Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T07:12:21.401Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

An exploratory investigation of interactions between syllabic prominence, initial geminates, and phrasal boundaries in Pattani Malay

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 November 2023

Francesco Burroni
Affiliation:
Cornell University Center of Excellence in Southeast Asian Linguistics, Chulalongkorn University
Sireemas Maspong
Affiliation:
Cornell University Center of Excellence in Southeast Asian Linguistics, Chulalongkorn University
Pittayawat Pittayaporn*
Affiliation:
Center of Excellence in Southeast Asian Linguistics, Chulalongkorn University
Pimthip Kochaiyaphum
Affiliation:
Center of Excellence in Southeast Asian Linguistics, Chulalongkorn University
*
*Corresponding author. Email: Pittayawat.P@chula.ac.th
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study investigates interactions among relative syllabic prominence, initial geminates (IGs), and prosodic boundaries in Pattani Malay (PM) against a background of previous analyses claiming that IGs are moraic and trigger a ‘stress shift’ or the linking of a pitch accent to the initial syllable. We conducted an acoustic study with fourteen PM speakers, producing singleton–IG minimal pairs in naturalistic sentences. Our results show that the presence of IGs is not associated with the hypothesized phonological changes. Instead, it is associated with moderate increases in the duration of initial syllables, the intensity of the initial syllable vowels, and the f0 of the initial and final syllable vowels. On the other hand, the presence of a phrase-final prosodic boundary correlates with more drastic changes: in phrase-final position, final syllables exhibit final lengthening and falling contours of f0 and intensity, while, in the phrase-medial position, no lengthening is observed and f0 contours are rising. Furthermore, the effects of IGs are strongest in the phrase-final position, suggesting interactions between IGs and prosodic boundaries. Taken together, results cast doubts on the claim that IGs are moraic and associated with categorical differences in syllabic prominence profiles in PM and show that IG effects are modulated by prosodic boundaries.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The International Phonetic Association

1. Introduction

In the world’s languages, consonantal duration can function contrastively to differentiate meaning, for instance, Italian [ˈpala] ‘shovel’ vs. [ˈpalːa] ‘ball’. Such contrasts between short or singleton and long or geminate consonants are subject to various constraints. A well-known constraint is that singleton vs. geminate contrasts are often licensed word-medially but more rarely in word-initial and/or word-final position (Kraehenmann Reference Kraehenmann2011; Topintzi & Davis Reference Topintzi, Davis and Kubozono2017). Pattani Malay (PM), an Austronesian language spoken in Southern Thailand, does not conform to this generalization. Instead, the language licenses singleton vs. geminate contrasts for all segments only in word-initial position, as illustrated in Table 1.

Table 1 Pattani Malay minimal pairs showing lexical contrast for initial singletons and geminates

Pattani Malay geminates are remarkable not only because they are always initial geminates (IGs) but also because their presence has been claimed to trigger categorical differences in the prominence profiles of words that contain them. Several researchers (Yupho Reference Yupho1989; Hajek & Goedemans Reference Hajek and Goedemans2003; Topintzi Reference Topintzi2008) have claimed that PM words without IGs exhibit final stress and words with IGs exhibit initial stress, for instance, [maˈtɔ] ‘eye’ vs. [ˈmːatɔ] ‘jewelry’. The difference between the two categories of words is captured by different phonological representations and rules that trigger a change in the position of stress. Specifically, PM IGs have been analyzed as moraic such that the presence of IGs increases the prosodic weight of initial syllables, which are therefore assigned stress (Hajek & Goedemans Reference Hajek and Goedemans2003; Topintzi Reference Topintzi2008).

This phonological analysis seems prima facie compatible with results of phonetic studies (Abramson Reference Abramson1987; Abramson Reference Abramson, Warotamasikkhadit and Panakul1998; Phuengnoi Reference Phuengnoi2010), which show that the presence of IGs is associated with an increased saliency of initial syllables. However, the narrow scope and limitations of previous phonetic work on the language suggest caution in marshalling phonetic evidence in favor of the phonological analyses outlined above. Notably, phonetic studies of PM were not designed to study prominence, but only the acoustic consequence of IGs.

An outstanding issue is that IGs in PM have only been studied in words produced in isolation or in phrase-final position within a carrier sentence (Abramson Reference Abramson1987; Abramson Reference Abramson, Warotamasikkhadit and Panakul1998; Phuengnoi Reference Phuengnoi2010). However, studying IGs and prominence only in isolation or in phrase-final position is problematic because the prominence profiles of words produced in isolation may reflect phrase level – rather than word level – acoustic patterns. Similarly, the presence of phrase-final boundaries may affect the acoustic profiles of words, which are related to phonological prominence. More specifically, final boundaries may affect the relative duration of syllables and induce more drastic f0 and intensity falling contours on final syllables. Thus, phrase-final boundaries can potentially obscure the relative prominence profiles of final and non-final syllables and the effect of IGs on them.

In this paper, we attempt to dissect the contribution of IGs and prosodic boundaries in PM to increase our understanding of syllabic prominence in this understudied Malay variety. We conducted an acoustic study of PM words with and without IGs produced in relatively naturalistic speech and two prosodic positions: phrase-medial and phrase-final.

Based on previous phonetic phonological work on PM, in this paper we pose two research questions. The first is whether the presence of IGs affects the prominence profile of PM words. This is to ascertain the merits of a ‘stress shift’ account (e.g., Topintzi Reference Topintzi2008), which predicts categorical differences in acoustic cues typically associated with stress or pitch accents (Gordon & Roettger Reference Gordon and Roettger2017). The second question is whether position in the utterance affects the acoustic prominence profile of PM words. We hypothesize that words in the phrase-medial position and at prosodic boundaries show categorial differences with respect to duration, intensity, f0, and spectral tilt. A more detailed overview of the research questions and our hypotheses is presented in Section 3.

2. Literature review

2.1 Language background and claims regarding the interactions between initial geminates and prominence in Pattani Malay

Known colloquially as Yawi or Jawi, PM is one of the few Austronesian languages spoken in Mainland Southeast Asia. According to the 2010 census (National Statistical Office 2012), approximately 1.47 million people speak PM at home. PM forms a dialect continuum with Kelantan Malay and other northeastern peninsular Malay varieties spoken to the south of Thailand, just across the Malaysian border (Collins Reference Collins1989: 241–244). While Standard Thai is the sole national and official language of Thailand and the only language of instruction in schools, PM is the dominant language in the three border provinces, namely, Pattani, Yala, and Narathiwat, and some districts in the province of Songkhla (Smalley Reference Smalley1994 155–175). Although the presence of Thai in the area has increased, PM remains the preferred language used in the family, and the primary linguistic code for communication among ethnic Malays, especially in rural areas. Additionally, proficiency in PM remains high because children acquire this language before starting school (Poonsub Reference Poonsub and Yunyasit2016; Premsrirat & Burarungrot Reference Premsrirat, Burarungrot, Premsrirat and Hirsh2018: 239; Premsrirat & Burarungrot Reference Premsrirat and Burarungrot2022).

Pattani Malay is well-known in the phonetic and phonological literature as a language with geminates attested only in word-initial position. PM shares this feature with a few other Austronesian languages, such as Leti, Ngada, and Yapese, as well as the Austroasiatic language Nyaheun (Muller Reference Muller2001; Topintzi & Davis Reference Topintzi, Davis and Kubozono2017). PM IGs are tied overwhelmingly to disyllabic words because they diachronically originated from the reduction of erstwhile trisyllabic forms or tetrasyllabic reduplicated forms (Paramal Reference Paramal1991).

All possible onset consonants in PM have a geminate counterpart, except for /r/ and /z/ which are found only in recent loan words (Uthai Reference Uthai1993: 91). Table 2 illustrates the consonant inventory of PM. Note that PM does not have phonemic /f/ and /x/. Surface geminates [fː] and [xː] are stylistic variants of /kɨɣ-/ and /pɨɣ/, respectively.

Only one study, Phuengnoi (Reference Phuengnoi2010), has been devoted to the acoustics of prominence in PM by comparing syllables within a word and across words with and without IGs. Assuming that Yupho’s (Reference Yupho1989) description of identifying stressed and unstressed syllables was valid, Phuengnoi investigated the acoustic correlates of the stress by using data from seven male native speakers. The stimuli were monosyllabic, disyllabic, and trisyllabic words without IGs and compared with disyllabic words with IGs. The main findings are as follows. For vowel duration, final syllables in words with and without IGs have a longer vowel duration than initial syllables. Regarding f0, in words with and without IGs, the mean f0 is higher on initial syllable vowels than on final syllable vowels (Figure 1). Moreover, the mean f0 of initial syllable vowels in words with IGs is higher than in words without IGs (Figure 1, left). For intensity, initial syllable vowels in words with and without IGs have higher mean intensity than final syllables. When comparing initial syllables of words with and without IGs, syllables with IGs have significantly higher mean intensities. Crucially, Phuengnoi also found that the f0 contours for both words with and without IGs show falling profiles over the whole word and that final syllables have nearly identical f0 contours (Figure 1, right).

A major finding of Phuengnoi (Reference Phuengnoi2010) is that words with and without IGs display the same relative duration and f0 and intensity contours: final syllables are always longer, have lower f0, and lower intensity than initial syllables. The magnitude of the values is different, however, especially for f0 and intensity of the initial syllable when comparing words with singleton and IG onsets (Figure 1, left). Phuengnoi’s results also decidedly speak against the idea of stress shift as the f0, intensity, and durational patterns are invariant across words with and without IG onsets.

Table 2 Consonant inventory in Pattani Malay (PM) (Uthai Reference Uthai2011)

Figure 1. f0 contour of disyllabic words with (CːV.CV) and without initial geminates (CV.CV) (Phuengnoi Reference Phuengnoi2010).

Unfortunately, one major caveat with Phuengnoi’s study is that it is limited to words recorded in isolation. Thus, the data presented only represent words immediately preceding an utterance boundary. Similar caveats hold for previous investigations of IGs in PM. The data presented in Abramson’s (Reference Abramson1987, Reference Abramson, Warotamasikkhadit and Panakul1998) studies were elicited in isolation and in a carrier sentence, but the analyses were performed without considering or discussing potential differences due to phrasal positions. Accordingly, what is unclear is whether the reported effects on syllabic prominence are due to IGs, phrasal boundaries, or a potential interaction between the two. Because of the difficulties in establishing a clear pattern of IG behavior and potential interactions with prominence, a study that simultaneously manipulates IGs and prosodic boundaries may help us dissect their relative contribution to syllabic prominence in PM.

2.2 Syllabic prominence in Malay varieties

The role that relative syllabic prominence plays in the sound systems of Malay varieties, especially those spoken in Indonesia, is still a matter of controversy among researchers. Maskikit-Essed and Gussenhoven (Reference Maskikit-Essed and Gussenhoven2016) identified three main views on word-level prominence in Malay varieties. The first view assumes that the position of stress is fixed either on the penultimate or the ultimate syllable, additionally, syllables with [ə] are unstressed (e.g., Cohn Reference Cohn1989; McDonnell Reference McDonnell2016). This approach is sometimes known as the “traditional view.” A second view also assumes that Malay varieties display word-level prominence, but the assignment of prominence is conditioned by the position of the word in the utterance, e.g., words in phrase-medial position show penultimate stress, and words in phrase-final position show final stress (e.g., Halim Reference Halim1974). Finally, the third view claims that these languages do not have word-level stress, and the “prominence” described as stress in other studies is phrase-level prominence (e.g., Mohd Don et al. Reference Mohd Don, Knowles and Yong2008; van Heuven et al. Reference Heuven, Roosman and van Zanten2008). While these conflicting views reflect actual linguistic heterogeneity, they may in part stem from lack of in-depth acoustic investigation on some of the reported varieties (cf. Kaland Reference Kaland2021 for a similar conclusion).

Despite disagreements among phonological analyses, many Malay varieties show remarkably similar acoustic behaviors when phrasal prosody is considered. Reports on the acoustic behavior of several Malay varieties show that durational increase and f0 movements that could be symptomatic of prominence are not concomitant. In most of the Malay varieties that have been studied acoustically, final syllables invariably display longer durations, and f0 contours vary by the position of words in the phrase and/or by discourse conditions (e.g., declarative vs. interrogative clauses). There are varieties that show falling f0 contours when words appear in phrase-final position, including words in isolation, and rising f0 contours when words appear in phrase-medial position, namely, Besemah (McDonnell Reference McDonnell2016), Malaysian Malay (Mohd Don et al. Reference Mohd Don, Knowles and Yong2008), and Ambonese Malay, which also display rising contours on final words in interrogative sentences (Maskikit-Essed & Gussenhoven Reference Maskikit-Essed and Gussenhoven2016). Other varieties, for example, Indonesian spoken by speakers from southern Sumatra (Halim Reference Halim1974) and Betawi Malay (van Heuven et al. Reference Heuven, Roosman and van Zanten2008), display rising contours in phrase-final position and falling contours in phrase-medial position. However, exceptions to this common pattern have also been reported in the literature. Indonesian spoken by Javanese speakers, as reported by Goedemans and van Zanten (Reference Goedemans and van Zanten2007), shows consistent f0 contour patterns: rising contours on phrase-medial words, elicited out of focus, and falling contours on phrase-final word, elicited under focus. In contrast, there is no clear duration pattern: neither penultimate nor ultimate syllables are reported to be consistently longer than the other syllables. Indonesian spoken by Toba Batak speakers is another interesting case, where f0 and duration are used together to increase prominence on a single syllable, such as in a prototypical stress language. This language also does not show the different contours depending on the position in a phrase.

Another variety that stands out is Papuan Malay, reported to have penultimate stress, except for words with [ɛ] in penultimate syllables, which have ultimate stress instead (Kluge Reference Kluge2017). An acoustic study of words in phrase-medial position by Kaland (Reference Kaland2019) shows that acoustic correlates of stress in Papuan Malay are the duration of segments, the timing of f0 movement (rising/falling contours), vowel formant displacement, and spectral tilt (H1−A2, H1−A3). Other parameters, such as relative pitch height, do not correlate with stress. Further work on the language has shown that f0 contours seem to correlate with phrasal-level prosody: rising contours are observed in phrase-medial position, while falling contours are observed in phrase-final position (Kaland & Baumann Reference Kaland and Baumann2020). This behavior of f0 follows the general trend of Malay varieties and suggests that in these languages, f0 may be associated with phrasal prosody, while other cues, such as duration, spectral tilt, etc., may be associated with word-level prosody (Gordon Reference Gordon2014; Gordon & Roettger Reference Gordon and Roettger2017; Kaland Reference Kaland2021).

As for PM, previous acoustic studies of PM (Abramson Reference Abramson1987, Reference Abramson, Warotamasikkhadit and Panakul1998; Phuengnoi Reference Phuengnoi2010) seem to suggest that the language shows a dissociation of duration and other parameters. More specifically, the duration of final syllables is always higher than other syllables; but intensity and f0 are found to be higher on the initial syllables, regardless of the presence of IGs (Phuengnoi Reference Phuengnoi2010) or found to be higher on initial syllables when the IGs are present (Abramson Reference Abramson1987, Reference Abramson, Warotamasikkhadit and Panakul1998). However, whether the behaviors are due to phrasal effects remains unclear because the words studied were recorded in isolation or at the end of a carrier sentence. However, to understand how relative syllabic prominence interacts with IGs and phrasal boundary in this understudied variety of Malay, all cues that have been reported as relevant for prominence in Malay varieties must be examined.

3. Research questions, hypotheses, and predictions

Based on the literature review, this paper aims to address two primary research questions concerning the prominence profiles of words in PM.

  1. (i) Does the presence of IGs affect the prominence profile of PM words?

  2. (ii) Does position in the utterance affect the prominence profile of PM words?

For our first question, we have two logical alternatives: either IGs affect the prominence profiles of PM words or they do not. If we follow previous phonological descriptions and analyses, it is reasonable to expect categorically different profiles in words with and without IGs. In disyllabic forms, prominence profiles of words with and without IGs are then expected to be mirror images of each other. Words with IGs are expected to have initial prominence, while words without IGs are expected to have final prominence.

By examining acoustic dimensions that are relevant for stress or pitch accents – such as duration, intensity, f0, vowel quality, and spectral tilt (Gordon & Roettger Reference Gordon and Roettger2017), words containing IGs would be expected to have more extreme values, compatible with prominence, on initial syllables than on final syllables. These acoustic cues include longer duration, higher intensity, higher f0, more extreme vowel quality, and higher spectral tilt. On the other hand, words not containing IGs would be expected to have values more compatible with prominence on final rather than initial syllables.

Alternatively, it is also possible that words with IGs do not display different prominence profiles. PM disyllabic words may have fixed prominence on the initial syllable, or on the final syllable, or neither of the two syllables may be more prominent than the other. If prominence is fixed initially or finally, we expect initial or final syllables respectively to display more extreme acoustic values compatible with prominence no matter whether they contain IGs or not. Finally, if neither of the two syllables is more prominent than the other, we expect to observe no clear patterns of greater relative prominence between the two syllables. Figure 2 illustrates the predicted acoustic profiles under the hypothesis that IGs have a categorical effect and the hypothesis that PM has a fixed (final) prominence.

Figure 2. Predicted acoustic profiles based on stress shift hypothesis (top) and fixed (final) prominence hypothesis (bottom).

For our second research question, we similarly have two alternative scenarios: either the presence of a phrasal boundary affects the realization of the prominence profiles of syllables, or it does not. If phrasal boundaries categorically influence prominence profiles, we expect final syllables to have different acoustic properties in phrase-medial and phrase-final position. Final syllables may be longer in phrase-final than in phrase-medial position because of phrase-final lengthening (Byrd & Saltzman Reference Byrd and Saltzman2003; Edwards et al. Reference Edwards, Beckman and Fletcher1991; Turk & Shattuck-Hufnagel Reference Turk and Shattuck-Hufnagel2007; Wightman et al. Reference Wightman, Shattuck-Hufnagel, Ostendorf and Price1992). Similarly, f0 may fall more drastically, possibly together with intensity, in the vicinity of a phrasal boundary, as has been observed in other Malay varieties (e.g., Mohd Don et al. Reference Mohd Don, Knowles and Yong2008; Maskikit-Essed & Gussenhoven Reference Maskikit-Essed and Gussenhoven2016). The hypothesis that PM would show falling contours instead of rising contours is according to Phuengnoi’s (Reference Phuengnoi2010) report of words in isolation.

Moreover, phrasal position may affect the differences between final and initial syllables. For example, it is possible that the durational difference between final syllables and initial syllables will be more pronounced in phrase-final position again due to boundary-adjacent lengthening. Similarly, f0 and intensity contours over the word may be flat or rising in phrase-medial position but falling in phrase-final position. Interestingly, the predicted phrasal boundary effects lead to a dissociation of the acoustic correlates that are typically manipulated in tandem by speakers producing stress or pitch accents. The syllables adjacent to a phrase-final boundary have longer durations, a property typical of prominent syllables, but also falling and generally lower f0 and intensity, a property typically associated with non-prominent syllables.

A logical alternative would be that prosodic boundaries do not systematically affect the relative prominence of syllables in PM words. If this is the case, we do not expect to observe differences in the acoustic realization of final and initial syllables in phrase-medial and phrase-final position. Studying IGs and phrasal boundaries together also allows us to investigate their potential interactions.

4. Methodology

4.1 Participants

The participants were fourteen speakers of PM (six males, eight females), aged from twenty to sixty-one years (μ = 33, σ = 15). All speakers were born and raised in the three southernmost provinces of Thailand, namely, Pattani, Yala, and Narathiwat; they reported native proficiency in PM and native-like fluency in Thai. These speakers are sequential bilinguals because the ages at which they learned Thai are the same as those of kindergarten and elementary school students. Speakers did not disclose any speech or hearing impairment.

4.2 Materials and experimental task

Disyllabic words were used as IGs are only attested in monosyllabic and disyllabic forms (Paramal Reference Paramal1991); however, the former are uninformative regarding prominence profiles. Thirteen disyllabic minimal pairs differing only in singleton vs. geminate onsets were selected from the literature (Abramson Reference Abramson1987, Reference Abramson, Warotamasikkhadit and Panakul1998; Hamzah et al. Reference Hamzah, Fletcher, Hajek, Lee and Zee2011, Reference Hamzah, Fletcher, Hajek, Hay and Parnell2014); Table 3 presents the full list. A fourteenth minimal pair [pitu] ‘door’ [pːitu] ‘at the door’ was also collected, but we did not analyze it because participants produced the geminate form in a non-assimilated prepositional phrase form, namely, [di pitu].

Note that the stimuli were not balanced for onset manners. This was not possible as we wanted to ensure that all participants were familiar with the target words. However, we do not expect stimuli with plosive and sonorant onsets to behave differently, as in Kelantan Malay (Hamzah Reference Hamzah2013), which forms dialect continuum with PM. Even though voiceless stop IGs show a stronger effect on vowel intensity and f0, voiced stop and sonorant IGs also show a significant effect on intensity and f0 with the same direction of effect.

Although Arabic-based, Roman-based, and Thai-based scripts have been designed for PM, its speakers are usually illiterate in the language (Paramal Reference Paramal1991: 40; Smalley Reference Smalley1994). Accordingly, it is not possible to rely on written stimuli to elicit the target words. To obviate this problem, participants were familiarized with pairs of PM sentences containing the target word and their Thai translation to avoid ambiguity of the stimuli and mistakes in translation. In the experiment, participants were cued using the Thai translation of the target sentences uttered by an experimenter, after which participants produced the corresponding PM version containing the target words. Whenever participants’ production contained a mistake, such as disfluencies, pauses, or speech errors, participants were asked to repeat the trial. Although this task may resemble a translation task or a repetition task, we consider it ecologically valid for the following reasons. First, because participants are familiarized with translations of each sentence before the experiment, they are not translating online word by word. Hence the Thai sentence is simply a cue for a PM sentence. Accordingly, it is not very likely that participants would be attending to finer details of the Thai production of the utterance.

Table 3 The thirteen minimal pairs of disyllabic forms that differed uniquely for the singleton or initial geminate (IG) onsets of the initial syllable

We opted to elicit target words by orally presenting them in natural-sounding PM and Thai sentences (Appendix A), our main consideration being that a more realistic picture of prominence requires the production of PM IGs in a more naturalistic context in which IGs are less likely to be hyperarticulated. The production of PM IGs in less naturalistic contexts, for example, in isolation or a carrier sentence, has been investigated; the results demonstrate that segmental and phrasal effects are difficult to separate (Section 1.2). This problem is frequently observed in studies on stress and prominence (Gordon & Roettger Reference Gordon and Roettger2017). On the basis of this background and claims in the literature on Malay varieties (Kaland Reference Kaland2019), we subscribe to the idea that naturalistic speech yields more representative samples than read speech for the study of word-level prominence.

The effects of IGs, prosodic boundaries, and their interaction were tested: target words were elicited in both phrase-medial and phrase-final positions. Each sentence was repeated three times. In total, twenty-six tokens (thirteen unique minimal pairs) × two phrasal conditions (medial vs. final) × three repetitions × fourteen speakers = 2,184 tokens were collected; 212 tokens were excluded because of strong background noise in the sound files or equipment failures. Thus, 1,972 tokens were analyzed: 932 contained IGs and 1,040 contained singletons.

4.3 Data collection and processing

Audio data were collected at 44.1 kHz in Praat (Boersma & Weenink Reference Boersma and Weenink2020) by using a Rode NT-USB, a cardioid condenser microphone. We use a cardioid microphone placed on a table near participants (like other work on Malay prominence, Kaland Reference Kaland2019) to have a more naturalistic setting. This choice may have led to more variation in intensity measurements and, thus, to less accurate estimates of intensity effects. Future work could attempt to produce a PM corpus where microphone distance is tightly controlled to ensure a more accurate picture of the effects of IG and prosodic boundaries on intensity.

All recordings were made in quiet rooms at the Prince of Songkla University, Pattani Campus, Pattani, Thailand. We first identified target words and labeled them using Praat TextGrids; subsequently, the portion of the audio signal representing the target words was extracted using MATLAB (MATLAB 2021). Segmental boundaries were obtained in Praat TextGrids by forced alignment with a custom-built model trained using the Montreal Forced Aligner on our dataset (McAuliffe et al. Reference McAuliffe, Michaela Socolof, Wagner and Sonderegger2017). The TextGrids were inspected and manually corrected when necessary. Corrected TextGrids containing segmental boundaries and audio signals of each word were read back in MATLAB for analysis.

The variables analyzed were syllable duration, vowel f0, vowel intensity, vowel formants, and spectral tilt (H1−A2, H1−A3). For the duration, we first analyzed the (Ia) raw duration of singleton and geminate consonants and the (Ib) durational ratio of the consonants to the entire word ( $\frac{C}{W}$ ) to confirm that singleton/IG onsets are distinguishable by duration. We then analyzed (II) the duration ratio of the initial syllable (σ i ) to the entire word ( $\frac{{{{\bf{\sigma }}_i}}}{W}$ ) across IG vs. singleton conditions, as well as (III) the ratio of the final syllable (σ f ) to the entire word ( $\frac{{{{\bf{\sigma }}_f}}}{W}$ ) in the IG vs. singleton condition. Duration ratios were used because target sentences differ in numbers of syllables, and we considered duration ratios of syllables to yield a more stable measurement across sentences with different numbers of syllables. Furthermore, (IV) we analyzed the difference in duration ratios between $\frac{{{{\bf{\sigma }}_f}}}{W}$ $\frac{{{{\bf{\sigma }}_i}}}{W}$ in the singleton and IG conditions. For the duration ratio, no outliers were excluded.

f0 values were calculated by using the MATLAB pitch() function, namely, the normalized correlation function algorithm (Atal Reference Atal1972) with a 30 ms window length and 29 ms overlap. F0 values were normalized by converting Hz to z-scores, separately for each participant, to remove anatomically driven differences in f0 due to the participant’s sex and age. Five trials of the vowels in final syllables were excluded because the f0 vectors contained less than two data points per vowel. F0 contours were smoothed using smoothing splines to remove microprosodic effects due to neighboring segments and potential pitch tracking artifacts. In particular, cubic spline interpolants were fit to the raw f0 data and their time points to obtain a smoother f0 contour that is amenable to analysis with polynomial growth curve functions. Finally, f0 contours were time-warped to a fixed length, a necessary step for the growth curve analyses.

Sound Pressure Level (SPL) normalized intensity was calculated by transforming the root mean squared amplitude of the signal to dB and normalizing it to human auditory threshold using the formula $20 \times lo{g_{10}}\frac{I}{{I0}}$ , where I is the amplitude of the signal, and I0 represents the normalizing term for the auditory threshold of a 1000 Hz sine wave, equal to $2 \times {10^{ - 5}}$ (Huang et al. Reference Huang, Acero and Hon2001: 22). Intensity values were calculated with a 30 ms window length and 29 ms overlap to match the f0 windowing. Next, intensity values in dB were converted to the z-score separately by participant to remove differences among participants due to recording conditions. No outliers were excluded except for five trials of the vowels in final syllables in which the intensity vectors contained fewer than two data points. Finally, intensity contours were also time-warped to obtain a fixed length.

We used a linear predictive coding algorithm in MATLAB (Yao et al. Reference Yao, Tilsen, Sprouse and Johnson.2010) to extract vowels F1 and F2. The audio signals were first downsampled to 8 kHz. LPC order was adjusted per the speaker’s sex: twelve coefficients for male speakers, and ten coefficients for female speakers. Minimum and maximum F1 and F2 values were set for each vowel, as required by the algorithm and to prevent erroneous tracking during the Viterbi step of formant tracking, on the basis of Iemwanthong’s (Reference Iemwanthong2008) report on the acoustics of PM vowels. We first extracted the acoustic measurements with fixed window length of 30 ms and 15 ms time step. Values at the midpoint of the trajectories were selected because all the target vowels in this study are monophthongs. Tokens with a duration shorter than 30 ms and those whose F1 and F2 were outside the expected frequency range were excluded. These amounted to twenty-three tokens for F1 of the first vowels, nineteen tokens for F2 of the first vowels, ninety-two tokens for F1 of the second vowels, and thirty-two tokens for F2 of the second vowels.

For spectral tilt measures, we used H1−A2 and H1−A3 because these were reported to be significant acoustic correlates of prominence in Papuan Malay (Kaland Reference Kaland2019), even though the cue is not significant in Ambonese Malay (Maskikit-Essed & Gussenhoven Reference Maskikit-Essed and Gussenhoven2016). First, we calculated FFT spectra, using the MATLAB fft() function (Frigo & Johnson Reference Frigo and Johnson1998). We used Hamming windows with 1,024 samples, or approximately 25 ms, extracted from the middle of the vowels. The amplitudes were then transformed into dB and normalized to human auditory threshold using the formula for intensity. The first harmonics, or H1, were extracted from the first peak of the FFT spectrum. A2 was extracted from the highest peak of the FFT spectrum closest to the F2 and A3 extracted from the highest peak of the FFT spectrum closest to the F3. The F2 and F3 values were extracted using the same method as that used to extract formant frequency in the prior paragraph. We did not correct the H1, A2, and A3 values, because Kaland (Reference Kaland2019) reported for another Malay variety that uncorrected values are better acoustic correlates for stress than corrected values.

4.4 Data analysis

Effects of IGs and phrasal position on duration, F1, F2, H1-A2, H1-A3, the mean intensity difference between the final and the initial syllables, and the mean f0 difference between the final and the initial syllables were tested by hierarchically building nested linear mixed-effect regression models. We started with models that contained random intercepts only. The random intercepts for the models with duration include speaker and onset, while the random intercepts for the other models include speaker, onset, and vowel. We then tested whether adding fixed effects for the presence of IGs, for phrasal position, and for the interaction between these two fixed effects improved model fit. Each time a fixed effect was added to the model, random slopes were included in the model. Contrast was coded such that singleton and phrase-medial positions represent the baseline values for the presence of IGs and for phrasal position, respectively. The random effects for models with duration as the dependent variable include speaker and manner/place of the syllable onset segments. On the other hand, the random effects for models with formant frequencies or spectral tilts as a dependent variable include speaker and vowel quality. Because we did not control for vowels in initial and final syllables, formant analysis was limited to a comparison of singletons/IGs and phrase-medial/phrase-final positions, i.e., no comparison between syllables was conducted. Linear mixed-effect models were compared using MATLAB compare() function to perform likelihood ratio tests. The test statistics ( ${\chi ^2}$ ) and the p-values reported are the result of the likelihood ratio tests.

F0 and intensity contours were analyzed using third-order orthogonal polynomial growth curve analysis. Effects of the presence of IGs and phrasal position were tested by hierarchically building nested models (Mirman Reference Mirman2014). We started with the models containing only a random effect for subject and word but neutralized for the singleton/geminate difference to avoid collinearity with the fixed effect for IG presence. We then tested whether adding fixed effects for the presence of IGs, for phrasal position, and for the interaction between these two fixed effects improved model fit. If the fixed effects improved the model fit, each time, we added the interactions of the fixed effects with orthogonal linear, quadratic, and cubic time (Mirman Reference Mirman2014). The fixed effect indicates differences in mean across conditions; the interaction with linear time indicates differences in slope, and the interaction with quadratic and cubic time indicates differences in shape and curvature. The random effects included in these models are random intercepts for subject and word. Growth curve models were compared using the anova() function in R which performs a likelihood ratio test. The test statistics ( ${\chi ^2}$ ) and the p-values reported are the result of the likelihood ratio tests between nested models. When the best fitting models were found to have an interaction term between IGs and utterance position, the test statistics reported were obtained from a comparison with a model that has fixed effects for IG and utterance position. On the other hand, when the best fitting models were found to have terms for both IGs and utterance position, but no interaction term, the test statistics come from a comparison with a model that has fixed effects for IG or utterance position, for which we report only the lower ${\chi ^2}$ . Finally, when neither IGs nor utterance position were found to have significant effects, the test statistics come from a comparison with a null model containing no fixed effects, but only random effects. Effects are reported as significant based on the approximate t-statistic returned by mixed effect models, based on the ratio of estimated coefficients to their standard error and n-p degrees of freedom where n is the number of observations and p the number of parameters (Gałecki & Burzykowski Reference Gałecki and Burzykowski2013). The models fitted for each dependent variable are reported in Appendix B.

5 Results

5.1 Duration

Before discussing the effects of different onsets, we first studied whether IGs and singletons onsets are reliably distinguished in duration. We found that the raw duration of IGs is longer than that of singletons ( $\chi _{\left( 1 \right)}^2$ = 6.07, p = .01), with an estimated effect size of 17 ms (95 $\%$ CI [6, 28] ms). We also found that IGs are longer than singletons in the duration ratio of the onset to the entire word ( $\frac{C}{W}$ ) ( $\chi _{\left( 1 \right)}^2$ = 7.44, p = .006), with an estimated effect size of .03 (95 $\%$ CI [.012, .048]). The differences between singletons and IGs in both raw and normalized duration suggest two distinct categories.

At the syllable level, we found that both phrasal position and the presence of IG onsets have a significant effect on the duration ratio of the initial syllable to the word $(\frac{{{{{\sigma }}_i}}}{W}$ ), but their interaction does not ( $\chi _{\left( 1 \right)}^2$ = 5.10, p = .023). In phrase-final position, initial syllables have a lower duration ratio to the word in comparison to final syllables, with an estimated effect size of −0.1 (95 $\%$ CI [−0.13, −0.07]). The presence of IGs makes the duration ratio of initial syllables to the word higher than the duration ratio of final syllables to the word by approximately 0.015 (95 $\%$ CI [0.004, 0.026]; Figure 3).

Figure 3. Left: duration ratio of initial syllables to the entire word phrase-medially. Right: duration ratio of initial syllables to the entire word phrase-finally. Dashed line marks 0.5 of syllable duration.

Similarly, we found that both phrasal position and the presence of IG onsets have a significant effect on the duration ratio of the final syllable to the word, but their interaction does not ( $\chi _{\left( 1 \right)}^2$ = 5.10, p = .023). In phrase-final position, final syllables have a higher duration ratio to the word in comparison to initial syllables, with an estimated effect size of 0.1 (95 $\%$ CI [0.07, 0.13]). The presence of IGs makes the duration ratio of the final syllable to the word lower than the duration ratio of initial syllables to the word by approximately −0.015 (95 $\%$ CI [−0.026, −0.004]; Figure 4).

Finally, we found that both phrasal position and the presence of IG onsets influence the difference in the duration ratio between the final and initial syllable, but their interaction does not ( $\chi _{\left( 1 \right)}^2$ = 5.10, p = .023). In phrase-final position, the difference in the duration ratio of the final and initial syllable increases in comparison to the duration ratio in phrase-medial position, with an estimated effect size of 0.2 (95 $\%$ CI [0.15, 0.26]). The presence of IGs makes the difference in duration ratios smaller in comparison to the difference in words with singleton onsets by approximately −0.03 (95 $\%$ CI [−0.05, −0.008]).

Figure 4. Left: duration ratio of the final syllables to the entire word phrase-medially. Right: duration ratio of the final syllables to the entire word phrase-finally.

5.2 Intensity

Our main findings for intensity show that IGs do not have strong effect, besides a small effect on curvature. Position has a strong effect on both vowels: intensity contours are lower in utterance-final position, and they also have steeper fall over words. We also observe an interaction: intensity on initial syllables is higher after IGs than after singleton, but only in final position. This finding showcases an interaction between IGs and phrasal boundaries that results in steeper intensity falls over words with IGs appearing in phrase-final position.

For intensity of the initial syllable, a stepwise likelihood ratio test suggests that the interaction of the presence of IGs and position with linear time, quadratic time, and cubic time are significant ( $\chi _{\left( 4 \right)}^2$ = 833.23, p <.001).

No significant effect of the presence of IGs on the mean intensity of the initial syllable vowels was observed. By contrast, we found a significant effect for position, with an estimated effect size of −0.26 z-scores. This finding indicates that vowels in phrase-final words have lower intensity than those in phrase-medial words, by −1.44 dB to −2.09 dB depending on the intensity range of the speaker. Similarly, the interaction of the presence of IGs and position has a significant effect, with an estimated effect size of 0.11 z-scores. This finding indicates that the vowels after IGs have an additional increase in mean intensity only in utterance-final position, estimated to be 0.61 dB to 0.89 dB depending on the intensity range of the speaker.

We only observe a significant effect of the interaction of linear time and position, with an estimated effect size of −0.10 z-scores (−0.55 to −0.81 dB). However, no effect of the interaction of linear time and the presence of IGs nor the interactions among linear time, presence of IGs, and position were observed. These findings indicate that position in the utterance influences the intensity contours’ slope but that the presence of IGs and their interaction with position does not. The effect of the interaction of quadratic time and the presence of IGs is significant. The effect of the interaction of quadratic time and position is also significant. However, no effect of the interaction among quadratic time, presence of IGs, and position was observed. These findings indicate that the presence of IGs and position affect the curvature of the intensity contour but that their interaction does not. Finally, none of the interaction with cubic time is significant. Figure 5 presents the model fit to the data.

Figure 5. Left: model fit of the intensity contour of the initial syllable vowels in phrase-medial position; shaded area represents two standard errors above and below the empirical mean. Right: model fit of the intensity contour of the initial syllable vowels in phrase-final position.

For intensity of the final syllable, a stepwise likelihood ratio test suggests that the interaction of the presence of IGs and position with linear time, quadratic time, and cubic time are significant ( $\chi _{\left( 4 \right)}^2$ = 280.84, p <.001).

The effect of the presence of IGs on the mean intensity of the final syllable vowels is significant, with an estimated effect size of 0.04 z-scores. This finding indicates that the vowels in the final syllables of words with IGs have higher mean intensity than those of words with singleton onsets; the estimated effect size is 0.22 dB to 0.32 dB, depending on the speakers’ range. The effect of position is significant, with an estimated effect size of approximately −0.60 z-scores. This finding indicates that the vowels in the final syllables of phrase-final words have lower mean intensity than those of phrase-medial words by approximately −3.32 dB to −4.83 dB. The effect of the interaction of the presence of IGs and position is also significant, with an estimated effect size of −0.05 z-scores. This finding indicates that the vowels after IGs have an additional decrease in mean intensity in the final position of an estimated −0.28 dB to −0.40 dB.

Figure 6. Left: model fit of the intensity contour of the final syllable vowels in phrase-medial position. Right: model fit of the intensity contour of the final syllable vowels in phrase-final position.

Figure 7. Left: intensity difference of the final minus initial syllable phrase-medially. Right: intensity difference of the final minus initial syllable phrase-finally.

No significant effect was observed for the interaction of linear time and the presence of IGs, but we found a significant effect for the interaction of linear time and position, with an effect size of approximately −2.44 z-scores (−13.53 to −19.74 dB). Similarly, the effect of the interaction among linear time, presence of IGs, and position is significant, with an effect size of approximately −0.21 z-scores (−1.16 to −1.69 dB). These findings indicate that position and the interaction of the presence of IGs and position affect intensity contours’ slope, but the presence of IGs does not. Similarly, the interaction of quadratic time and the presence of IGs is not significant. The effect of the interaction of quadratic time and position is significant. The interaction among quadratic time, presence of IGs, and position is also significant. No effect was observed for the interaction between cubic time with the presence of IGs, and the interaction among cubic time, presence of IGs, and position. However, the effect of the interaction of cubic time and position is significant. Figure 6 illustrates the model fit.

Finally, the interaction between the presence of IG onsets and phrasal position influences the difference in intensity of the vowels in the final and initial syllables ( $\chi _{\left( 1 \right)}^2$ = 11.85, p < .001). The presence of IGs in phrase-final position corresponds with a more negative difference in intensity between the final and initial syllables, with an estimated effect size of −0.12 z-scores (95 $\%$ CI [−0.19, −0.05]); this finding corresponds to a range of −0.66 dB to −0.97 dB (Figure 7). Note that even though the best model includes terms for IGs, position, and their interaction, we only observe a significant effect for the interaction term.

Table 4 shows the summary of statistical analyses of intensity.

Table 4 Summary of statistical analyses of intensity (significance and effect size)

***p < .001, ** 0.001 < p < 0.01, * 0.01 < p < 0.05, n.s. p > 0.05

5.3 Fundamental frequency

Our findings show that words with IGs have slightly higher f0 on both syllables, but the effect is small. Position has a strong effect on f0: f0 contours are rising phrase-medially but falling phrase-finally. As for the interaction between position and IGs, words with IGs in phrase-final position have a less steep fall than predicted by the addition of individual effects. The outcome is that f0 fall is comparable across words with IGs and singletons.

For f0 of the initial syllable, the stepwise likelihood ratio test suggests the interaction between the presence of IGs and position with linear time, quadratic time, and cubic time are significant ( $\chi _{\left( 4 \right)}^2$ = 12.80, p =.012).

For vowels of initial syllables, the effect of the presence of IGs, the position in the phrase, and the interaction of the presence of IGs and position on mean f0 are significant. The effect size of the presence of IGs is approximately 0.01 z-scores. This finding indicates that the vowels following IGs have a higher mean f0 than those following singletons, by 0.23 to 0.4 Hz, depending on the speaker’s f0 range. The effect size of the position is approximately −0.23 z-scores. This finding indicates that the vowels in phrase-final position have a lower mean f0 than those in phrase-medial position, by −5.17 to −9.2 Hz, depending on the speaker’s f0 range. The effect size of the interaction between the presence of IGs and position is approximately 0.01 z-scores. This finding indicates that vowels after IGs have an additional increase of mean f0 in phrase-final position, estimated to be from 0.23 to 0.40 Hz.

The effect of the interaction between linear time and the presence of IGs is also significant, with an estimated effect size of approximately −0.08 z-scores (−1.8 to −3.2 Hz), and that between linear time and phrasal position is significant, with an estimated effect size of approximately −0.17 z-scores (−3.85 to −6.81 Hz). We also observed the interaction among linear time, presence of IGs, and position, with an estimated effect size of approximately −0.07 z-scores (−1.58 to −2.8 Hz). These findings indicate that the presence of IGs and position and their interaction influence the f0 slope. We observed the interaction between quadratic time with the presence of IGs, but there was no effect of the phrasal position, or their interaction. The effect of the interaction between cubic time and the presence of IGs is significant. Similarly, the interaction between cubic time and phrasal position is significant. No effect was observed for the interaction among cubic time, presence of IGs, and position. Figure 8 illustrates the model fit.

Figure 8. Left: model fit of the f0 contour of the initial syllable vowels in phrase-medial position. Right: model fit of the f0 contour of the initial syllable vowels in phrase-final position.

For f0 of the final syllable, the stepwise likelihood ratio test suggests that the interaction of the presence of IGs and position with linear time, quadratic time, and cubic time is significant ( $\chi _{\left( 4 \right)}^2$ = 455.28, p <.001).

Figure 9. Left: model fit of the f0 contour of the final syllable vowels in phrase-medial position. Right: model fit of the f0 contour of the final syllable vowels in phrase-final position.

Figure 10. Left: f0 difference of final minus initial syllable phrase-medially. Right: f0 difference of the final syllable minus the initial syllable phrase-finally.

For vowels in final syllables, a significant effect is observed for the presence of IGs, with an estimated effect size of approximately 0.09 z-scores. This finding indicates that vowels of final syllables in words with IGs have higher mean f0 than those in words without IGs, by 2.03 to 3.6 Hz. The effect of phrasal position is also significant, with an estimated effect size of approximately −1.28 z-scores. This finding indicates that vowels of final syllables in phrase-final position have lower mean f0 than those in phrase-medial position, by −28.8 to −51.2 Hz. The interaction of the presence of IGs and phrasal position also has a significant effect on mean f0, with an estimated effect size of approximately −0.09 z-scores. This finding indicates that the final syllable vowels in words with IGs have an additional decrease in mean f0 in phrase-final position, estimated to be −2.03 to −3.6 Hz.

No significant effect was observed for the interaction between linear time and the presence of IGs, but the effect of the interaction between linear time and position is significant, with an estimated effect size of approximately −2.24 z-scores (−50.4 to −89.6 Hz). The effect of interaction among linear time, the presence of IGs, and phrasal position is also significant, with an estimated effect size of approximately 0.57 z-scores (12.83–22.8 Hz). These findings indicate that position and the interaction of the presence of IGs and position affect the f0 slope but that the presence of IGs does not. Additionally, the interaction between quadratic time and the presence of IGs is significant. Similarly, the interaction between quadratic time and phrasal position is significant. The effect of the interaction among quadratic time, the presence of IGs, and position is also significant. No effect was observed for the interaction between cubic time with the presence of IGs or the interaction among cubic time, the presence of IGs, and position. The effect of the interaction of cubic time and position is significant. Figure 9 illustrates the model fit.

Finally, we conducted a comparison between initial and final syllable differences in f0. The best performing model is one with a term for position only ( $\chi _{\left( 1 \right)}^2$ = 12.15, p < .001). The results demonstrate that phrasal position influences the difference in f0 between the final and initial syllable and that phrase-final position reduces the difference in f0 between the final and initial syllables, with an estimated effect size of −0.66 z-scores (95 $\%$ CI [−0.96, −0.36]); this finding corresponds to a range from −14.93 to −26.45 Hz (Figure 10).

Table 5 shows the summary of statistical analyses of f0.

Table 5 Summary of statistical analyses of f0 (significance and effect size)

***p < .001, ** 0.001 < p < 0.01, * 0.01 < p < 0.05

5.4 Formants

We found no effects of the presence of IGs, phrasal position, or their interaction on either F1 ( $\chi _{\left( 3 \right)}^2$ = 4.40, p = .22) or F2 ( $\chi _{\left( 3 \right)}^2$ = 1.24, p = .74) of vowels in initial syllables. The presence of IGs and phrasal position, as well as their interaction had no effect on either the F1 ( $\chi _{\left( 3 \right)}^2$ = 4.61, p = .20) or F2 ( $\chi _{\left( 3 \right)}^2$ = 3.13, p = .37) of the vowels in final syllables.

5.5 Spectral tilt

A significant effect is observed for phrasal position on initial syllable vowel H1−A2 ( $\chi _{\left( 1 \right)}^2$ = 6.63, p = .01). In phrase-final position, initial syllable vowels have a larger H1−A2 than in phrase-medial position, with an estimated effect size of 2.25 dB (95 $\%$ CI [0.95, 3.55] dB). There is also a significant effect of phrasal position on initial syllable vowels H1−A3 ( $\chi _{\left( 1 \right)}^2$ = 4.95, p = .026). In phrase-final position, initial syllable vowels have a larger H1−A3 than in phrase-medial position, with an estimated effect size of 2.45 dB (95 $\%$ CI [0.73, 4.18]) (Figure 11). No significant effect is observed for the presence of IGs and the interaction between the presence of IGs and position.

Figure 11. Left: H1−A2 (top) and H1−A3 (bottom) of initial syllable vowels in phrase-medial position. Right: H1−A2 (top) and H1−A3 (bottom) of initial syllable vowels in phrase-final position.

Similar effects are found for the final syllable vowels. The effect of phrasal position on final syllable vowel H1−A2 is significant ( $\chi _{\left( 1 \right)}^2$ = 5.89, p = .015). In phrase-final position, final syllable vowels have a larger H1−A2 than in phrase-medial position, with an estimated effect size of 3.06 dB (95 $\%$ CI [0.83, 5.30] dB). There is also a marginally significant effect of phrasal position on final syllable vowel H1−A3 ( $\chi _{\left( 1 \right)}^2$ = 3.91, p = .048). In phrase-final position, final syllable vowels have a larger H1−A3 than in phrase-medial position, with an estimated effect size of 2.49 dB (95 $\%$ CI [0.19, 4.79] dB) (Figure 12). No significant effect is observed for the presence of IGs and the interaction between the presence of IGs and position.

Figure 12. Left: H1−A2 (top) and H1−A3 (bottom) of final syllable vowels in phrase-medial position. Right: H1−A2 (top) and H1−A3 (bottom) of final syllable vowels in phrase-final position.

6 Discussion

We now return to our two research questions:

  1. (i) Does the presence of IGs affect the prominence profile of PM words?

  2. (ii) Does position in the utterance affect the prominence profile of PM words?

For (i), our experimental findings do not support the claim that the presence of IGs categorically affects the prominence profiles of PM words; instead, they have only small phonetic effects on syllable duration, intensity, and f0. Significant differences are not observed in the relative prominence profiles of words with and without IGs. For (ii), the presence of phrasal boundary has categorical effects on the relative f0 of initial and final syllables because words in phrase-medial position show a rising f0 contour, and words in phrase-final position show a falling f0 contour.

Initial syllables with IG onsets have a consistently higher syllable-to-word duration ratio than initial syllables without IG onsets. The analysis of onset raw duration and duration ratios suggests that the effect is due to IGs being longer than the corresponding singletons. A mirror image effect is observed in final syllables: final syllable duration ratios exhibit a small decrease when words contain IGs because IGs increase the raw duration of initial syllables. Nonetheless, final syllables always display a longer duration ratio to the word than initial syllables, regardless of the presence of IGs (Figure 13, top).

Figure 13. Duration ratio (top), intensity contour (middle), and f0 contour (bottom) of words with IG and singleton onsets.

Initial geminates also have small effects on the intensity of the vowels after them, with the clearest effect occurring in phrase-final position where vowels after IGs have a higher intensity than those after singletons (Figure 13, middle). In other words, how strongly IGs affect intensity appears to be related to position in the phrase as well. In terms of relative prominence, intensity is always slightly higher on initial syllables than on final syllables, regardless of the presence of IGs. It is worth noting that prominent syllables are found to have higher intensity than non-prominent ones cross-linguistically (Gordon & Roettger Reference Gordon and Roettger2017).

Similar increases due to the presence of IGs are observed for f0, but the effect size is even smaller. Furthermore, words with and without IGs do not have different f0 profiles in the same phrasal position (Figure 13, bottom). No effect of the presence of IGs is observed for vowel formants or spectral tilt. Overall, the presence of IGs has small effects corresponding to longer duration ratios and higher intensity on the vowel of the initial syllables, especially when the words appear phrase-finally, and a general increase in f0 for both the initial and final syllable vowels. These findings demonstrate that the effects of IGs are subtle and not in line with a phonological change of stress or pitch accent position.

By contrast, the effects of phrasal position are significantly stronger than the effect of the presence of IGs. Occurrence in phrase-final position strongly increases the raw duration of final syllables, decreasing the duration ratio of the initial syllable to the word, and the duration ratio of the final syllable to the word (Figure 13, top).

Phrasal position also correlates with categorical differences in intensity and f0 contours. Namely, words in phrase-final position have lower intensity and f0 for both initial and final syllable vowels than those in phrase-medial position (Figure 13, middle and bottom). We also found a stronger effect on the vowels in final syllables than on the vowels in initial syllables. The final syllable vowels in phrase-final position show a steeper intensity fall than final syllable vowels in phrase-medial position (Figure 13, middle). Similarly, final syllable vowels in phrase-final position show falling f0 contours, and final syllable vowels in phrase-medial position show rising f0 contours (Figure 13, bottom). It is important to note that the falling f0 contour in phrase-final position is commonly observed cross-linguistically. This phenomenon is known as f0 declination (Ladd Reference Ladd1984).

No effect on vowel formants is observed. We also observed an effect of phrasal position on the spectral tilt of initial and final syllable vowels. The H1−A2 and H1−A3 values are the most positive in phrase-final position. In other words, the decrease in energy from the first harmonic to the higher frequency range (A2 or A3) is larger when words appear in phrase-final position than when words appear in phrase-medial position. Based on previous reports of spectral tilt as a correlate of prominence (Gordon & Roettger Reference Gordon and Roettger2017), our observation is that vowels of both initial and final syllables in phrase-final position seem to be similar to less prominent vowels in that they have a steeper intensity roll-off. Table 6 summarizes the effects of the presence of IGs and phrasal position.

Table 6 Direction of the effects of the presence of IGs and phrasal position on duration ratio, z-scored intensity, z-scored f0, vowel formants (Hz), H1−A2 (dB), and H1−A3 (dB). Significant effects are presented with their estimated effect size

In summary, the effects of IGs are less salient than those of phrasal position; this finding is evident from the difference in the order of magnitude of the effects. Moreover, the effects of IGs seem to be amplified in phrase-final position, thus, showcasing a hitherto unnoticed interesting interaction between these two factors.

Based on our experimental results, one open question is why PM has been reported to have final stress and stress shift in words with IGs (e.g., Chotikakamthorn Reference Chotikakamthorn1981; Yupho Reference Yupho1989). Previous studies (Section 1.2) elicited PM words in isolation or as a final word in a carrier sentence. In other words, the prominence patterns reported in the literature reflect prominence in phrase-final position. Our study shows that the presence of IGs has stronger effects when words appear in phrase-final position. We found intensity and f0 boosts due to the presence of IGs on initial syllable vowels in phrase-final position. In phrase-medial position no intensity boost and a less strong f0 boost on initial syllable vowels are observed. The presence of a phrase-final boundary decreases the intensity and f0 of final syllable vowels in words with IGs, even more than it does in words without IGs (Table 4 and Table 5). Thus, in phrase-final position, words with IGs have higher intensity and f0 on initial syllables than words without IGs. However, on final syllables, the values are comparable. Combining these two effects, we observed that words with IGs show a steeper fall of intensity and f0 over the word than words without IGs in phrase-final position.

In view of these findings, one possibility is that previous reports of categorical effects of IGs are due to the steeper falling f0 and intensity profiles compared to words with singletons. This sharper fall may lead to the perception of a stress shift, especially for native speakers of Thai (e.g., Chotikakamthorn Reference Chotikakamthorn1981; Yupho Reference Yupho1989), who perceive falling contours as posttonic (Gandour Reference Gandour and Luangthongkam1979). If researchers have been assuming that PM is a non-tonal language and somewhat similar to a prototypical stress language, like English, the higher pitch and intensity on the initial syllable and the sharp falling contour in IG words may have been interpreted as stress on the initial syllable, especially by Thai field linguists. Additionally, because words were possibly elicited in isolation, that is, phrase-finally, final lengthening of the final syllable may be perceived as stress in non-IG words, where initial syllables have lower intensity and f0 and a less steep falling contour over the word.

We conclude with a final remark on perhaps the single most famous claim on PM IGs, namely, their alleged moraic status (Hajek & Goedemans Reference Hajek and Goedemans2003; Topintzi Reference Topintzi2008). In previous work, PM IGs have been described as a case of moraic onset, based on the claim that IGs attract stress: words with IGs have initial stress, while final stress is the default. However, our data suggest that IGs do not affect prominence profiles of words in PM in the categorical manner predicted by this account. Our findings cast doubt on the claim that PM IGs trigger a stress shift. This, in turn, invalidates the only positive evidence for the moraic status of IGs in PM. Regarding stress or prominence, our findings suggest that PM is more similar to most Malay varieties in lacking clear evidence for stress and the contemporaneous usage of f0 and other acoustic cues typically attributed to word-level prominence.

Some limitations of the present work should also be acknowledged. First, our data collection procedures and stimuli are more typical of a corpus collection rather than tightly controlled lab speech. Additional limitations result from a tradeoff between constructing natural sounding PM sentences and control over stimuli and carriers of segmental composition. For instance, differences in length of the carrier sentence, the use of different vowels across initial syllables, as well as in initial and final syllables of the same word, are all factors that introduce more variability in measurements like duration, due to compression in longer sentences, and f0 and intensity, due to intrinsic f0 and intensity effects. At the same, they also prevent us from conducting certain analyses, like a comparison of formants across the two vowels in the same word. We tried to account for this type of variation by presenting both raw and normalized measurements, like ratios and deltas. Furthermore, we also included random effects in the model that capture word-specific variations due to segmental composition and context, as each word is embedded in a unique context.

A final disclaimer worth emphasizing is that this paper presented only (acoustic) production evidence. Acoustic evidence seems to broadly agree with previous reports of longer duration, higher f0, and intensity (only in phrase-final position) after IGs. The effects do not seem, however, to be compatible with previous claims of suprasegmental changes to words in the presence of IGs; as the acoustic profiles of words with and without IGs are comparable. Perceptual work is, however, necessary, to ascertain how the acoustic patterns reported may be mapped to the perception of phonological contrasts in PM both in terms of segmental and of suprasegmental information, as well as their interaction. Especially given that recent work has shown that the perception of cues to duration is modulated by prosody (e.g., Steffman Reference Steffman2019). However, this will not be an easy task given the complex interplay of acoustic cues underlying IGs contrasts in PM (Abramson Reference Abramson1986, Reference Abramson, Solé, Recasens and Romero2003) and cross-linguistically (Burroni et al. Reference Burroni, Lau-Preechathammarach, Maspong, Jurgec, Duncan, Elfner, Kang, Kochetov, O’Neill, Ozburn, Rice, Sanders, Schertz, Shaftoe and Sullivan2022).

Despite these limitations, our results broadly confirm the findings of previous work where IGs, examined in words recorded in isolation, caused increases in f0 and intensity over the vowels following them. This is in line with acoustic work on IGs in other languages, where concomitant changes in f0 and intensity are also present (Burroni et al. Reference Burroni, Lau-Preechathammarach, Maspong, Jurgec, Duncan, Elfner, Kang, Kochetov, O’Neill, Ozburn, Rice, Sanders, Schertz, Shaftoe and Sullivan2022), but no stress shift process in their presence has been alleged. Our work additionally contributed by showing that some of these effects are probably due to the interaction of IGs and prosodic boundaries, which was not examined in previous work. In sum, both tightly controlled and more naturalistic speech point in the same direction. Future work on PM could be dedicated to uncovering how the differences between IGs and singletons may vary as a function of communicative context, for instance, in the presence of an interlocutor or in different speech tasks.

7 Conclusion

In this paper, we have studied the interaction between syllabic prominence, the presence of IGs, and phrasal boundaries in PM. We conclude that IGs do not trigger categorically different prominence profiles as claimed in the literature, but do correspond to a subtle acoustic modulation of duration, intensity, and f0. Additionally, a phrasal boundary triggers categorical changes that can be considered phonological effects, as shown by the difference in duration, intensity, f0, and spectral tilt. We also uncovered an interaction between IGs and prosodic boundaries, which may be responsible for the perception of stress shift reported in the literature. Our acoustic investigation has also allowed us to contextualize PM among other Malay varieties. PM looks similar to other Malay varieties, for example, Besemah and Malaysian Malay, in which duration and f0 contours are dissociated from each other. In summary, we conclude that PM is less typologically exceptional than previously claimed in the phonological literature.

Acknowledgment

This study is built upon findings from a distinct pilot project concluded in 2014 and financially supported by the Ratchadaphiseksomphot Endowment Fund of Chulalongkorn University (RES560530179-HS). We are especially grateful to Jakrabhop Iamdanush for his essential involvement in the pilot project, to D. Robert Ladd for his insightful comments, and to the audiences at the twenty-fourth Annual Meeting of the Southeast Asian Linguistic Society and seventeenth Conference on Laboratory Phonology for their valuable feedback. We extend our thanks to the associate editor at JIPA and the anonymous reviewers for their constructive insights. Our gratitude also goes to Ruslan Uthai and Santhawat Thanyawong for their assistance in connecting with the community and facilitating the fieldwork, to Nur Ida ibni Sulaiman for providing valuable resources, and to the Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, Prince of Songkhla University, Pattani Campus, for allowing us to use their space to conduct the fieldwork. We appreciate Nirohani Chansang, Haphifi Sama-ae, and Kanura Aphibanbae for their language input, and all the participants for generously contributing their time and patience.

Author contribution

This research article is the outcome of a collaborative effort among all the listed authors. Burroni and Maspong contributed equally to this work and share first authorship, while Pittayaporn is the principal investigator. All four authors has made substantial contributions to the conception, design, execution, and interpretation of the research work. For clarity and to facilitate academic recruitment processes, individual author responsibilities are delineated as follows:

Francesco Burroni: Sections 4.3, 4.4, 5.1, 5.3, 5.4, 6

Sireemas Maspong: Sections 2.1, 2.2, 3, 4.3, 4.4, 5.2, 5.5

Pittayawat Pittayaporn: Sections 1, 2.1, 7

Pimthip Kochaiyaphum: Sections 4.1, 4.2

While the aforementioned divisions are indicative of the primary contributions, it is important to emphasize that the collaborative nature of this work involved iterative discussions, mutual feedback, and collective decision-making among all authors. As such, the final manuscript represents the synergistic efforts of the entire team.

Appendix A

Table A1 Prompts

Appendix B

Statistical models (Best fitting models)

Duration

Onset Duration ∼ IG + (IG |Speaker) + (IG|Onset Voicing/Place/Manner)

Onset Duration Ratio ∼ IG + Position + (IG + Position|Speaker) + (IG + Position|Onset Voicing/Place/Manner)

Syllable 1 Ratio ∼ IG + Position + (IG + Position|Speaker) + (IG + Position|Onset Voicing/Place/Manner)

Syllable 2 Ratio ∼ IG + Position + (IG + Position|Speaker) + (IG + Position|Onset Voicing/Place/Manner)

Delta Syllable 2 - Syllable 1 Ratio ∼ IG + Position + (IG + Position|Speaker) + (IG + Position|Onset Voicing/Place/Manner)

Intensity

Delta Syllable 2 - Syllable 1 Intensity ∼ IG * Position + (IG + Position|Speaker) + (IG + Position|Onset Voicing/Place/Manner) + (IG + Position|Vowel)

Formants

V1/2 F1/F2 ∼ 1 _(IG + Position|Speaker) + (IG + Position|Onset Voicing/Place/Manner) + (IG + Position|Vowel)

Spectral tilt

V1/2 H1–A2/H1–A3 ∼ position + (IG + Position|Speaker) + (IG + Position|Onset Voicing/Place/Manner) + (IG + Position|Vowel)

Delta V1–V2 H1–A2/H1–A3 ∼ 1 + (IG + Position|Speaker) + (IG + Position|Onset Voicing/Place/Manner) + (IG + Position|Vowel)

Footnotes

These authors contribute equally to this work and share first authorship.

References

Abramson, Arthur S. 1986. The perception of word-initial consonant length: Pattani Malay. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 16(1), 816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abramson, Arthur S. 1987. Word-initial consonant length in Pattani Malay. International Congress of Phonetic Association 6, 6870.Google Scholar
Abramson, Arthur S. 1998. The complex acoustic output of a single articulatory gesture: Pattani Malay word-initial consonant length. In Warotamasikkhadit, Udom and Panakul, Thanyarat (eds.), Papers from the Fourth Annual Meeting of the Southeast Asian Linguistics Society, 120, Arizona State University, Program for Southeast Asian Studies.Google Scholar
Abramson, Arthur S. 2003. Acoustic cues to word-initial stop length in Pattani Malay. In Solé, Maria-Josep, Recasens, Daniel & Romero, Joaquin (eds.), Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS XV), Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, 387390.Google Scholar
Abramson, Arthur S. 2004. Toward prosodic contrast: Suai and Pattani Malay, In Bel, Bernard & Marlien, Isabelle (eds.), International Symposium on Tonal Aspects of Languages: Emphasis on Tone Languages. The Institute of Linguistics, Chinese Academy of Social Science, Beijing, 14.Google Scholar
Atal, Bishnu S. 1972. Automatic speaker recognition based on pitch contours. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 52, 16871697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boersma, Paul & Weenink, David. 2020. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (version 6.1.12). Available at: www.praat.org/ [last accessed 21 September 2023].Google Scholar
Burroni, Francesco, Lau-Preechathammarach, Raksit & Maspong, Sireemas. 2022. Unifying initial geminates and fortis consonants via laryngeal specification: Three case studies from Dunan, Pattani Malay, and Salentino, In Jurgec, Peter, Duncan, Liisa, Elfner, Emily, Kang, Yoonjung, Kochetov, Alexei, O’Neill, Brittney K., Ozburn, Avery, Rice, Keren, Sanders, Nathan, Schertz, Jessamyn, Shaftoe, Nate, & Sullivan, Lisa (eds.), Proceedings of the 2021 Annual Meeting on Phonology, 113. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America.Google Scholar
Burroni, Francesco, Maspong, Sireemas, Pittayaporn, Pittayawat & Kochaiyaphum, Pimthip. 2020. A new look at Pattani Malay Initial Geminates: A statistical and machine learning approach. In Proceedings of the 34th Pacific Asia Conference on Language, Information and Computation, 21–29. Hanoi: Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Byrd, Dani & Saltzman, Elliot. 2003. The elastic phrase: Modeling the dynamics of boundary-adjacent lengthening. Journal of Phonetics 31(2), 149180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chotikakamthorn, Maneerat. 1981. A comparative study of phonology in Satun Malay and Pattani Malay. Master’s thesis, Mahidol University.Google Scholar
Cohn, Abigail C. 1989. Stress in Indonesian and bracketing paradoxes. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 7(2), 167216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, James T. 1989. Malay dialect research in Malaysia: The issue of perspective. Bijdragen tot de Taal-, Land- en Volkenkunde. Journal of the Humanities and Social Sciences of Southeast Asia and Oceania 145(2), 235264.Google Scholar
Davis, Stuart. 2011a. Quantity. In Goldsmith, John, Riggle, Jason & Yu, Alan C. L. (eds.), The handbook of phonological theory, 2nd edn, 103140. Chichester and Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, Stuart. 2011b. Geminates. In Marc van Oostendorp, Colin J. Ewen, Elizabeth Hume & Rice, Keren (eds.), The Blackwell companion to phonology, vol. 1, 837859, Malden, MA & Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Edwards, Jan, Beckman, Mary E. & Fletcher, Janet. 1991. The articulatory kinematics of final lengthening. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 89(1), 369382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frigo, Matteo & Johnson, Steven G.. 1998. FFTW: An adaptive software architecture for the FFT. In The 1998 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing, vol. 3, 1381–1384, Seattle.Google Scholar
Gałecki, Andrzej & Burzykowski, Burzykowski. 2013. Linear mixed-effects models using R: A step-by-step approach (Springer Texts in Statistics). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gandour, Jack. 1979. Tonal rules for English loanwords in Thai. In Luangthongkam, Theraphan (ed.), Studies in Tai and Mon-Khmer phonetics and phonology in honour of E. J. A. Henderson, 94105. Bangkok: Chulalongkorn University Press.Google Scholar
Goedemans, Rob & van Zanten, Ellen. 2007. Stress and accent in Indonesian. Prosody in Indonesian Languages, 35–62. Ultrecht: LOT.Google Scholar
Gordon, Matthew. 2014. Disentangling stress and pitch-accent: A typology of prominence at different prosodic levels. Word stress: Theoretical and typological issues, 83118. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, Matthew & Roettger, Timo. 2017. Acoustic correlates of word stress: A cross-linguistic survey. Linguistics Vanguard 3(1), 20170007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hajek, John & Goedemans, Rob. 2003. Word-initial geminates and stress in Pattani Malay. The Linguistic Review 20(1), 7994. https://doi.org/10.1515/tlir.2003.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halim, Amran. 1974. Intonation in relation to syntax in Bahasa Indonesia. Jakarta: Djambatan.Google Scholar
Hamzah, Hilmi. 2013. The acoustics and perception of the word-initial singleton/geminate contrast in Kelantan Malay. Doctoral dissertation, University of Melbourne.Google Scholar
Hamzah, M. Hilmi, Fletcher, Janet & Hajek, John. 2011. Durational correlates of word-initial voiceless geminate stops: The case of Kelantan Malay. In Lee, Wai Sum & Zee, Eric (eds.), Proceedings of the 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS XVII), 815818. Hong Kong: City University of Hong Kong.Google Scholar
Hamzah, M. Hilmi, Fletcher, Janet & Hajek, John. 2014. Amplitude and f0 as acoustic correlates of Kelantan Malay word-initial geminates. In Hay, Jennifer & Parnell, Emma (eds.), Proceedings of the 15th Australasian International Conference on Speech Science and Technology, 6366. Christchurch: Australian Speech Science and Technology Association.Google Scholar
Heuven, Vincent J. van, Roosman, Lilie M. & van Zanten, Ellen. 2008. Betawi Malay word prosody. Lingua 118, 12711287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heuven, Vincent J. van & van Zanten, Ellen (eds.). 2007. Prosody in Indonesian languages. Utrecht: LOT.Google Scholar
Huang, Xuedong, Acero, Alex & Hon, Hsiao-Wuen. 2001. Spoken language processing: A guide to theory, algorithm, and system development. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall PTR.Google Scholar
Iemwanthong, Kanyarat. 2008. Acoustic characteristics of Pattani Malay vowels. Master’s thesis, Chulalongkorn University.Google Scholar
Kaland, Constantijn. 2019. Acoustic correlates of word stress in Papuan Malay. Journal of Phonetics 74, 5574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaland, Constantijn. 2021. The perception of word stress cues in Papuan Malay: A typological perspective and experimental investigation. Laboratory Phonology. Open Library of Humanities 12(1), 133.Google Scholar
Kaland, Constantijn & Baumann, Stefan. 2020. Demarcating and highlighting in Papuan Malay phrase prosody. Journal of Acoustical Society of America 147(4), 29742988.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kluge, Angela. 2017. A grammar of Papuan Malay (Studies in Diversity Linguistics), vol. 11. Berlin: Language Science Press.Google Scholar
Kraehenmann, Astrid. 2011. Initial geminates. In Marc van Oostendorp, Colin J. Ewen, Elizabeth Hume, & Keren Rice (eds.), The Blackwell companion to phonology, vol. 2, 11241146. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kraehenmann, Astrid & Lahiri, Aditi. 2008. Duration differences in the articulation and acoustics of Swiss German word-initial geminate and singleton stops. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 123, 44464455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krisnapan, Daranee. 1985. A phonology of Taba Malay. Master’s thesis, Mahidol University.Google Scholar
Ladd, D. Robert. 1984. Declination: A review and some hypotheses. Phonology Yearbook 1, 5374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maskikit-Essed, Raechel & Gussenhoven, Carlos. 2016. No stress, no pitch accent, no prosodic focus: The case of Ambonese Malay. Phonology 33, 353389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MATLAB. 2021. version 9.10.0.1602886 (R2021a). Natick, MA: The MathWorks Inc.Google Scholar
McAuliffe, Michael, Michaela Socolof, Sarah Mihuc, Wagner, Michael & Sonderegger, Morgan. 2017. Montreal Forced Aligner. Available at: http://montrealcorpustools.github.io/Montreal-Forced-Aligner/ [last accessed 21 September 2023].Google Scholar
McDonnell, Bradley. 2016. Acoustic correlates of stress in Besemah. NUSA: Linguistic studies of languages in and around Indonesia 60, 128.Google Scholar
Mirman, Daniel. 2014. Growth curve analysis and visualization using R. Boca Raton, CA: Chapman & Hall/CRC the R series.Google Scholar
Mohd Don, Zuraidah, Knowles, Gerry & Yong, Janet. 2008. How words can be misleading: A study of syllable timing and “stress” in Malay. Linguistics Journal 3(2), 6681.Google Scholar
Muller, Jennifer. 2001. The phonology and phonetics of word-initial geminates. Ph.D. dissertation, Ohio State University.Google Scholar
National Statistical Office. 2012. The 2010 population and housing census: Southern region. Bangkok: National Statistical Office.Google Scholar
Pakkhem, Siriwan. 2007. A phonological comparison of Pattani Malay as spoken in Narathiwat, Krabu and Pathumthani. Master’s thesis, Mahidol University.Google Scholar
Paramal, Waemaji. 1991. Long consonants in Pattani Malay: The result of word and phrase shortening. Master’s thesis, Mahidol University.Google Scholar
Phuengnoi, Nattaphon. 2010. An acoustic study of stressed and unstressed syllables in Pattani Malay and Urak Lawoi. Master’s thesis, Chulalongkorn University.Google Scholar
Pittayaporn, Pittayawat & Iamdanush, Jakrabhop. 2014. Metrical stress in Pattani Malay. Presented at the 24th Annual Meeting of Southeast Asian Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Poonsub, Areeluk. 2016. The linguistic identity factors in the question of Southern border provinces: Some policy recommendations for the Thai state. In Yunyasit, Suphatmet (ed.), Collections of studies in the linguistic status in Southern border provinces, 157181. Nakornpathom: Institute of Human Rights and Peace Studies, Mahidol University.Google Scholar
Premsrirat, Suwilai & Burarungrot, Mirinda. 2018. Patani Malay-Thai bi/multilingual education in Thailand’s Deep South. In Premsrirat, Suwilai & Hirsh, David (eds.), Language revitalization: Insights from Thailand, 239250. Bern: Peter Lang.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premsrirat, Suwilai & Burarungrot, Mirinda. 2022. Multilingualism, bi/multilingual education and social inclusion: A case study in Southern Thailand. MANUSYA: Journal of Humanities 24(3), 373389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ridouane, Rachid. 2007. Gemination in Tashlhiyt Berber: An acoustic and articulatory study. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 37, 119142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ringen, Catherine & Vago, Robert M.. 2010. Geminates: Heavy or long? In Charles Cairns & Eric Raimy (eds.), Handbook of the syllable. Leiden: Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smalley, William A. 1994. Linguistic diversity and national unity: Language ecology in Thailand. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Steffman, Jeremy. 2019. Phrase-final lengthening modulates listeners’ perception of vowel duration as a cue to coda stop voicing. Journal of Acoustical Society of America 145, EL560EL566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Topintzi, Nina. 2008. On the existence of moraic onsets. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 26, 147184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Topintzi, Nina. 2010. Onsets: Suprasegmental and prosodic behaviour. Cambridge, UK and New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Topintzi, Nina. 2016. Iquito: The prosodic colon and evaluation of OT stress accounts. In Heinz, Jeffrey, Goedemans, Rob & van der Hulst, Harry (eds.), Dimensions of phonological stress, 123167. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Topintzi, Nina & Davis, Stuart. 2017. On the weight of edge geminates. In Kubozono, Haruo (ed.), The phonetics and phonology of geminate consonants, 260282. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Topintzi, Nina & Nevins, Andrew I.. 2017. Moraic onsets in Arrernte. Phonology 34(4), 615650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turk, Alice E. & Shattuck-Hufnagel, Stefanie. 2007. Multiple targets of phrase-final lengthening in American English words. Journal of Phonetics 35(4), 445472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uthai, Ruslan. 1993. A comparison of word formation in standard Malay and Pattani Malay. Bangkok: Chulalongkorn University.Google Scholar
Uthai, Ruslan. 2011. Keistimewaan dialek Malayu Patani. Bangi: Penerbit Universiti Kebangsaan Malaysia.Google Scholar
Wightman, Colin W., Shattuck-Hufnagel, Stefanie, Ostendorf, Mari & Price, Patti J.. 1992. Segmental durations in the vicinity of prosodic phrase boundaries. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 91(3), 17071717. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.402450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yao, Yao, Tilsen, Sam, Sprouse, Ronald L. & Johnson., Keith 2010. Automated measurement of vowel formants in the Buckeye Corpus. UC Berkeley PhonLab Annual Report 6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yupho, Nawanit. 1989. Consonant clusters and stress rules in Pattani Malay. Mon-Khmer Studies 15, 125137.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Pattani Malay minimal pairs showing lexical contrast for initial singletons and geminates

Figure 1

Table 2 Consonant inventory in Pattani Malay (PM) (Uthai 2011)

Figure 2

Figure 1. f0 contour of disyllabic words with (CːV.CV) and without initial geminates (CV.CV) (Phuengnoi 2010).

Figure 3

Figure 2. Predicted acoustic profiles based on stress shift hypothesis (top) and fixed (final) prominence hypothesis (bottom).

Figure 4

Table 3 The thirteen minimal pairs of disyllabic forms that differed uniquely for the singleton or initial geminate (IG) onsets of the initial syllable

Figure 5

Figure 3. Left: duration ratio of initial syllables to the entire word phrase-medially. Right: duration ratio of initial syllables to the entire word phrase-finally. Dashed line marks 0.5 of syllable duration.

Figure 6

Figure 4. Left: duration ratio of the final syllables to the entire word phrase-medially. Right: duration ratio of the final syllables to the entire word phrase-finally.

Figure 7

Figure 5. Left: model fit of the intensity contour of the initial syllable vowels in phrase-medial position; shaded area represents two standard errors above and below the empirical mean. Right: model fit of the intensity contour of the initial syllable vowels in phrase-final position.

Figure 8

Figure 6. Left: model fit of the intensity contour of the final syllable vowels in phrase-medial position. Right: model fit of the intensity contour of the final syllable vowels in phrase-final position.

Figure 9

Figure 7. Left: intensity difference of the final minus initial syllable phrase-medially. Right: intensity difference of the final minus initial syllable phrase-finally.

Figure 10

Table 4 Summary of statistical analyses of intensity (significance and effect size)

Figure 11

Figure 8. Left: model fit of the f0 contour of the initial syllable vowels in phrase-medial position. Right: model fit of the f0 contour of the initial syllable vowels in phrase-final position.

Figure 12

Figure 9. Left: model fit of the f0 contour of the final syllable vowels in phrase-medial position. Right: model fit of the f0 contour of the final syllable vowels in phrase-final position.

Figure 13

Figure 10. Left: f0 difference of final minus initial syllable phrase-medially. Right: f0 difference of the final syllable minus the initial syllable phrase-finally.

Figure 14

Table 5 Summary of statistical analyses of f0 (significance and effect size)

Figure 15

Figure 11. Left: H1−A2 (top) and H1−A3 (bottom) of initial syllable vowels in phrase-medial position. Right: H1−A2 (top) and H1−A3 (bottom) of initial syllable vowels in phrase-final position.

Figure 16

Figure 12. Left: H1−A2 (top) and H1−A3 (bottom) of final syllable vowels in phrase-medial position. Right: H1−A2 (top) and H1−A3 (bottom) of final syllable vowels in phrase-final position.

Figure 17

Figure 13. Duration ratio (top), intensity contour (middle), and f0 contour (bottom) of words with IG and singleton onsets.

Figure 18

Table 6 Direction of the effects of the presence of IGs and phrasal position on duration ratio, z-scored intensity, z-scored f0, vowel formants (Hz), H1−A2 (dB), and H1−A3 (dB). Significant effects are presented with their estimated effect size

Figure 19

Table A1 Prompts