Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T01:42:44.015Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Verbal learning and memory after childhood stroke

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 September 2004

AMY E. LANSING
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego
JEFFREY E. MAX
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego Children's Hospital and Health Center, San Diego
DEAN C. DELIS
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego Veteran's Administration San Diego Healthcare Center
PETER T. FOX
Affiliation:
University of Texas Health Science Center, San Antonio
JACK LANCASTER
Affiliation:
University of Texas Health Science Center, San Antonio
FACUNDO F. MANES
Affiliation:
Raul Carrea Institute for Neurological Research–FLENI, Buenos Aires, Argentina
AMY SCHATZ
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego

Abstract

Verbal learning and memory (VLM) following pediatric stroke was characterized in a cross-sectional neuropsychological and neuroimaging study of 26 subjects, aged 5 to 17, with a history of pediatric stroke and 26 age, SES, and gender matched orthopedic controls. Further comparisons were made between the VLM profiles of stroke subjects with right versus left hemisphere lesions and early (≤12 months) versus late (>12 months) strokes. Overall, stroke subjects scored significantly lower than control subjects on several VLM indices (California Verbal Learning Test–Children; CVLT–C), as well as on measures of intellectual functioning (IQ) and auditory attention/working memory (Digit Span). Subgroup analyses of the stroke population found no significant differences in VLM, Digit Span, Verbal IQ or Performance IQ when left-hemisphere lesion subjects were compared to right-hemisphere lesion subjects. In contrast, early strokes were associated with significantly fewer words recalled after delay, reduced discriminability (fewer correct hits relative to false positive errors on recognition testing), and relatively worse auditory attention/working memory scores (Digit Span). These findings indicate that pediatric stroke subjects demonstrated more VLM impairment than control subjects, and early strokes were associated with greater recall and recognition deficits. In stark contrast with adult-onset stroke, both left- and right-hemisphere lesions during childhood resulted in similar VLM performance. (JINS, 2004, 10, 742–752.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
2004 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Aram, D.M. (1998). Neuroplasticity: Evidence from unilateral brain lesions in children. In Broman & Fletcher (Eds.), The changing nervous system (pp. 254273). London: Oxford University Press.
Aram, D.M. & Eisele, J.A. (1994). Intellectual stability in children with unilateral brain lesions. Neuropsychologia, 32, 8595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aram, D.M. & Ekelman, B.L. (1986). Cognitive profiles of children with early onset of unilateral lesions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2, 155172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aram, D.M. & Ekelman, B.L. (1987). Unilateral brain lesions in childhood: Performance on the Revised Token Test. Brain and Language, 32, 137158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aram, D.M., Ekelman, B.L., Rose, D.F., & Whitaker, H.A. (1985). Verbal and cognitive sequelae following unilateral lesions acquired in early childhood. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7, 5578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldo, J., Delis, D.C., Kramer, J., & Shimamura, A.P. (2002). Memory performance on the California Verbal Learning Test–II: Findings from patients with focal frontal lesions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 539546.Google Scholar
Ballantyne, A.O., Scarvie, K.M., & Trauner, D.A. (1994). Verbal and performance IQ patterns in children after perinatal stroke. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10, 3950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E. (1994). Modularity, domain specificity and the development of language. Discussions in Neuroscience, 10, 136149.Google Scholar
Bates, E. & Roe, K. (2001). Language development in children with unilateral brain injury. In C.A. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 281307). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bates, E., Vicari, S., & Trauner, D.A. (1999). Neural mediation of language development: Perspectives from lesion studies of infants and children. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 533581) Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Block, G.W., Nanson, J.L., & Lowry, N.J. (1999). Attention, memory, and language after pediatric ischemic stroke. Child Neuropsychology, 5, 8191.Google Scholar
Brewer, V.R., Fletcher, J.M., Hiscock, M., & Davidson, K.C. (2001). Attention processes in children with shunted hydrocephalus versus attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 15, 185198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cohen, M.J. (1997). Children's Memory Scale manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Damasio, H. & Damasio, A.R. (1989). Lesion analysis in neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press.
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (1994). California Verbal Learning Tests, Children's Version manual. Boston: The Psychological Corporation.
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Eisele, J.A. & Aram, D.M. (1994). Comprehension and imitation of syntax following early hemisphere damage. Brain and Language, 46, 212231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridlund, A.J. & Delis, D.C. (1994). California Verbal Learning Test scoring assistant user's guide (Version 1, Windows Version). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Goodglass, H. & Kaplan, E. (1972). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
Goodman, R. & Yude, C. (1996). IQ and its predictors in childhood hemiplegia. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 38, 881890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, R.N. (1989). Neuronal misconnections and psychiatric disorder: Is there a link? British Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 292299.Google Scholar
Hollingshead, A. (1975). Four Factor Index of Social Status. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Isaacson, R.L. (1975). The myth of recovery from early brain damage. In N.R. Ellis (Ed.), Aberrant development in infancy: Human and animal studies (pp. 125). Potomac, MD: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Jaffe, K.M., Fay, G.C., Polissar, N.L., Martin, K.M., Shurtleff, H.A., Rivara, J.M., & Winn, H.R. (1993). Severity of pediatric traumatic brain injury and neurobehavioral recovery at one year: A cohort study. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 74, 587595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancaster, J.L., Woldorff, M.G., Parsons, L.M., Liotti, M., Freitas, C.S., Rainey, L., Kochunov, P.V., Nickerson, D., Mikiten, S.A., & Fox, P.T. (2000). Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Human Brain Map, 10, 120131.3.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, H.S., Aldrich, E.F., Saydjari, C., Eisenberg, H.M., Foulkes, M.A., Bellefleur, M., Luerssen, T.G., Jane, J.A., Marmarou, A., & Marshall, L.F. (1992). Severe head injury in children: Experience of the Traumatic Coma Data Bank. Neurosurgery, 31, 435444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, H.S., Culhane, K.A., Mendelsohn, D., & Lilly, M.A. (1993). Cognition in relation to magnetic resonance imaging in head-injured children and adolescents. Archives of Neurology, 50, 897905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattson, S.N., Riley, E.P., Delis, D.C., Stern, C., & Jones, K.L. (1996). Verbal learning and memory in children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcohol Clinical Experimental Research, 20, 810816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Max, J.E., Mathews, K., Lansing, A.E., Robertson, B.A.M., Fox, P., Lancaster, J., Manes, F.F., & Smith, J. (2002). Psychiatric disorders after childhood stroke. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 555562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, A.M. (1997). The functional organization of working memory processes within human lateral frontal cortex: the contribution of functional neuroimaging. European Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 13291339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paus, T., Otaky, N., Caramanos, Z., MacDonald, D., Zijdenbos, A., D'Avirro, D., Gutmans, D., Holmes, C., Tomaiuolo, F., & Evans, A.C. (1996). In vivo morphometry of the intrasulcal gray matter in the human cingulate, paracingulate, and superior-rostral sulci: Hemispheric asymmetries, gender differences and probability maps. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 376, 664673.3.0.CO;2-M>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrides, M. (1994). Frontal lobes and working memory: Evidence from investigations of the effects of cortical excisions in nonhuman primates. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 5982). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Riva, D. & Cazzaniga, L. (1986). Late effects of unilateral brain lesions sustained before and after age one. Neuropsychologia, 24, 423428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roman, M.J., Delis, D.C., Willerman, L., Magulac, M., Demadura, T.L., de la Pena, J.L., Loftis, C., Walsh, J., & Kracun, M. (1998). Impact of pediatric traumatic brain injury on components of verbal memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 245258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shear, P.K., Tallal, P., & Delis, D.C. (1992). Verbal learning and memory in language impaired children. Neuropsychologia, 30, 451458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheslow, D. & Adams, W. (1990). Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.
Squire, L.R. (1987). Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University Press.
Stiles, J. & Nass, R. (1991). Spatial grouping activity in young children with congenital right or left hemisphere brain injury. Brain and Cognition, 15, 201222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiles, J. & Thal, D. (1993). Linguistic and spatial cognitive development following early focal brain injury: Patterns of deficit and recovery. In M.H. Johnson (Ed.), Brain development and cognition: A reader (pp. 643664). Malden, UK: Blackwell Publishers Inc.
Taylor, H.G. & Alden, J. (1997). Age-related differences in outcomes following childhood brain insults: an introduction and overview. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 555567.Google Scholar
Vargha-Khadem, F., O'Gorman, A.M., & Watters, G.V. (1985). Aphasia and handedness in relation to hemispheric side, age at injury and severity of cerebral lesion during childhood. Brain, 108, 677696.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Third Edition. New York: The Psychological Corporation.
Winter, R.B. & Lonstein, J.E. (1999). Juvenile and adolescent scoliosis. In H.N. Herkowitz, S.R. Garfin, R.A. Balderston, F.J. Eismont, G.R. Bell, & S.W. Wiesel (Eds.), Rothman-Simeone, the spine (4th ed., pp. 325372). Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders.
Woods, B.T. (1980). The restricted effects of right-hemisphere lesions after age one: Wechsler test data. Neuropsychologia, 18, 6570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woods, B.T. & Carey, S. (1979). Language deficits after apparent clinical recovery from childhood aphasia. Annals of Neurology, 6, 405409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woods, B.T. & Teuber, H.L. (1973). Early onset of complementary specialization of cerebral hemispheres in man. Transactions of the American Neurological Association, 98, 113117.Google Scholar
Woods, B.T. & Teuber, H.L. (1978). Changing patterns of childhood aphasia. Annals of Neurology, 3, 273280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeates, K.O., Blumenstein, E., Patterson, C.M., & Delis, D.C. (1995). Verbal learning and memory following pediatric closed-head injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1, 7887.CrossRefGoogle Scholar