Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T09:29:32.885Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Time-Based Prospective Memory in Severe Traumatic Brain Injury Patients: The Involvement of Executive Functions and Time Perception

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 March 2012

Giovanna Mioni*
Affiliation:
Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università di Padova, Padova, Italy
Franca Stablum
Affiliation:
Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università di Padova, Padova, Italy
Shawn M. McClintock
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, UT Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, Texas Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University/New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, New York
Anna Cantagallo
Affiliation:
Dipartimento di Neuroscienze/Riabilitazione Azienda Ospedaliero - Universitaria di Ferrara, Modulo di Neuropsicologia Riabilitativa, UMR, Ferrara, Italy
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Giovanna Mioni, Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università di Padova, Via Venezia 8, 35131 Padova, Italy. E-mail: giovanna.mioni@unipd.it

Abstract

Prospective memory (PM) is the ability to remember to perform a future action at a specified later time, which is investigated through the use of event-based and time-based tasks. Prior investigations have found that PM is impaired following traumatic brain injury (TBI). However, there is limited information regarding the cognitive functions that mediate TBI and PM performance. Thus, this study investigated time-based PM in TBI patients, and the relationship among time-based PM, time perception, and executive functions. To accomplish this objective, 18 severe TBI patients and 18 healthy matched controls performed a time-based PM task, a time reproduction task, and two executive functions (Stroop and n-back) tasks. While both groups increased their monitoring frequency close to the target time, TBI patients monitored more and were less accurate than healthy controls at the target time confirming the time-based PM dysfunction in these patients. Importantly, executive functions, particularly inhibition and updating abilities, were strongly related to time-based PM performance; both time perception and executive functions are involved in time-based prospective memory in controls, whereas, only executive functions appear to be involved in TBI time-based prospective memory performance. (JINS, 2012, 18, 1–9)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baudouin, A., Vanneste, S., Pouthas, V., Isingrini, M. (2006). Age-related changes in duration reproduction: Involvements of working memory processes. Brain and Cognition, 62, 1723.Google Scholar
Block, R.A., Zakay, D. (2006). Prospective remembering involves time estimation and memory processes. In J. Glickson & M. Myslobodsky (Eds.), Timing the future: The case of time-based prospective memory (pp. 2549). New York: World Scientific Publishing.Google Scholar
Block, R.A., Zakay, D., Hancock, P.A. (1998). human aging and duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Psychology and Aging, 13, 584596.Google Scholar
Bowie, C.R., Harvey, P.D. (2006). Administration and interpretation of the Trail Making Test. Nature Protocols, 1, 22772281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlesimo, G.A., Formisano, R., Bivona, U., Barba, L., Caltagirone, C. (2004). Prospective memory in patients with severe closed-head injury: Role of concurrent activity and encoding instructions. Behavioral Neurology, 22, 101110.Google Scholar
Cockburn, J. (1996). Failure of prospective memory after acquired brain damage: Preliminary investigation and suggestions for future directions. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Psychology, 18, 304309.Google Scholar
Del Missier, F., Mäntylä, T., Bruine de Bruin, W. (2010). Executive functions in decision making: An individual differences approach. Thinking & Reasoning, 16, 6997.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Einstein, G.O., McDaniel, M.A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 717726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Einstein, G.O., McDaniel, M.A., Richardson, S.L., Guynn, M.J., Cufer, A.R. (1995). Aging and prospective memory: Examining the influence of self-initiated retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 9961007.Google Scholar
Ellis, J., Kvavilashvili, L. (2000). Prospective memory in 2000: Past, present and future directions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 19.Google Scholar
Fleming, J., Shum, D., Strong, J., Lightbody, S. (2005). Prospective memory rehabilitation for adults with traumatic brain injury: A compensatory training programme. Brain Injury, 19, 113.Google Scholar
Glickson, J., Myslobodsky, M. (2006). Timing the future: The case of time-based prospective memory. New York: World Scientific Publishing.Google Scholar
Gonneaud, J., Kalpouzos, G., Bon, L., Viader, F., Eustache, F., Desgranges, B. (2011). Distinct and shared cognitive functions mediate event- and time-based prospective memory impairment in normal ageing. Memory, 19, 360377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groot, Y.C., Wilson, B.A., Evans, J., Watson, P. (2002). Prospective memory functioning in people with and without brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 645654.Google Scholar
Hagen, C., Malkmus, D., Durham, P., Bowman, K. (1979). Level of cognitive functioning. In Rehabilitation of head injured adult. Comprehensive physical management. Downey, CA: Professional Staff Association of Rancho Los Amigos Hospital.Google Scholar
Hall, K.M., Hamilton, B.B., Gordon, W.A., Zasler, N.D. (1993). Characteristic and comparison of functional assessment indices: Disability Rating Scale, Functional Independence Measure and Functional Assessment Measure. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 8, 6074.Google Scholar
Henry, J.D., Phillips, L.H., Crawford, J.R., Kliegel, M., Theodorou, G., Summers, F. (2007). Traumatic brain injury and prospective memory: Influence of task complexity. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 1, 110.Google Scholar
Kinch, J., McDonald, S. (2001). Traumatic brain injury and prospective memory: An examination of the influence of executive functioning and retrospective memory. Brain Impairment, 2, 119130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kliegel, M., Eschen, A., Thöne-Otto, A.I. (2004). Planning and realization of complex intentions in traumatic brain injury and normal aging. Brain and Cognition, 56, 4354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kliegel, M., Martin, M., McDaniel, M.A., Einstein, G.O. (2001). Varying the importance of a prospective memory task: Differential effects across time- and event-based prospective memory. Memory, 9, 111.Google Scholar
Kliegel, M., McDaniel, M.A., Einstein, G.O. (Eds.). (2008). Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspective. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kopp, U.A., Thöne-Otto, A.I. (2003). Disentangling executive function and memory processes in event-based prospective remembering after brain damage: A neuropsychological study. International Journal of Psychology, 38, 229235.Google Scholar
Labelle, M.A., Graf, P., Grondin, S., Gragné-Roy, L. (2009). Time-related processes in time-based prospective memory and in time-interval production. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 501521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laiacona, M., Inzaghi, M.G., De Tanti, A., Capitani, E. (2000). Wisconsin card sorting test: A new global score, with Italian norms, and its relationship with the Weigl sorting test. Neurological Sciences, 21, 279291.Google Scholar
Mäntylä, T., Carelli, M.G. (2006). Time monitoring and executive functioning: Individual and developmental differences. In J. Glickson & M. Myslobodsky (Eds.), Timing the future: The case of time-based prospective memory (pp. 191211). New York: World Scientific Publishing.Google Scholar
Mäntylä, T., Carelli, M.G., Forman, H. (2007). Time monitoring and executive functioning in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 119.Google Scholar
Mathias, J.L., Mansfield, K.M. (2005). Prospective and declarative memory problems following moderate and severe traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 4, 271282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathias, J.L., Wheaton, P. (2007). Changes in attention and information-processing speed following severe traumatic brain injury: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology, 21, 212223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maujean, A., Shum, D., McQueen, R. (2003). Effect of cognitive demand on prospective memory in individuals with traumatic brain injury. Brain Impairment, 4, 135145.Google Scholar
McDaniel, M.A., Einstein, G.O. (1993). The importance of cue familiarity and the cue distinctiveness in prospective memory. Memory, 1, 2341.Google Scholar
McDaniel, M.A., Guynn, M.J., Glisky, E.L., Rubin, S.R., Routhieaux, B.C. (1999). Prospective memory: A neuropsychological study. Neuropsychology, 13, 103110.Google Scholar
McFarland, C.P., Glisky, E.I. (2009). Frontal lobe involvement in a task of time-based prospective memory. Neuropsychologia, 47, 16601669.Google Scholar
Meyers, C.A., Levin, H.S. (1992). Temporal perception following closed head injury. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioural Neurology, 5, 2832.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N.P., Emerson, M.J., Witzki, A.H., Howerter, A., Wager, T.D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘‘Frontal Lobe’’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100.Google Scholar
Nichelli, P., Clark, K., Hollnagel, C., Grafman, J. (1995). Duration processing after frontal lobe lesion. Annals of New York Academy of Sciences, 759, 183190.Google Scholar
Owen, A.M., McMillan, K.M., Laird, A.R., Bullmore, E. (2005). N-Back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 4659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perbal, S., Couillet, J., Azouvi, P., Pouthas, V. (2003). Relationship between time estimation, memory, attention, and processing speed in patients with severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 41, 15991610.Google Scholar
Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., Rueda, A.D. (2008). Time estimation and episodic memory following traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, 212223.Google Scholar
Shum, D., Levin, H., Chan, R.C. (2011). Prospective memory in patients with closed head injury: A review. Neuropsychologia, 49, 21562165.Google Scholar
Shum, D., Valentine, M., Cutmore, T. (1999). Performance of individuals with severe long-term traumatic brain injury on time-, event-, and activity-based prospective memory tasks. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 4958.Google Scholar
Teasdale, G., Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness: A practical scale. Lancet, 2, 8184.Google Scholar
Vallat-Azouvi, C., Weber, T., Legrand, L., Azouvi, P. (2007). Working memory after severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 770780.Google Scholar
Wilson, B.A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P.W., Emslie, H., Evans, J.J. (1996). Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS). Oxford: Thames Valley Test Company.Google Scholar
Zakay, D. (1993). Time estimation methods — Do they influence prospective duration estimates? Perception, 22, 91101.Google Scholar
Zakay, D., Block, R.A. (1996). The role of attention in time estimation processes. In M.A. Pastor & J. Artieda (Eds.), Time, internal clocks and movement (pp. 143164). Amsterdam: Elsevier/North Holland.Google Scholar
Zimmermann, P., Fimm, B. (2002). Batteria di Test per l'esame dell'attenzione (TEA). Italian adaptation by: L. Pizzamiglio; P. Zoccolotti; P.A. Pittau. V. Fimm, Psycholgische Testsuteme.Google Scholar