, Heilman, Herscovitch and Post1996; Hornak et al., Reference Hornak, Rolls and Wade1996, Reference Hornak, Bramham, Rolls, Morris, O'Doherty, Bullock and Polkey2003; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Scott and Dolan1999; Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Drevets, Rauch and Lane2003).

Extensive evidence has indicated that processing of emotional stimuli may be predominantly lateralized to the right hemisphere. This may especially be the case with processing of emotional prosody (Adolphs et al., Reference Adolphs, Damasio and Tranel2002; Adolphs & Tranel, Reference Adolphs and Tranel1999; George et al., Reference George, Parekh, Rosinsky, Ketter, Kimbrell, Heilman, Herscovitch and Post1996; Grimshaw et al., Reference Grimshaw, Kwasny, Covell and Johnson2003; Heilman et al., Reference Heilman, Bowers, Speedie and Coslett1984; Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, Elliott, Barry, Cruttenden and Woodruff2003; Pell, Reference Pell2006; Pell & Baum, Reference Pell and Baum1997; Pihan et al., Reference Pihan, Altenmuller, Hertrich and Ackermann2000), although some bilateral contribution has been found in a few studies (Adolphs et al., Reference Adolphs, Damasio and Tranel2002; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Scott and Dolan1999; Sander & Scheich, Reference Sander and Scheich2001). The issue of lateralization appears to be more complicated for recognition of facial expressions. Some reports are indicative of right hemispheric dominance in the processing of all emotions (Adolphs & Damasio, Reference Adolphs, Damasio and Borod2000; Borod, Reference Borod1993; Borod et al., Reference Borod, Cicero, Obler, Welkowitz, Erhan, Santschi, Grunwald, Agosti and Whalen1998; Rapcsak et al., Reference Rapcsak, Comer and Rubens1993), and others suggest hemispheric asymmetry in processing positive and negative emotions (Adolphs & Damasio, Reference Adolphs, Damasio and Borod2000; Adolphs et al., Reference Adolphs, Jansari and Tranel2001a,Reference Adolphs, Tranel and Damasiob; Silberman & Weingartner, Reference Silberman and Weingartner1986). In the case of the latter, that is, the “valence hypothesis,” evidence has been seen to favor either clear hemispheric biases toward processing positive (left) and negative (right) emotions (Silberman & Weingartner, Reference Silberman and Weingartner1986) or the possibility that positive emotions are processed bilaterally, whereas negative emotions are processed chiefly by the right hemisphere (Adolphs et al., Reference Adolphs, Jansari and Tranel2001a). Still other data have suggested that emotion processing performed by the left hemisphere is modality specific, whereas the right hemisphere appears to predominate in processing all emotional expressions across modalities (Kucharska-Pietura et al., Reference Kucharska-Pietura, Phillips, Gernand and David2003). Overall, although findings remain inconclusive (Critchley et al., Reference Critchley, Daly, Bullmore, Williams, Van Amelsvoort, Robertson, Rowe, Phillips, McAlonan, Howlin and Murphy2000a), most evidence appears at least suggestive of right hemispheric primacy in emotion processing (Adolphs et al., Reference Adolphs, Damasio, Tranel and Damasio1996, Reference Adolphs, Damasio and Tranel2002; Erhan et al., Reference Erhan, Borod, Tenke and Bruder1998).

The neural structures associated with emotion processing, whether lateralized or distributed bilaterally, may be particularly vulnerable to damage in TBI due to their anatomical location in the frontal and temporal lobes (Fontaine et al., Reference Fontaine, Azouvi, Remy, Bussel and Samson1999). Most of these structures are situated near the orbital surfaces of the brain, which are directly adjacent to common points of impact and numerous bony protuberances lining the interior of the skull. Rapid jolting of the brain within the skull cavity due to sudden impact, for example, in a motor-vehicle accident, the most common cause of severe TBI (Kahn, Reference Kahn1970; Kalsbeek, Reference Kalsbeek1980; Tate et al., Reference Tate, McDonald and Lulham1998), can thus lead to multifocal lesions concentrated in these sites as well as shearing of axonal connections with other systems (Adams et al., Reference Adams, Graham and Jennett2001; Besenski et al., Reference Besenski, Broz, Jadro-Santel, Pavi and Mikuli1996; Gaetz, Reference Gaetz2004). The high likelihood of this kind of damage in the majority of severe TBI cases is consistent with the now established finding that emotion perception difficulties are a common feature of this type of injury.

Numerous neuroanatomical models of emotion perception have been proposed (for other examples, see: Adolphs, Reference Adolphs2001, Reference Adolphs2002a,Reference Adolphsb; Adolphs et al., Reference Adolphs, Damasio and Tranel2002; George et al., Reference George, Ketter, Gill, Haxby, Ungerleider, Herscovitch and Post1993; Heberlein & Adolphs, Reference Heberlein, Adolphs, Easton and Emery2005). Perhaps the most comprehensive of these to date is that of Phillips and colleagues (Reference Phillips, Drevets, Rauch and Lane2003), whose model (summarized in Figure 1) incorporates processes for both the rapid appreciation of emotionally significant stimuli and processes for automatic and controlled responding to emotional input. Reviewing evidence from a range of human and animal research, these authors identify several neuroanatomical structures that potentially underpin key processes involved in emotion perception. With the exception of the sensorimotor region, which Phillips and colleagues do not mention, these structures are the same as those described earlier. Two neighboring and closely linked neural systems are indicated: a ventral system, including the amygdala, insula, and ventral regions of the anterior cingulate gyrus and prefrontal cortex, and a dorsal system, including the dorsal regions of the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate gyrus as well as the hippocampus. According to the model, the ventral system supports the rapid orientation to, appraisal, and identification of emotionally significant information as well as the production of affective responses even before consciousness, while the dorsal system is responsible for effortful processing of emotional stimuli, including the engagement of other cognitive processing such as language and memory, as well as the regulation of emotional behavior. These systems, although neuroanatomically separate, are not functionally independent, such that damage to either is likely to disturb emotion perception generally. Thus, the processes underpinned by the ventral system initiate activity in the dorsal system, and, conversely, the dorsal system can also inhibit or modulate ventral activity to facilitate the rapid processing of critical emotional information (e.g., threat) and to ensure that behavioral responses to what is perceived remain contextually appropriate.

Fig. 1. Model of the neuroanatomical correlates of three key processes in emotion perception (adapted from Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Drevets, Rauch and Lane2003). Anatomical structures within the ventral system include the amygdala, insula, and ventral regions of both the prefrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate gyrus. This system is thought to mediate both the production of emotional states and the identification of emotion-related stimuli. The dorsal system includes the dorsal regions of the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate gyrus, and the hippocampus. This system mediates the regulation of emotional states and behavior and can, as indicated by the negative and positive signs in the circles, modulate or inhibit the activity of the ventral system so that emotional states and behaviors are contextually appropriate.

A strength of this model is its configuration of the interrelationship between conceptually different processes governing emotion perception. The model suggests that interventions designed to treat emotion perception deficits may address several component processes of emotion: initial orientation and appraisal, affective responsivity, and strategic, cognitively mediated processing. Furthermore, as the system is highly interactive, improvements in one aspect of emotional processing may lead to improvements in others. One example of how this interaction may occur arises from a study of patients with focal frontal lesions reported by Damasio and colleagues (Reference Damasio, Tranel and Damasio1990). These patients failed to show skin conductance changes (arousal) when passively viewing emotionally charged material but demonstrated a normal response when they were required to describe the pictures. These findings suggest that the dorsal (effortful) system can stimulate the ventral to overcome an initial lack of orientation and responsivity to the emotional material. Although similar dissociations have not yet been reported in people with TBI, it is highly likely that some will experience relatively greater impairment in one process involved in emotion perception compared with another. According to this interactive model, such individuals may derive benefit from treatment targeting both systems.

Before leaving this discussion of the neuropsychology of emotion perception deficits in TBI, it is important to acknowledge that impairment of “nonsocial cognitive processes” (a term used by Adolphs and others in research on social cognition—Adolphs, Reference Adolphs1999, Reference Adolphs2003, Reference Adolphs, Cacioppo, Visser and Pickett2006; Brüne et al., Reference Brüne, Abdel-Hamid, Lehmkamper and Sonntag2007; Decety & Jackson, Reference Decety and Jackson2004; Satpute & Lieberman, Reference Satpute and Lieberman2006), such as attention, learning, information processing speed, cognitive flexibility, and awareness of deficits, are likely to contribute to the failure of those with TBI to perceive emotional cues accurately and efficiently (Lezak, Reference Lezak1995; McDonald, Reference McDonald2003; McDonald et al., Reference McDonald, Bornhofen, Shum, Long, Saunders and Neulinger2006; Prigatano, Reference Prigatano1999; Sohlberg & Mateer, Reference Sohlberg and Mateer2001; Trower, Reference Trower1980). There is some evidence for an association between executive function and emotion perception in TBI (Bornhofen & McDonald, in press; McDonald et al., Reference McDonald, Bornhofen, Shum, Long, Saunders and Neulinger2006; McDonald & Saunders, Reference McDonald and Saunders2005). Other types of dysfunction, such as slowed information processing and variable attention may compromise an individual's ability to track the continuous flow of data presented in social interactions. The impact of these factors may account for some of the variability seen in emotion perception competence among individuals with TBI noted earlier; however, no research to date has investigated whether remediating attention and/or executive deficits improves emotion perception ability in individuals with TBI. In the meantime, it appears clear that any intervention that is tailored to addressing emotion perception deficits in people with TBI must address these nonsocial deficits also either directly or in terms of modifying techniques to maximize the likelihood of success on remediation tasks.

A MODEL OF COGNITIVE REMEDIATION

Having identified major cognitive and affective processes that contribute to emotion perception, the next task is to consider whether contemporary models of remediation are relevant to emotion perception and/or whether there is sufficient evidence to consider them a credible basis for the development of treatment strategies in TBI. Before the 1990s, the prevailing view in neuroscience was that neural structures in the brain were fixed at maturity, and thus damage to neural systems after early childhood was regarded as permanent (Sohlberg & Mateer, Reference Sohlberg and Mateer2001). Over the past decade, however, research into mechanisms of functional recovery after brain injury has produced evidence to suggest that the potential for neuronal reorganization and regeneration following injury is much greater than previously believed (Cornelissen et al., Reference Cornelissen, Laine, Tarkiainen, Jarvensivu, Martin and Salmelin2003; Hallett, Reference Hallett2001; Keyvani & Schallert, Reference Keyvani and Schallert2002; Mohammed et al., Reference Mohammed, Zhu, Darmopil, Hjerling-Leffler, Ernfors, Winblad, Diamond, Eriksson and Bogdanovic2002).

Largely the product of research into motor and sensory recovery after deafferentation of limbs in animal models and stroke in humans, this evidence has indicated that the brain not only remains plastic throughout the lifespan, but it is, in fact, constantly changing as a result of experience (Merzenich & Jenkins, Reference Merzenich and Jenkins1993; Merzenich et al., Reference Merzenich, Nelson, Stryker, Cynader, Schoppmann and Zook1984). Functional (motor) recovery can be achieved by intensive, carefully targeted training and is associated with reorganization and expansion of relevant cortical areas (Liepert et al., Reference Liepert, Bauder, Wolfgang, Miltner, Taub and Weiller2000; Nudo & Milliken, Reference Nudo and Milliken1996; Nudo et al., Reference Nudo, Milliken, Jenkins and Merzenich1996a,Reference Nudo, Wise, SiFuentes and Millikenb). The critical therapeutic factor in such remediation appears to be a combination of attention and carefully targeted, graduated practice (Taub et al., Reference Taub, Crago and Uswatte1998; Taub & Wolf, Reference Taub and Wolf1997). Beyond applications to motor recovery, this kind of approach has been used with cognitive-perceptual impairments, including aphasia (Cornelissen et al., Reference Cornelissen, Laine, Tarkiainen, Jarvensivu, Martin and Salmelin2003; Fridriksson et al., Reference Fridriksson, Morrow-Odom, Moser, Fridriksson and Baylis2006; Pulvermuller et al., Reference Pulvermuller, Neininger, Elbert, Mohr, Rockstroh, Koebbel and Taub2001), cortical blindness (Pleger et al., Reference Pleger, Foerster, Widdig, Henschel, Nicolas, Jansen, Frank, Knecht, Schwenkreis and Tegenthoff2003), sustained attention (Longoni et al., Reference Longoni, Sturm, Weis, Holtel, Specht, Herzog and Willmes2000), unilateral neglect (Pizzamiglio et al., Reference Pizzamiglio, Perani, Cappa, Vallar, Paolucci, Grassi, Paulesu and Fazio1998), and dyslexia in children (Temple et al., Reference Temple, Deutsch, Poldrack, Miller, Tallal, Merzenich and Gabrieli2003). In each case, improvement in function has been associated with use-dependent expansion of cortical areas subserving those processes (Cornelissen et al., Reference Cornelissen, Laine, Tarkiainen, Jarvensivu, Martin and Salmelin2003; Fridriksson et al., Reference Fridriksson, Morrow-Odom, Moser, Fridriksson and Baylis2006; Longoni et al., Reference Longoni, Sturm, Weis, Holtel, Specht, Herzog and Willmes2000; Pleger et al., Reference Pleger, Foerster, Widdig, Henschel, Nicolas, Jansen, Frank, Knecht, Schwenkreis and Tegenthoff2003; Temple et al., Reference Temple, Deutsch, Poldrack, Miller, Tallal, Merzenich and Gabrieli2003).

The neurological underpinnings of these functional gains have been conceptualized in terms of a Hebbian learning framework (Keyvani & Schallert, Reference Keyvani and Schallert2002; Mateer & Kerns, Reference Mateer and Kerns2000; Robertson & Murre, Reference Robertson and Murre1999), whereby the strength and density of synaptic connections within a functional circuit change significantly through repeated, synchronous activation of pre- and postsynaptic neurons. At the neural level, “learning” occurs as synaptic connections are forged (i.e., through dendritic and axonal branching) and strengthened by experience, according to the Hebbian principle that “cells that fire together, wire together.” In practical terms, this suggests that repeated practice of a functional activity is likely to strengthen the neural correlates of that activity, whereas disuse or loss of stimulation may eventually lead to loss of function (Fitzsimonds & Poo, Reference Fitzsimonds and Poo1998; Kayser & Miller, Reference Kayser and Miller2002; Kolb, Reference Kolb1999; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Jia and Li2003; Rosenzweig, Reference Rosenzweig1999).

Building on Hebb's central principle of activity-dependent synaptic activity, Robertson and Murre (Reference Robertson and Murre1999) have argued that a neural network which has been partially damaged by a lesion should be amenable to reconnection if the process subserved by the network is repeatedly activated through precisely targeted experience enabling connectivity to be reinstated. According to Robertson and Murre's proposed model, it is critical that the target process, in this case, emotion recognition, is precisely and repeatedly activated by means of either bottom-up (i.e., stimulus-bound) or top-down (attention-enhancing) techniques (Robertson & Murre, Reference Robertson and Murre1999). Precise targeting is required, as evidence suggests that an impaired system may be dominated, masked or suppressed by the activity of other systems which remain intact (Kapur, Reference Kapur1996; Robertson & Murre, Reference Robertson and Murre1999; Vuilleumier et al., Reference Vuilleumier, Hester, Assal and Regli1996). Care must be taken to minimize or eliminate stimulation that is inaccurately or too-broadly defined, because attention directed to such stimuli may foster maladaptive or faulty connections within the impaired system.

Robertson and Murre's model seems well matched to the task of emotion perception remediation for two related reasons. First, given the relatively localized systems in the frontotemporal and somatosensory areas that appear to underpin emotion processing, it seems plausible that a Hebbian mechanism could support the restoration of emotion perception if activation during remediation was precisely targeted. Second, the bottom-up and top-down techniques described by Robertson and Murre fit well with the model of ventral and dorsal affective processes outlined by Phillips and colleagues (Reference Phillips, Drevets, Rauch and Lane2003) and may furnish the means by with both types of processes could be targeted. Specifically, bottom-up techniques which use external cues could potentially activate processes mediated by the ventral system. Examples might include repetitive practice of orientation to important emotion cues (e.g., the eyes and mouth in facial expressions) and stimulus discrimination. Top-down strategies may activate the dorsal system by reinforcing the use of sustained and selective attention and by supporting self-regulation of behavior. Examples of these strategies include cued rehearsal, mental rehearsal and self-monitoring procedures such as self-cueing with structured questions. Both approaches are eminently suited to the direct retraining of emotion perception, as the stimuli can be specific and easily exaggerated, and discriminative features easily highlighted and described. Thus, abundant opportunities for graduated practice of the process/es can be provided while minimizing maladaptive influences on gains achieved (Robertson & Murre, Reference Robertson and Murre1999). In summary, a program should entail both training in orienting to basic features of emotional cues and repetition (bottom-up strategies), especially in the early stages of the program, plus training in self-regulation of attention and responses to emotional stimuli (top-down strategies), especially toward the latter half of the program once basic ability to discriminate between key features has been established. Use of both approaches is likely to maximize benefit across all components of emotion perception in a way that supports independence of the trainee and thereby increases the likelihood that improvements will be sustained.

According to Robertson and Murre's position, there may also be instances in which damage to the emotion processing system is so severe as to make restoration of function impossible. In such cases remediation targeting impairment would not likely be successful, and compensatory training aimed at minimizing the impact of deficits would be more appropriate. Such training could, for example, engage support persons to provide structured verbal cues to assist in appropriate responding within social contexts.

CONTEXTUALIZED APPROACHES

While such a theoretical account of remediation as applied to emotion perception deficits seems plausible, whether or not it is suitable for people with TBI remains questionable. Robertson and Murre's (Reference Robertson and Murre1999) model of rehabilitation, based largely on findings from cognitive neuroscience derives primarily from research on focal injury. As such, it has been heavily criticized as a model for guiding treatment in brain injury cases whose profiles reflect more widespread, multifocal damage. Wilson (Reference Wilson2005) argued that a broader-based approach to rehabilitation was necessary for the provision of effective support and treatment of rehabilitation recipients, many of whom have multifaceted needs. According to this position, decisions as to whether restitution- or compensation-oriented rehabilitation is more appropriate for any one individual cannot reliably or practically be done on the basis of the extent of system damage, as imaging findings may often be unavailable or inconclusive and behavioral observations before therapy unreliable for this purpose. This may be particularly true for emotion recognition deficits. According to the interactive model proposed by Phillips et al. and others, superficially similar deficits in emotion recognition may reflect damage to numerous systems. Thus, with the current state of knowledge, it would be difficult to assess the extent of system damage on the basis of behavioral observation or imaging results. Such a situation may not actually represent a huge obstacle, as a range of remediation techniques could potentially be used to target several key emotion processes (e.g., appraisal, arousal/responsivity, and regulation of emotional behavior) simultaneously, which may provide convergent activation. Notwithstanding, Wilson has argued that in the vast majority of rehabilitation settings, decisions concerning which approach to use are best made on the basis of individual response to restitutional strategies. She concluded that rehabilitation should be guided by multiple models, including models of emotion, behavior, learning, and neuroscience, as no single model or theory now available is able to address the full range of needs experienced by rehabilitation recipients and their families within the environments in which they live.

Another important factor to consider is the need for remediation to be contextualized. As emphasized by Wilson (Reference Wilson2005) and others (Prigatano, Reference Prigatano1999; Sohlberg & Mateer, Reference Sohlberg and Mateer2001; Turkstra, Reference Turkstra2001; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Evans and Keohane2002; Ylvisaker et al., Reference Ylvisaker, Jacobs and Feeney2003), rehabilitation following TBI takes place amidst an array of social, institutional, medical, and personality factors that must be taken into consideration for a program to be successful in providing both short-term and long-term benefits for any one individual. Just as a major shortcoming of emotion remediation research in schizophrenia has been its failure to address emotion deficits in context, so too the diverse interplay of these factors has often been overlooked in TBI remediation in the past, to the detriment of the long-term outcome for individuals and their families (Turkstra, Reference Turkstra2001; Wilson, Reference Wilson2002, Reference Wilson2005; Ylvisaker & Feeney, Reference Ylvisaker and Feeney1998, Reference Ylvisaker and Feeney2000; Ylvisaker et al., Reference Ylvisaker, Hanks and Johnson-Greene2002). A context-sensitive approach to rehabilitation, in contrast, makes clear and explicit the relevance of a rehabilitation program to the individual's personal goals, and aims to highlight the personal significance of gains achieved throughout the course of treatment (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Evans and Keohane2002; Ylvisaker & Feeney, Reference Ylvisaker and Feeney1998). It, therefore, serves to enhance motivation as well as facilitate transfer of newly learned skills to the treatment recipient's day to day environment. In addition, directly linking remediation goals to the individual's context may capitalize on procedural/implicit learning processes, which tend to be context-specific, and remain relatively more intact following TBI than explicit learning mechanisms (Ylvisaker & Feeney, Reference Ylvisaker and Feeney1998).

In the realm of emotion perception remediation, there is good reason to believe that a contextualized approach could be compatible with focused attention techniques as advocated by Robertson and Murre (Reference Robertson and Murre1999). This is so because the inherent social nature of emotion processing, which readily supports repeated practice of emotional perception skills across a variety of social interactions (i.e., therapeutic, domestic, commercial, etc.), provides the clinician with abundant opportunities to demonstrate relevance. For example, education regarding the fundamental role of emotion perception in social life, discussion of real-life examples in which an individual may have misperceived another's intentions or mood, and role play of inappropriate versus appropriate responding to emotional cues may all be used to engender greater awareness of the function of emotion perception skills. Once a working understanding of the importance of these skills has been established, this understanding may be further enhanced in the course of a specifically targeted training program. Role-plays within session and practice tasks for completion outside of sessions (ideally with caregivers or primary support figures) can repeatedly highlight and reinforce the significance of emotional cues in any interaction with others (e.g., family, friends, or members of the community). All aspects of treatment, including emotion stimuli and therapy activities, can be made relevant to the individual's typical social exchanges and aimed at strengthening skills that can directly be applied to improve functioning in the everyday environment.

Finally, the inclusion of a full range of emotion stimuli within training, spanning visual, auditory and audiovisual modalities, rather than only still photographs, is likely to support holistic encoding of naturalistic emotion displays. Using static and dynamic stimuli as well as role play interaction activities could provide rich opportunities for developing skills necessary for efficient and accurate interpretation of emotion cues in a social context. Extending practice of these skills by means of homework tasks to be completed in the individual's home and community environment will further support transfer of learning to these settings. If these considerations were met, it could be anticipated that the overall benefits of treatment would include increased maintenance and generalization of newly acquired skills to the individual's day-to-day setting, particularly when supported by liaison with family and other support persons. Thus, it is likely that a contextualized approach to emotion perception training would complement an approach based on the neuroscientific model of rehabilitation described by Robertson and Murre (Reference Robertson and Murre1999). Used in combination, they may broaden the benefits to individuals with TBI which could otherwise be gained from restitution strategies alone.

Unfortunately, little research investigating emotion perception training in people with TBI has thus far been carried out. The authors have published two small studies (Bornhofen & McDonald, Reference Bornhofen and McDonald2008a, Reference Bornhofen and McDonald2008b) evaluating remediation programs that encompass both the notion of directed attention and repeated activation as well as the use of contextually relevant activities. Results indicated gains in both cases, but highlighted the complex nature of emotion perception disorders in people with chronic TBI. As foreshadowed by Wilson's work, deficits in emotion perception sat amidst highly variable constellations of other cognitive deficits and circumstantial issues that both limited and defined the rehabilitation process. The fact that improvements were found is encouraging, and provides an impetus for further research in this emerging field. But it is clear, that there is a long way to go in understanding the nature of these critical deficits and the best approach to their amelioration.

CONCLUSION

In this review we have laid out the evidence to date regarding the neuropsychological mechanisms of emotion perception disorders in TBI and recent theoretical discussions concerning the potential basis for remediation. The fact that contemporary remediation theory is highly relevant to emotion perception suggests that specific treatment approaches for emotion perception disorders with the TBI population are feasible. This combined with the positive findings of treatment efficacy seen in other clinical populations and the amenability of emotion perception training to both targeted practice and a contextualized approach gives scope for optimism with regard to treatment gains in the TBI group. But there is much yet to be learned regarding the optimal approach for individuals with unique constellations of deficits and how techniques can be designed to ensure maximal generalization and actual social benefit. Given the central importance of emotional communication in our daily lives, this area offers potentially unique benefits for those treated and their families and a compelling area for future research.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The information contained in this manuscript is based on parts of a PhD thesis completed by the first author and has not previously been published. This research was facilitated by a project grant from the National Medical and Research Council of Australia

References

REFERENCES

Adams, J.H., Graham, D.I., & Jennett, B. (2001). The structural basis of moderate disability after traumatic brain damage. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 71, 521524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Addington, J., Saeedi, H., & Addington, D. (2006). Facial affect recognition: A mediator between cognitive and social functioning in psychosis? Schizophrenia Research, 85, 142150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R. (1999). Social cognition and the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 469479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R. (2001). The neurobiology of social cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11, 231239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R. (2002a). Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12, 169177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R. (2002b). Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: Psychological and neurological mechanisms. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 1, 2162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R. (2003). Investigating the cognitive neuroscience of social behavior. Neuropsychologia, 41, 119126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R. (2006). What is special about social cognition? In Cacioppo, J.T., Visser, P.S., & Pickett, C.L. (Eds.), Social Neuroscience: People Thinking About Thinking People (pp. 269285). Boston: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R. & Damasio, A.R. (2000). Neurobiology of emotion at a systems level. In Borod, J.C.E. (Ed.), The Neuropsychology of Emotion (pp. 194213). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., & Tranel, D. (2002). Neural systems for recognition of emotional prosody: A 3-D lesion study. Emotion, 2, 2351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., Cooper, G., & Damasio, A.R. (2000). A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 26832690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A.R. (1996). Cortical systems for the recognition of emotion in facial expressions. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 76787687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R., Jansari, A., & Tranel, D. (2001a). Hemispheric perception of emotional valence from facial expressions. Neuropsychology, 15, 516524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R. & Tranel, D. (1999). Intact recognition of emotional prosody following amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia, 37, 12851292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R. & Tranel, D. (2003). Amygdala damage impairs emotion recognition from scenes only when they contain facial expressions. Neuropsychologia, 41, 12811289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R. & Tranel, D. (2004). Impaired judgments of sadness but not happiness following bilateral amygdala damage. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 453462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2001b). Emotion recognition from faces and prosody following temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychology, 15, 396404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A.R. (1995). Fear and the human amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 58795891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aigner, M., Sachs, G., Bruckmuller, E., Winklbaur, B., Zitterl, W., Kryspin-Exner, I., Gur, R., & Katschnig, H. (2007). Cognitive and emotion recognition deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 149, 121128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allerdings, M.D. & Alfano, D.P. (2006). Neuropsychological correlates of impaired emotion recognition following traumatic brain injury. Brain & Cognition, 60, 193194.Google ScholarPubMed
Anderson, M. (2001). Annotation: Conceptions of intelligence. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 42, 287298.Google ScholarPubMed
Angrilli, A., Palomba, D., Cantagallo, A., Maietti, A., & Stegagno, L. (1999). Emotional impairment after right orbitofrontal lesion in a patient without cognitive deficits. Neuroreport, 10, 17411746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrash, J., Tranel, D., & Anderson, S.W. (2000). Acquired personality disturbances associated with bilateral damage to the ventromedial prefrontal region. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18, 355381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Besenski, N., Broz, R., Jadro-Santel, D., Pavi, D., & Mikuli, D. (1996). The course of the traumatising force in acceleration head injury: CT evidence. Neuroradiology, 38(Suppl. 1), S36S41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bibby, H. & McDonald, S. (2005). Theory of mind after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 43, 99114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biddle, K.R., McCabe, A., & Bliss, L.S. (1996). Narrative skills following traumatic brain injury in children and adults. Journal of Communication Disorders, 29, 446469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R.J. & Cipolotti, L. (2000). Impaired social response reversal: A case of “acquired sociopathy”. Brain, 123, 11221141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R.J., Morris, J.S., Frith, C.C., Perrett, D.I., & Dolan, R.J. (1999). Dissociable neural responses to facial expressions of sadness and anger. Brain, 122, 883893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boice, R. (1983). Observation skills. Psychological Bulletin, 93, 329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolte, S., Feineis-Matthews, S., Leber, S., Dierks, T., Hubl, D., & Poustka, F. (2002). The development and evaluation of a computer-based program to test and to teach the recognition of facial affect. International Journal of Circumpolar Health, 61(Suppl. 2), 6168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolte, S., Hubl, D., Feineis-Matthews, S., Prvulovic, D., Dierks, T., & Poustka, F. (2006). Facial affect recognition training in autism: Can we animate the fusiform gyrus? Behavioral Neuroscience, 120, 211216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolte, S. & Poustka, F. (2003). The recognition of facial affect in autistic and schizophrenic subjects and their first-degree relatives. Psychological Medicine, 33, 907915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bond, F. & Godfrey, H.P. (1997). Conversation with traumatically brain-injured individuals: A controlled study of behavioural changes and their impact. Brain Injury, 11, 319329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boraston, Z., Blakemore, S.J., Chilvers, R., & Skuse, D. (2007). Impaired sadness recognition is linked to social interaction deficit in autism. Neuropsychologia, 45, 15011510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornhofen, C. & McDonald, S. (2008a). Treating emotion perception deficits following traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 18, 2244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornhofen, C. & McDonald, S. (2008b). Comparing strategies for treating emotion perception deficits in traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 23, 103115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borod, J.C. (1993). Cerebral mechanisms underlying facial, prosodic and lexical emotional expression: A review of neuropsychological studies and methodological issues. Neuropsychology, 7, 445463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borod, J.C., Cicero, B.A., Obler, L.K., Welkowitz, J., Erhan, H.M., Santschi, C., Grunwald, I.S., Agosti, R.M., & Whalen, J.R. (1998). Right hemisphere emotional perception: Evidence across multiple channels. Neuropsychology, 12, 446458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brüne, M., Abdel-Hamid, M., Lehmkamper, C., & Sonntag, C. (2007). Mental state attribution, neurocognitive functioning, and psychopathology: What predicts poor social competence in schizophrenia best? Schizophrenia Research, 92, 151159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, W.H., Zencius, A.H., Wesolowski, M.D., & Doubleday, F. (1991). Improving executive function disorders in brain-injured clients. Brain Injury, 5, 241252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burleigh, S.A., Farber, R.S., & Gillard, M. (1998). Community integration and life satisfaction after traumatic brain injury: Long-term findings. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 52, 4552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calder, A.J., Keane, J., Manes, F., Antoun, N., & Young, A.W. (2000). Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 10771078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calder, A.J., Keane, J., Manly, T., Sprengelmeyer, R., Scott, S., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Young, A.W. (2003). Facial expression recognition across the adult life span. Neuropsychologia, 41, 195202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calder, A.J., Lawrence, A.D., & Young, A.W. (2001). Neuropsychology of fear and loathing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 352363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calhoun, J.A. (2006). Executive functions: A discussion of the issues facing children with autism spectrum disorders and related disorders. Seminars in Speech & Language, 27, 6072.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardinal, R.N., Parkinson, J.A., Hall, J., & Everitt, B.J. (2002). Emotion and motivation: The role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 321352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, S.B. (1997). Cognitive-communication abilities in children with closed head injury. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 6, 5058.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, S.B., Levin, H.S., Matejka, J., Harward, H.N., & Kufera, J. (1995). Discourse ability in head injured children: Consideration of linguistic, psychosocial, and cognitive factors. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 10, 3654.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, S.B., Watkins, R., Gustafson, C., Moore, S., Levin, H.S., & Kufera, J.A. (1997). Narrative discourse in children with closed head injury, children with language impairment, and typically developing children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 6, 6676.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicerone, K.D. & Tanenbaum, L.N. (1997). Disturbance of social cognition after traumatic orbitofrontal brain injury. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 12, 173188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Combs, D.R., Adams, S.D., Penn, D.L., Roberts, D., Tiegreen, J., & Stem, P. (2007). Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for inpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: Preliminary findings. Schizophrenia Research, 91, 112116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Combs, D.R., Tosheva, A., Wanner, J., & Basso, M.R. (2006). Remediation of emotion perception deficits in schizophrenia: The use of attentional prompts. Schizophrenia Research, 87, 340341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornelissen, K., Laine, M., Tarkiainen, A., Jarvensivu, T., Martin, N., & Salmelin, R. (2003). Adult brain plasticity elicited by anomia treatment. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 444461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Critchley, H., Daly, E.M., Bullmore, E.T., Williams, S.C., Van Amelsvoort, T., Robertson, D.M., Rowe, A., Phillips, M., McAlonan, G., Howlin, P., & Murphy, D.G. (2000a). The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour: Changes in cerebral blood flow when people with autistic disorder process facial expressions. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 123, 22032212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Critchley, H., Daly, E., Phillips, M., Brammer, M., Bullmore, E., Williams, S., Van Amelsvoort, T., Robertson, D., David, A., & Murphy, D. (2000b). Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Human Brain Mapping, 9, 93105.3.0.CO;2-Z>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Croker, V. & McDonald, S. (2005). Recognition of emotion from facial expression following traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 19, 787789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. London: Papermac.Google Scholar
Damasio, A.R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1990). Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behavioural Brain Research, 41, 8194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J. & Jackson, P.L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral & Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 71100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dombovy, M.L. & Olek, A.C. (1997). Recovery and rehabilitation following traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 11, 305318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dou, Z.L., Man, D.W.K., Ou, H.N., Zheng, J.L., & Tam, S.F. (2006). Computerized errorless learning-based memory rehabilitation for Chinese patients with brain injury: A preliminary quasi-experimental clinical design study. Brain Injury, 20, 219225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durgin, C.J. (2000). Increasing community participation after brain injury: Strategies for identifying and reducing the risks. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 15, 11951207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, J., Jackson, H.J., & Pattison, P.E. (2002). Emotion recognition via facial expression and affective prosody in schizophrenia: A methodological review. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 789832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elsass, L. & Kinsella, G. (1987). Social interaction following severe closed head injury. Psychological Medicine, 17, 6778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erhan, H., Borod, J.C., Tenke, C.E., & Bruder, G.E. (1998). Identification of emotion in a dichotic listening task: Event-related brain potential and behavioral findings. Brain and Cognition, 37, 286307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eslinger, P.J., Grattan, L.M., & Geder, L. (1995). Implications of frontal lobe lesions on rehabilitation and recovery from acute brain injury. NeuroRehabilitation, 5, 161182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitzsimonds, R.M. & Poo, M.M. (1998). Retrograde signaling in the development and modification of synapses. Physiological Reviews, 78, 143170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fontaine, A., Azouvi, P., Remy, P., Bussel, B., & Samson, Y. (1999). Functional anatomy of neuropsychological deficits after severe traumatic brain injury. Neurology, 53, 19631968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fridriksson, J., Morrow-Odom, L., Moser, D., Fridriksson, A., & Baylis, G. (2006). Neural recruitment associated with anomia treatment in aphasia. Neuroimage, 32, 14031412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frommann, N., Streit, M., & Wolwer, W. (2003). Remediation of facial affect recognition impairments in patients with schizophrenia: A new training program. Psychiatry Research, 117, 281284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaetz, M. (2004). The neurophysiology of brain injury. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115, 418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
George, M.S., Ketter, T.A., Gill, D.S., Haxby, J.V., Ungerleider, L., Herscovitch, P., & Post, R.M. (1993). Brain regions involved in recognizing facial emotion or identity: An oxygen-15 PET study. Journal of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical Neurosciences, 5, 384394.Google ScholarPubMed
George, M.S., Parekh, P.I., Rosinsky, N., Ketter, T.A., Kimbrell, T.A., Heilman, K.M., Herscovitch, P., & Post, R.M. (1996). Understanding emotional prosody activates right hemisphere regions. Archives of Neurology, 53, 665670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Godfrey, H.P., Knight, R.G., & Bishara, S.N. (1991). The relationship between social skill and family problem-solving following very severe closed head injury. Brain Injury, 5, 207211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Godfrey, H.P. & Shum, D. (2000). Executive functioning and the application of social skills following traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology, 14, 443444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godfrey, H.P.D., Knight, R.G., & Partridge, F.M. (1996). Emotional adjustment following traumatic brain injury: A stress-appraisal-coping formulation. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 6, 2940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomez-Hernandez, R., Max, J.E., Kosier, T., Paradiso, S., & Robinson, R.G. (1997). Social impairment and depression after traumatic brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine Rehabilitation, 78, 13211326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gosling, J. & Oddy, M. (1999). Rearranged marriages: Marital relationships after head injury. Brain Injury, 13, 785796.Google ScholarPubMed
Grandjean, D., Sander, D., Pourtois, G., Schwartz, S., Seghier, M.L., Scherer, K.R., & Vuilleumier, P. (2005). The voices of wrath: Brain responses to angry prosody in meaningless speech. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 145146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grattan, L.M. & Ghahramanlou, M. (2002). The rehabilitation of neurologically based social disturbances. In Eslinger, P.J. (Ed.), Neuropsychological Interventions: Clinical Research and Practice (pp. 266293). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Green, M.F., Olivier, B., Crawley, J.N., Penn, D.L., & Silverstein, S. (2005). Social cognition in schizophrenia: Recommendations from the measurement and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia new approaches conference. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31, 882887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M., Kern, R., Braff, D., & Mintz, J. (2000). Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: Are we measuring the “right stuff”? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26, 119136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, R.E., Turner, G.R., & Thompson, W.F. (2004). Deficits in facial emotion perception in adults with recent traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 42, 133141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimshaw, G.M., Kwasny, K.M., Covell, E., & Johnson, R.A. (2003). The dynamic nature of language lateralization: Effects of lexical and prosodic factors. Neuropsychologia, 41, 10081019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallett, J.D., Zasler, N.D., Maurer, P., & Cash, S. (1994). Role change after traumatic brain injury in adults. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 48, 241246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallett, M. (2001). Plasticity of the human motor cortex and recovery from stroke. Brain Research—Brain Research Reviews, 36, 169174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammond, F.M., Hart, T., Bushnik, T., Corrigan, J.D., & Sasser, H. (2004). Change and predictors of change in communication, cognition, and social function between 1 and 5 years after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 19, 314328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haxby, J.V., Hoffman, E.A., & Gobbini, M.I. (2002). Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 5967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heberlein, A.S. & Adolphs, R. (2005). Functional anatomy of human social cognition. In Easton, A. & Emery, N.J. (Eds.), The Cognitive Neuroscience of Social Behaviour (pp. 157194). New York: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heilman, K.M., Bowers, D., Speedie, L., & Coslett, H.B. (1984). Comprehension of affective and nonaffective prosody. Neurology, 34, 917921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, E., Berthoz, S., & Frith, U. (2004). Brief report: Cognitive processing of own emotions in individuals with autistic spectrum disorder and in their relatives. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 34, 229235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holland, A.L. & Sonderman, J.C. (1974). Effects of a program based on the token test for teaching comprehension skills to aphasics. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 17, 589598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoofien, D., Gilboa, A., Vakil, E., & Donovick, P.J. (2001). Traumatic brain injury (TBI) 10–20 years later: A comprehensive outcome study of psychiatric symptomatology, cognitive abilities and psychosocial functioning. Brain Injury, 15, 189209.Google Scholar
Hooker, C. & Park, S. (2002). Emotion processing and its relationship to social functioning in schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Research, 112, 4150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopkins, M.J., Dywan, J., & Segalowitz, S.J. (2002). Altered electrodermal response to facial expression after closed head injury. Brain Injury, 16, 245257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hornak, J., Bramham, J., Rolls, E., Morris, R., O'Doherty, J., Bullock, P., & Polkey, C. (2003). Changes in emotion after circumscribed surgical lesions of the orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 126, 16911712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hornak, J., O'Doherty, J., Bramham, J., Rolls, E., Morris, R., Bullock, P., & Polkey, C. (2004). Reward-related reversal learning after surgical excisions in orbito-frontal or dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 463478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hornak, J., Rolls, E., & Wade, D. (1996). Face and voice expression identification in patients with emotional and behavioural changes following ventral frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia, 34, 247261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, K., Minshew, N., Leonard, G.L., & Behrmann, M. (2007). A fine-grained analysis of facial expression processing in high-functioning adults with autism. Neuropsychologia, 45, 685695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, H.F. & Moffat, N.J. (1987). Impaired emotional recognition following severe head injury. Cortex, 23, 293300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahn, M. (1970). Non-verbal communication and marital satisfaction. Family Processes, 9, 449456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalsbeek, W.D. (1980). The national head and spinal cord injury survey: Major findings. Journal of Neurosurgery, 53, S19S31.Google Scholar
Kapur, N. (1996). Paradoxical functional facilitation in brain-behaviour research: A critical review. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 119, 17751790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kats-Gold, I., Besser, A., & Priel, B. (2007). The role of simple emotion recognition skills among school aged boys at risk of ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 363378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kayser, A.S. & Miller, K.D. (2002). Opponent inhibition: A developmental model of layer 4 of the neocortical circuit. Neuron, 33, 131142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kee, K.S., Green, M.F., Mintz, J., & Brekke, J.S. (2003). Is emotion processing a predictor of functional outcome in schizophrenia? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 29, 487497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kersel, D.A., Marsh, N.V., Havill, J.H., & Sleigh, J.W. (2001). Psychosocial functioning during the year following severe traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 15, 683696.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, H., Roth, J., von Wietersheim, J., Deighton, R.M., & Traue, H.C. (2007). Emotion recognition patterns in patients with panic disorder. Depression & Anxiety, 24, 223226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keyvani, K. & Schallert, T. (2002). Plasticity-associated molecular and structural events in the injured brain. Journal of Neuropathology & Experimental Neurology, 61, 831840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohler, C.G., Bilker, W., Hagendoorn, M., Gur, R.E., & Gur, R.C. (2000). Emotion recognition deficit in schizophrenia: Association with symptomatology and cognition. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 127136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolb, B. (1999). Synaptic plasticity and the organization of behaviour after early and late brain injury. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 6276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kucharska-Pietura, K., Phillips, M.L., Gernand, W., & David, A.S. (2003). Perception of emotions from faces and voices following unilateral brain damage. Neuropsychologia, 41, 10821090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawson, M.J. & Rice, D.N. (1989). Effects of training in use of executive strategies on a verbal memory problem resulting from closed head injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Psychology, 11, 842854.Google ScholarPubMed
Leonard, S., Msall, M., Bower, C., Tremont, M., & Leonard, H. (2002). Functional status of school-aged children with Down syndrome. Journal of Paediatrics & Child Health, 38, 160165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leppanen, J.M. & Hietanen, J.K. (2001). Emotion recognition and social adjustment in school-aged girls and boys. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42, 429435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lezak, M.D. (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment (3rd ed.) (Vol. xviii). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Liepert, J., Bauder, H., Wolfgang, H.R., Miltner, W.H., Taub, E., & Weiller, C. (2000). Treatment-induced cortical reorganization after stroke in humans. Stroke, 31, 12101216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindner, J.L. & Rosen, L.A. (2006). Decoding of emotion through facial expression, prosody and verbal content in children and adolescents with Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 36, 769777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Longoni, F., Sturm, W., Weis, S., Holtel, C., Specht, K., Herzog, H., & Willmes, K. (2000). Functional reorganization after training of alertness in two patients with right-hemisphere lesions. Zeitschrift fur Neuropsychologie, 11, 250261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandal, M.K., Pandey, R., & Prasad, A.B. (1998). Facial expressions of emotions and schizophrenia: A review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24, 399412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, N.V. (1999). Social skill deficits following traumatic brain injury: Assessment and treatment. In McDonald, S., Togher, L., & Code, C. (Eds.), Communication Disorders Following Traumatic Brain Injury (pp. 175210). England: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis (UK).Google Scholar
Mateer, C.A. & Kerns, K.A. (2000). Capitalizing on neuroplasticity. Brain and Cognition, 42, 106109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazefsky, C.A. & Oswald, D.P. (2007). Emotion perception in Asperger's syndrome and high-functioning autism: The importance of diagnostic criteria and cue intensity. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 37, 10861095.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAlpine, C., Singh, N.N., Ellis, C.R., Kendall, K.A., & Hampton, C. (1992). Enhancing the ability of adults with mental retardation to recognize facial expressions of emotion. Behavior Modification, 16, 559573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, S. (1992). Communication disorders following closed head injury: New approaches to assessment and rehabilitation. Brain Injury, 6, 283292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, S. (1993). Pragmatic language skills after closed head injury: Ability to meet the informational needs of the listener. Brain and Language, 44, 2846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, S. (2000). Putting communication disorders in context after traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology, 14, 339347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, S. (2003). Traumatic brain injury and social function: Let's get social. Brain Impairment, 4, 3647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, S., Bornhofen, C., Shum, D., Long, E., Saunders, C., & Neulinger, K. (2006). Reliability and validity of ‘The Awareness of Social Inference Test’ (TASIT): A clinical test of social perception. Disability and Rehabilitation, 28, 15291542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, S. & Flanagan, S. (2004). Social perception deficits after traumatic brain injury: Interaction between emotion recognition, mentalizing ability, and social communication. Neuropsychology, 18, 572579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, S., Flanagan, S., Martin, I., & Saunders, C. (2004). The ecological validity of TASIT: A test of social perception. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 14, 285302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, S., Flanagan, S., Rollins, J., & Kinch, J. (2003). TASIT: A new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 18, 219238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, S. & Pearce, S. (1996). Clinical insights into pragmatic language theory: The case of sarcasm. Brain and Language, 53, 81104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, S. & Saunders, J.C. (2005). Differential impairment in recognition of emotion across different media in people with severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 392399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McHugo, G.J. & Smith, C.A. (1996). The power of faces: A review of John Lanzetta's research on facial expression and emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 20, 85120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKenzie, K., Matheson, E., McKaskie, K., Hamilton, L., & Murray, G.C. (2000). Impact of group training on emotion recognition in individuals with a learning disability. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 143147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melton, A.K. & Bourgeois, M.S. (2005). Training compensatory memory strategies via the telephone for persons with TBI. Aphasiology, 19, 353364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merzenich, M.M. & Jenkins, W.M. (1993). Reorganization of cortical representations of the hand following alterations of skin inputs induced by nerve injury, skin island transfers, and experience. Journal of Hand Therapy, 6, 89104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merzenich, M.M., Nelson, R.J., Stryker, M.P., Cynader, M.S., Schoppmann, A., & Zook, J.M. (1984). Somatosensory cortical map changes following digit amputation in adult monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 224, 591605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milders, M., Fuchs, S., & Crawford, J.R. (2003). Neuropsychological impairments and changes in emotional and social behaviour following severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 157172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, R.L., Elliott, R., Barry, M., Cruttenden, A., & Woodruff, P.W. (2003). The neural response to emotional prosody, as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia, 41, 14101421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohammed, A.H., Zhu, S.W., Darmopil, S., Hjerling-Leffler, J., Ernfors, P., Winblad, B., Diamond, M.C., Eriksson, P.S., & Bogdanovic, N. (2002). Environmental enrichment and the brain. Progress in Brain Research, 138, 109133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montagne, B., Schutters, S., Westenberg, H.G., van Honk, J., Kessels, R.P., & de Haan, E.H. (2006). Reduced sensitivity in the recognition of anger and disgust in social anxiety disorder. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 11, 389401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, J.S., DeGelder, B., Weiskrantz, L., & Dolan, R.J. (2001). Differential extrageniculostriate and amygdala responses to presentation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field. Brain, 124, 12411252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, J.S., Frith, C.D., Perrett, D.I., Rowland, D., Young, A.W., Calder, A.J., & Dolan, R.J. (1996). A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature, 383, 812815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, J.S., Scott, S.K., & Dolan, R.J. (1999). Saying it with feeling: Neural responses to emotional vocalizations. Neuropsychologia, 37, 11551163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, R.L. & Bellack, A.S. (1981). The role of social perception in social skill. Behaviour Therapy, 12, 6979.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morton, M.V. & Wehman, P. (1995). Psychosocial and emotional sequelae of individuals with traumatic brain injury: A literature review and recommendations. Brain Injury, 9, 8192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mueser, K.T., Doonan, R., Penn, D.L., Blanchard, J.J., Bellack, A.S., Nishith, P., & DeLeon, J. (1996). Emotion recognition and social competence in chronic schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 271275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, P.G., Jia, M., & Li, M.X. (2003). Protein kinases and Hebbian function. Neuroscientist, 9, 110116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nudo, R.J. & Milliken, G.W. (1996). Reorganization of movement representations in primary motor cortex following focal ischemic infarcts in adult squirrel monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 75, 21442149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nudo, R.J., Milliken, G.W., Jenkins, W.M., & Merzenich, M.M. (1996a). Use-dependent alterations of movement representations in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 785807.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nudo, R.J., Wise, B.M., SiFuentes, F., & Milliken, G.W. (1996b). Neural substrates for the effects of rehabilitative training on motor recovery after ischemic infarct. Science, 272, 17911794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oddy, M., Coughen, T., Tyerman, A., & Jenkins, D. (1985). Social adjustment after closed head injury: A further follow-up seven years after injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 48, 564568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oddy, M. & Humphrey, M. (1980). Social recovery during the year following severe head injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 43, 798802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oddy, M., Humphrey, M., & Uttley, D. (1978). Subjective impairment and social recovery after closed head injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 41, 611616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olver, J.H., Ponsford, J.L., & Curran, C.A. (1996). Outcome following traumatic brain injury: A comparison between 2 and 5 years after injury. Brain Injury, 10, 841848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelc, K., Kornreich, C., Foisy, M.L., & Dan, B. (2006). Recognition of emotional facial expressions in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatric Neurology, 35, 9397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pell, M.D. (2006). Cerebral mechanisms for understanding emotional prosody in speech. Brain and Language, 96, 221234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pell, M.D. & Baum, S.R. (1997). Unilateral brain damage, prosodic comprehension deficits, and the acoustic cues to prosody. Brain and Language, 57, 195214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penn, D.L. & Combs, D. (2000). Modification of affect perception deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 46, 217229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penn, D.L., Combs, D.R., & Mohamed, S. (2001). Social cognition and social functioning in schizophrenia. In Corrigan, P.W. & Penn, D.L. (Eds.), Social Cognition and Schizophrenia (pp. 97122). Washington, DC: APA Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, L.C., Stambrook, M., Moore, A.D., & Esses, L. (1990). Psychosocial sequelae of closed head injury: Effects on the marital relationship. Brain Injury, 4, 3947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, M.L., Drevets, W.C., Rauch, S.L., & Lane, R. (2003). Neurobiology of emotion perception I: The neural basis of normal emotion perception. British Journal of Psychiatry, 54, 504514.Google ScholarPubMed
Pihan, H., Altenmuller, E., Hertrich, I., & Ackermann, H. (2000). Cortical activation patterns of affective speech processing depend on concurrent demands on the subvocal rehearsal system: A DC-potential study. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 123, 23382349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pizzamiglio, L., Perani, D., Cappa, S.F., Vallar, G., Paolucci, S., Grassi, F., Paulesu, E., & Fazio, F. (1998). Recovery of neglect after right hemispheric damage: H2(15)O positron emission tomographic activation study. Archives of Neurology, 55, 561568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pleger, B., Foerster, A.F., Widdig, W., Henschel, M., Nicolas, V., Jansen, A., Frank, A., Knecht, S., Schwenkreis, P., & Tegenthoff, M. (2003). Functional magnetic resonance imaging mirrors recovery of visual perception after repetitive tachistoscopic stimulation in patients with partial cortical blindness. Neuroscience Letters, 335, 192196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pourtois, G., de Gelder, B., Bol, A., & Crommelinck, M. (2005). Perception of facial expressions and voices and of their combination in the human brain. Cortex, 41, 4959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prigatano, G.P. (1999). Principles of Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prigatano, G.P. & Pribram, K.H. (1982). Perception and memory of facial affect following brain injury. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54, 859869.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermuller, F., Neininger, B., Elbert, T., Mohr, B., Rockstroh, B., Koebbel, P., & Taub, E. (2001). Constraint-induced therapy of chronic aphasia after stroke. Stroke, 32, 16211626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapcsak, S.Z., Comer, J.F., & Rubens, A.B. (1993). Anomia for facial expressions: Neuropsychological mechanisms and anatomical correlates. Brain & Language, 45, 233252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rieffe, C., Meerum Terwogt, M., & Kotronopoulou, K. (2007). Awareness of single and multiple emotions in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 37, 455465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, I.H. & Murre, J.M. (1999). Rehabilitation of brain damage: Brain plasticity and principles of guided recovery. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 544575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rojahn, J. & Warren, V.J. (1997). Emotion recognition as a function of social competence and depressed mood in individuals with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 41, 469475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenzweig, M.R. (1999). Effects of differential experience on brain and cognition throughout the life span. In The changing nervous system: Neurobehavioral consequences of early brain disorders (pp. 2550). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russo, N., Flanagan, T., Iarocci, G., Berringer, D., Zelazo, P.D., & Burack, J.A. (2007). Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: Implications for cognitive neuroscience. Brain & Cognition, 65, 7786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rydin-Orwin, T., Drake, J., & Bratt, A. (1999). The effects of training on emotion recognition skills for adults with an intellectual disability. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 12, 253262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sander, K. & Scheich, H. (2001). Auditory perception of laughing and crying activates human amygdala regardless of attentional state. Cognitive Brain Research, 12, 181198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Satpute, A.B. & Lieberman, M.D. (2006). Integrating automatic and controlled processes into neurocognitive models of social cognition. Brain Research, 1079, 8697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saunders, J.C., McDonald, S., & Richardson, R. (2006). Loss of emotional experience after traumatic brain injury: Findings with the startle probe procedure. Neuropsychology, 20, 224231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, S.K., Young, A.W., Calder, A.J., Hellawell, D.J., Aggleton, J.P., & Johnson, M. (1997). Impaired auditory recognition of fear and anger following bilateral amygdala lesions. Nature, 385, 254257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sergi, M.J., Rassovsky, Y., Nuechterlein, K.H., & Green, M.H. (2006). Social perception as a mediator of the influence of early visual processing on functional status in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 448454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silberman, E.K. & Weingartner, H. (1986). Hemispheric lateralization of functions related to emotion. Brain and Cognition, 5, 322353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sohlberg, M.M. & Mateer, C.A. (2001). Cognitive rehabilitation: An integrative neuropsychological approach. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Solomon, M., Goodlin-Jones, B.L., & Anders, T.F. (2004). A social adjustment enhancement intervention for high functioning autism, Asperger's syndrome, and pervasive developmental disorder NOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 649668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spell, L.A. & Frank, E. (2000). Recognition of nonverbal communication of affect following traumatic brain injury. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 285300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Squires, E.J., Hunkin, N.M., & Parkin, A.J. (1997). Errorless learning of novel associations in amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 35, 11031111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stork, O. & Pape, H.C. (2002). Fear memory and the amygdala: Insights from a molecular perspective. Cell & Tissue Research, 310, 271277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Streit, M., Ioannides, A.A., Liu, L., Wolwer, W., Dammers, J., Gross, J., Gaebel, W., & Muller-Gartner, H.W. (1999). Neurophysiological correlates of the recognition of facial expressions of emotion as revealed by magnetoencephalography. Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 481491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tate, R.L. (1987). Issues in the management of behaviour disturbance as a consequence of severe head injury. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 19, 1318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tate, R.L., Lulham, J., Broe, G., Strettles, B., & Pfaff, A. (1989). Psychosocial outcome for the survivors of severe blunt head injury: The results from a consecutive series of 100 patients. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 52, 11281134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tate, R.L., McDonald, S., & Lulham, J.M. (1998). Incidence of hospital-treated traumatic brain injury in an Australian community. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Public Health, 22, 419423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taub, E., Crago, J.E., & Uswatte, G. (1998). Constraint-induced movement therapy: A new approach to treatment in physical rehabilitation. Rehabilitation Psychology, 43, 152170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taub, E. & Wolf, S. (1997). Constraint induced movement techniques to facilitate upper extremity use in stroke patients. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 3, 3861.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Temple, E., Deutsch, G.K., Poldrack, R.A., Miller, S.L., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M.M., & Gabrieli, J.D. (2003). Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: Evidence from functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 28602865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomsen, I.V. (1974). The patient with severe head-injury and his family—follow-up study of 50 patients. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 6, 180183.Google Scholar
Thomsen, I.V. (1984). Late outcome of very severe blunt head trauma: A 15 year second follow-up. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 47, 260268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tremeau, F. (2006). A review of emotion deficits in schizophrenia. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 8, 5970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trower, P. (1980). Situational analysis of the components and processes of behaviour of socially skilled and unskilled patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 3, 327339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turkstra, L.S. (2001). Treating memory problems in adults with neurogenic communication disorders. Seminars in Speech and Language, 22, 147154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turkstra, L.S. & Bourgeois, M. (2005). Intervention for a modern day HM: Errorless learning of practical goals. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 13, 205212.Google Scholar
van der Gaag, M., Kern, R.S., van den Bosch, R.J., & Liberman, R.P. (2002). A controlled trial of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 28, 167176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuilleumier, P., Hester, D., Assal, G., & Regli, F. (1996). Unilateral spatial neglect recovery after sequential strokes. Neurology, 46, 184189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watts, A.J. & Douglas, J.M. (2006). Interpreting facial expression and communication competence following severe traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology, 20, 707722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webster, J.S. & Scott, R.R. (1983). The effects of self-instructional training on attentional deficits following head injury. Clinical Neuropsychology, 5, 6974.Google Scholar
Weddell, R., Oddy, M., & Jenkins, D. (1980). Social adjustment after rehabilitation: A 2 year follow-up of patients with severe head injury. Psychological Medicine, 10, 257263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weddell, R.A. (1994). Effects of subcortical lesion site on human emotional behavior. Brain & Cognition, 25, 161193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Welsh, J.P., Ahn, E.S., & Placantonakis, D.G. (2005). Is autism due to brain desynchronization? International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 253263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wild, B., Erb, M., & Bartels, M. (2001). Are emotions contagious? Evoked emotions while viewing emotionally expressive faces: Quality, quantity, time course and gender differences. Psychiatry Research, 102, 109124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, K.R., Wishart, J.G., Pitcairn, T.K., & Willis, D.S. (2005). Emotion recognition by children with down syndrome: Investigation of specific impairments and error patterns. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 110, 378392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, B.A. (2002). Towards a comprehensive model of cognitive rehabilitation. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12, 97110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, B.A. (2005). The clinical neuropsychologist's dilemma. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 488493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, B.A., Evans, J.J., & Keohane, C. (2002). Cognitive rehabilitation: A goal-planning approach. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 17, 542555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wishart, J.G., Cebula, K.R., Willis, D.S., & Pitcairn, T.K. (2007). Understanding of facial expressions of emotion by children with intellectual disabilities of differing aetiology. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51, 551563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolwer, W., Frommann, N., Halfmann, S., Piaszek, A., Streit, M., & Gaebel, W. (2005). Remediation of impairments in facial affect recognition in schizophrenia: Efficacy and specificity of a new training program. Schizophrenia Research, 80, 295303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, T.T., Menon, V., Eliez, S., Blasey, C., White, C.D., Reid, A.J., Gotlib, I.H., & Reiss, A.L. (2002). Amygdalar activation associated with positive and negative facial expressions. Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 13, 17371741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yates, P.J. (2003). Psychological adjustment, social enablement and community integration following acquired brain injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 13, 291306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ylvisaker, M. (1993). Communication outcome in children and adolescents with traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 3, 367387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ylvisaker, M., & Feeney, T.J. (1998). Collaborative Brain Injury Intervention: Positive Everyday Routines (Vol. xii). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Ylvisaker, M. & Feeney, T. (2000). Reflections on Dobermans, poodles, and social rehabilitation for difficult-to-serve individuals with traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology, 14, 407431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ylvisaker, M., Hanks, R., & Johnson-Greene, D. (2002). Perspectives on rehabilitation of individuals with cognitive impairment after brain injury: Rationale for reconsideration of theoretical paradigms. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 17, 191209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ylvisaker, M., Jacobs, H.E., & Feeney, T. (2003). Positive supports for people who experience behavioral and cognitive disability after brain injury: A review. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 18, 732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Model of the neuroanatomical correlates of three key processes in emotion perception (adapted from Phillips et al., 2003). Anatomical structures within the ventral system include the amygdala, insula, and ventral regions of both the prefrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate gyrus. This system is thought to mediate both the production of emotional states and the identification of emotion-related stimuli. The dorsal system includes the dorsal regions of the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate gyrus, and the hippocampus. This system mediates the regulation of emotional states and behavior and can, as indicated by the negative and positive signs in the circles, modulate or inhibit the activity of the ventral system so that emotional states and behaviors are contextually appropriate.