Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T04:59:43.407Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Truly Cross-fit: The Association of Exercise and Clinical Outcomes: Introduction to a JINS Special Section

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2021

Glenn E Smith*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical & Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
Ozioma C. Okonkwo
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine and the Wisconsin Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, USA
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Dr. Smith, Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, PO Box 100165, Gainesville, FL 32610-0165, USA. E-mail: glennsmith@phhp.ufl.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

We introduce a JINS special section inspired by a symposium presented at INS 2020 in Denver. The symposium was entitled Truly Cross-fit: The Association of Exercise and Cognitive Reserve. The collection of papers herein spans diverse methods, a range of developmental and clinical conditions, and a variety of outcomes all reflecting on the association of exercise and cognition-related outcomes. Taken together, the studies in this Special Section direct us to the variety of dimensions to be considered in understanding this association including what mode, intensity, duration, and timing of physical activity and aspects of age, sex, genetics, baseline characteristics, and disease status moderate these findings. We hope this Special Section will not only provide a framing for important future research on exercise and clinical outcomes but also inspiration to pursue them.

Type
Short Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2021

A 2015 JINS Special Issue focused on physical activity and brain plasticity. In that Issue, organizers (Smith, et al., Reference Smith, Erickson and Rao2015) undertook a panoramic exploration of the impact that physical activity and exercise have on the brain’s plasticity, broadly construed. The articles contained in the 2015 Issue spanned a variety of paradigms, modalities, and outcomes. From single bouts of intensive exercise to lifelong leisure physical activity; from resistance training to exergaming; and from cognitive measures to top shelf brain imaging. In sum, the works presented in that Issue expanded our understanding of the myriad ways that physical activity and exercise contribute to improved (or preserved) brain health in humans, even in the context of underlying disease.

An invited symposium at the INS annual meeting in Denver in 2020, entitled Truly Cross-fit: The Association of Exercise and Cognitive Reserve, serves as the basis for this special section of JINS. The symposium was initiated by Dr. Okonkwo and organized and chaired by Dr. Smith. Presentations in that symposium included those of Drs. Jill Barnes, Kaitlin Cassaletto, Vonetta Dotson, and Aliyah Snyder, as well as commentary and data shared by Dr. Smith. The collection of papers herein spans diverse methods of acquiring knowledge (literature review, randomized single-intervention pilot clinical trial, multicomponent comparative effectiveness study). It considers a range of developmental and clinical conditions including mild brain injury, cognitive aging, late-life depression, pre-clinical Alzheimer’s disease, and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) of nonspecific etiology. The studies provide a range of methods for establishing the association of an independent variable linked to physical activity with dependent variables related to symptomatology, cognitive function (including global cognition and domain-specific cognitive abilities), and daily function. The studies are summarized in Table 1. These study variations provide the opportunity for a convergence of findings that strengthen our understanding of the phenomena of interest. However, the study variations also result in divergent observations. These divergent observations may result from methodological differences, or could reflect on variations in the association of physical exercise and cognition. However, even the latter case allows us to modify, broaden, and improve our conceptual models.

Table 1 Summarizes the studies included in this special section according to design, population, exposure type, outcome, and findings

In the first paper in this series, Dr. Jill Barnes and her research group (Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Pearson, Corkery, Eisenmann and Miller2021) provide a review of the putative but foundational physiologic mechanisms by which exercise could impact brain and cognitive function. They note two prevailing and complementary theories, the neurogenesis hypothesis and the vascular hypothesis. They then comprehensively review research emanating from the vascular hypothesis. From their own work, the Barnes group demonstrates that cerebral arterial stiffness and vasoreactivity change in normal aging. They then review others’ research which often, but not always, suggests that habitual exercise may impact arterial stiffness and vasoreactivity measures. They argue that one reason for inconsistent findings is that exercise benefits on arterial stiffness and vasoreactivity may be limited to older adults, i.e. that exercise effects in younger adults may be minimal because arterial elasticity is at peak. The Barnes group summarizes: “Arterial stiffening in the large central arteries is associated with indicators of brain health and improved by regular exercise. Therefore, the reported beneficial effects of exercise on arterial stiffness may also indicate favorable changes in the brain. Measuring cerebral vascular function offers a promising way to assess vascular health in the cerebral circulation and could be used as an early biomarker [of future cognitive decline].”

In the next paper, Dotson et al. (Reference Dotson, M, Bogoian, Minto, Taiwo and Salling2021) discuss research findings suggesting that exercise may benefit cognition in late-life depression. This review provides strong evidence for the association of depression with cognitive impairment in later life and describes the neurobiological basis for this association. Dotson et al. subsequently describe meta-analysis that reveals that, when age is unrestricted, the impact of exercise on cognitive function among adults with depression was minimal. In contrast, the association of exercise with improved cognition in older adults with depression was consistently robust. They discuss how ceiling effects may limit associations in younger groups or across depression severities. Ultimately, the Dotson paper conveys the “structural and functional brain changes that are linked to both cognitive impairment and mood disruption, and explains how exercise targets these same brain changes and can thus provide an alternative or adjunctive treatment for cognitive impairment in late-life depression.”

In her presentation at INS, Dr. Cassaletto reported on sex-specific effects of exercise on cognitive aging. The findings included in that presentation indicated that in a cohort of cognitively normal older adults, greater levels of self-reported physical activity were associated with lower levels of inflammatory markers and blood pressure, greater parahippocampal volume, and better performance on processing speed and visual memory measures among men but not among women. Those findings are published elsewhere (Casaletto et al., Reference Casaletto, Lindbergh, Memel, Staffaroni, Elahi, Weiner-Light and Kramer2020).

A team led by Dr. Snyder (Snyder, et al., Reference Snyder, Grief, Clugston, Fitzgerald, Yarrow, Babikian and Bauer2021) share results of a pilot clinical trial in a group of young adults presenting with concussion/mild traumatic brain injury. Participants were between 18 and 40 years old. In the period between 14 and 25 days post injury, they were randomized to 7 days of supervised aerobic exercise or stretching. The aerobic exercise involved 40 min of stationary bike riding on 6 of the 7 days, with a target of 70% maximum heart rate during exercise. Groups were compared on self-reported concussion symptoms, mood, sleep, and cognitive measures involving memory and fluency. No between group differences were found, as both groups showed comparable improvement over time. These results did not support the value of aerobic over nonaerobic activity in the post-acute epoch but did suggest that the dogma of sustained rest after mTBI may not be supported by the evidence.

Shandera-Ochsner, et al. (Reference Shandera-Ochsner, Chandler, Locke, Ball, Crook, Phatak and Smith2021) examined yoga as one piece of a multicomponent intervention program for persons with MCI and their partners. This study involves a unique comparative effectiveness design, with cluster randomization (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Chandler, Locke, Fields, Phatak, Crook and Cochran2017). This project previously reported a positive impact of yoga on informant-reported memory-based activities of daily living (Chandler et al., Reference Chandler, Locke, Crook, Fields, Ball, Phatak and Smith2019). In the present report (Shandera-Ochsner et al., Reference Shandera-Ochsner, Chandler, Locke, Ball, Crook, Phatak and Smith2021), the primary outcomes were broader measures of daily function. They observed that the inclusion of yoga instead of wellness training in a multiple component intervention which included support groups, memory support training (Greenaway, et al., Reference Greenaway, Hanna, Lepore and Smith2008) and computerized cognitive training (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Housen, Yaffe, Ruff, Kennison, Mahncke and Zelinski2009) led to the best functional outcomes at 18-months post program.

Taken together, the studies in this Special Section direct us to the what, when, and whom of the association between physical activity and cognition. In other words, there is a variety of dimensions to be considered in understanding this association:

  • What are the parameters of physical activity; what is the mode of activity, the frequency, the time course, the duration, the intensity?

  • For whom do these associations hold? What aspects of age, sex, genetics, baseline characteristics, and disease status moderate these findings?

  • Is there an optimal time point in the lifespan (or in the aging process itself) when engagement in physical activity has the greatest potency for brain health? Relatedly, is it ever too late to adopt a physically active lifestyle?

  • If we are examining change, what is the natural trajectory of the outcome of interest and how does this impact our assessment of exercise benefit?

  • An important consideration for this field as a whole has to do with adherence and power. Specifically, what role does adherence/nonadherence play in the findings? Are studies sufficiently powered (not just in the usual sense of sample size but also with respect to the “dose” of exercise) to detect the effects of interest?

FUTURE RESEARCH DIRECTIONS

The INS symposium and this collection of papers ensuing from it perhaps reflects the need to develop a “precision public health’ approach to understanding and implementing exercise programs for the purposes of improved cognitive and brain health. Populations with different cognitive risk profiles by virtue of age, sex, genetics, and etiology may have greater or lesser ability to benefit from prescribed or free-living physical activity. More research is needed, and hopefully this Special Section will not only provide a framing for important future questions but also inspiration to pursue them.

Acknowledgments

We express our thanks to the authors of the papers in this special section as well as Stephen Rao and Melissa Lamar, who encouraged us to develop the INS symposium in Denver into this special section.

Financial Support

This work supported by NIA grants R01AG062167 and P30AG066506.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

REFERENCES

Barnes, J.N., Pearson, A.G., Corkery, A.T., Eisenmann, N.A., & Miller, K.B. (2021). Exercise and cerebral vascular function: insights on aging in healthy adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 27, 761775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casaletto, K.B., Lindbergh, C., Memel, M., Staffaroni, A., Elahi, F., Weiner-Light, S., … Kramer, J.H. (2020). Sexual dimorphism of physical activity on cognitive aging: role of immune functioning. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.05.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chandler, M.J., Locke, D.E., Crook, J.E., Fields, J.A., Ball, C.T., Phatak, V.S.,… Smith, G.E. (2019). Comparative effectiveness of behavioral interventions on quality of life for older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Network Open, 2(5), e193016. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.3016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dotson, V.M.G., M, A., Bogoian, H.R., Minto, L.R., Taiwo, Z., & Salling, Z.N. (2021). Be Fit, Be Sharp, Be Well: the case for exercise as a treatment for cognitive impairment in late-life depression. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 27, 776789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenaway, M.C., Hanna, S.M., Lepore, S.W., & Smith, G.E. (2008). A behavioral rehabilitation intervention for amnestic mild cognitive impairment. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias, 23(5), 451461. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=18955724 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shandera-Ochsner, A.L., Chandler, M.J., Locke, D.E.C., Ball, C.T., Crook, J.E., Phatak, V.S., Smith, G.E. (2021). Comparative effects of physical exercise and other behavioral interventions on functional status outcomes in mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 27, 805812.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, G., Chandler, M., Locke, D.E., Fields, J., Phatak, V., Crook, J., … Cochran, D. (2017). Behavioral interventions to prevent or delay dementia: protocol for a randomized comparative effectiveness study. JMIR Research Protocols, 6(11), e223. doi: 10.2196/resprot.8103 Google ScholarPubMed
Smith, J.C., Erickson, K.I., & Rao, S.M. (2015). Introduction to the JINS Special Issue: physical activity and brain plasticity.J Int Neuropsychol Soc, 21(10), 743744. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715001149 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, G.E., Housen, P., Yaffe, K., Ruff, R., Kennison, R.F., Mahncke, H.W., & Zelinski, E.M. (2009). A cognitive training program based on principles of brain plasticity: results from the Improvement in Memory with Plasticity-based Adaptive Cognitive Training (IMPACT) study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 57(4), 594603. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.02167.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snyder, A.R., Grief, S., Clugston, J.R., Fitzgerald, D.B., Yarrow, J.F., Babikian, T, …Bauer, R. (2021). The effect of Aerobic exercise on concussion recovery: a pilot clinical trial. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 27, 790804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Summarizes the studies included in this special section according to design, population, exposure type, outcome, and findings