Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T08:53:17.273Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Nepali

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 November 2009

Rajesh Khatiwada*
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de phonétique et phonologie, UMR 7018, CNRS/Sorbonne-Nouvelle, Parisrajesh.khatiwada@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Nepali, a language of the Indo-Aryan family, is the official language of Nepal. According to the 2001 population census of Nepal, more than 11 million Nepalese (48.61% of the total population) claim this language as their mother tongue. It is also spoken in other countries of South Asia, such as India and Bhutan, as well as by members of the Nepalese Diaspora around the world. The population census of India of 1991 reported that more than two million Indians use Nepali as their mother tongue. Within Nepal, Nepali shows some dialectal variation mainly linked to geographical and socio-cultural factors. Nevertheless, the dialect called the ‘eastern’ dialect spoken by a majority of Nepali speakers shows rather little variation (Bandhu et al. 1971).

Type
Illustrations of the IPA
Copyright
Copyright © International Phonetic Association 2009

Nepali, a language of the Indo-Aryan family, is the official language of Nepal. According to the 2001 population census of Nepal, more than 11 million Nepalese (48.61% of the total population) claim this language as their mother tongue. It is also spoken in other countries of South Asia, such as India and Bhutan, as well as by members of the Nepalese Diaspora around the world. The population census of India of 1991 reported that more than two million Indians use Nepali as their mother tongue. Within Nepal, Nepali shows some dialectal variation mainly linked to geographical and socio-cultural factors. Nevertheless, the dialect called the ‘eastern’ dialect spoken by a majority of Nepali speakers shows rather little variation (Bandhu et al. Reference Bandhu, Dahal, Holzhausen and Hale1971).Footnote 1

Consonants

The above table presents the phonologically contrastive Nepali consonants, as well as two further segments in parentheses: the approximants [j w] are the nonsyllabic variants of /i u/ when preceded or followed by vowels. All the other allophonic variants will be presented in the discussion of the segments.

This study is based on the speech of Nepali speakers whose mother tongue is Nepali. The style of speech illustrated here is considered to be the standard variety spoken mostly by literate people and used in the national mass media. Except when otherwise indicated, our description is based on the speech of a speaker who originates from Biratnagar in eastern Nepal and who has spent most of his childhood and adolescence in eastern Nepal. He was 35 years old at the time of the recording.

The consonant chart above does not include consonants found uniquely in loanwords, including those from Sanskrit. Nepali orthography shows the influence of Sanskrit and makes a few distinctions that are not reflected in usual Nepali pronunciation but are sometimes heard in the pronunciation of educated speakers. A few of these ‘spelling pronunciations’ will be mentioned below. Nepali consonants are illustrated as follows:

Footnote 2 Footnote 3

Obstruents

Retroflex

The Nepali retroflexes are produced in the region posterior to the alveolar ridge. They show a lesser degree of retroflexion than those in other languages of the Indian subcontinent such as Marathi, Gujarati and Tamil (Ladefoged & Bhaskararao Reference Ladefoged and Bhaskararao1983). Due to this lesser degree of retroflexion or its complete absence in some cases, some authors do not use this term (see Pokharel Reference Pokharel1989). A recent palatographic study (Khatiwada Reference Khatiwada2007) shows that the tongue tip is curled back for some speakers after /a/ and /u/; for others, these sounds are apical. Following this former articulatory study, the term retroflex is used in this Illustration. When preceded by a vowel, a voiced retroflex is produced as a retroflex flap [ɽ] (see figure 1 for the two allophones of voiced retroflex /ɖ/). The retroflex stop articulation [ɖ] is maintained in geminates (see an example in the section on gemination).

Figure 1 The above spectrograms and amplitude waveforms show the voiced retroflex /ɖ/ in two different positions in the word. This phoneme is produced as a plosive retroflex [ɖ] at the beginning of the word (left), whereas it is produced as a flap retroflex [ɽ] in intervocalic position (right). We can observe vocal fold vibration over a longer closure duration followed by a visible burst in the voiced retroflex [ɖ], whereas there is no closure duration or burst in the flap [ɽ].

Affricates

Palatograms of the Nepali affricates /ts tsʰ dz dzɦ/ by Pokharel (Reference Pokharel1989) and palatograms and linguograms of several speakers by the present writer (reported in Clements & Khatiwada Reference Clements and Khatiwada2007) have shown that these sounds are produced with laminal contact in the alveolar region. Auditorily, however, they often sound rather like palato-alveolar sounds to English and French speakers.

Aspiration

Nepali has a phonological opposition between aspirated and unaspirated consonants in both voiced and voiceless obstruents. The voiced and voiceless aspirated sounds are produced with greater glottal opening than in the case of the unaspirated sounds. In the present Illustration, the term ‘aspiration’ is used as a cover term for both voiced and voiceless stops (see Dixit Reference Dixit1987 for more information on this subject, and also Masica Reference Masica1991 for the use of aspiration in Indo-Aryan languages). The voiced aspirated sounds are also called breathy voiced (see e.g. Ladefoged & Maddieson Reference Ladefoged and Maddieson1996).

A recent study has shown that voiced aspirated sounds induce both breathy voice and lowered F0 on the following vowel, especially at its beginning, so that a syllable like /dɦa/ could be more narrowly transcribed [dɦ-]. This study also found that for the speakers examined, breathy-voiced vowels occur after voiceless aspirates as well (Clements & Khatiwada Reference Clements and Khatiwada2007).

Often in spontaneous speech, the voiced aspirates lose their aspiration intervocalically and word-finally, where they are realized with looser occlusion. However, it has been noticed that some speakers keep aspiration in dentals, affricates and velars in this context. Both realizations, aspirated and unaspirated, are frequent in Nepali.

Fricativization

As shown in the consonant chart above, Nepali has only two contrastive fricatives, /s/ and /ɦ/, but in spontaneous speech the voiced and voiceless aspirated labials and velars can also be realized as the corresponding homorganic fricatives.

Geminates

Except for the glottal fricative and the approximants, all consonants have geminate counterparts. Geminates occur only medially. The contrast between simple and geminated consonants is shown in the following list. Geminate consonants do not undergo the lenition processes described above.

Initial clusters

Word-initial clusters are allowed under the condition that the second consonant is a rhotic /r/ or a glide [j w]. However, the sequence of a consonant followed by a rhotic is subject to sociocultural conditioning in that educated Nepali speakers may produce clusters, whilst the majority of the population uses an epenthetic vowel.

Sonorants

Nepali has 5 sonorants – /m n ŋ l r/. In this section we will observe some characteristics of the nasals and the rhotic.

Nasals

Though the Nepali orthographic system represents five nasals, <m n ŋ ɳ ɲ>, only the first three are phonologically pertinent. Of these, the velar nasal /ŋ/ occurs only as a word-final except in some onomatopoeic words. A retroflex nasal flap realization [ɽ ˜] of the retroflex nasal <ɳ> is found phonetically before homorganic consonants and in spelling pronunciations of some Sanskrit loanwords.

Rhotics

Nepali /r/ has been described as a tap [ɾ] in intervocalic position and as a trill [r] elsewhere (Pokharel Reference Pokharel1989). In our data, it is realized as a short (two- or three-tap) trill [r] or sometimes as a single tap [ɾ] at the beginning of the word, but as a single tap or flap when intervocalic or at the end of the word. These differences may reflect inter-speaker variation. We take the word-initial trill variant [r] as the basic variant of this phoneme, consistent with our practice elsewhere, but it should be kept in mind that the Nepali trills are weaker than Spanish or Italian trills. Geminate /r/ is fully trilled and, in this case, the trill is quite strong.

A retroflex flap [ɽ] also occurs as a postvocalic variant of /ɖ/ as described above.

Vowels

Nepali has eleven contrastive vowels in its sound inventory. Nasalization is distinctive in Nepali, but any vowel can become slightly nasalized in contact with a nasal.

The quadrilateral presented in figure 2 is based on isolated vowel productions by five male Nepali speakers. They grew up in a wide range of geographical areas but all belong to the same eastern dialect mentioned earlier. Nepali vowels are exemplified as follows:

/a/ is a central vowel in Nepali. The phonetic symbol [ʌ] used here replaces the so-called ‘schwa’ classically represented as [ə] (Bandhu et al. Reference Bandhu, Dahal, Holzhausen and Hale1971, Dahal Reference Dahal1974). In isolation, this sound is slightly rounded, and is acoustically lower and more back than a schwa, as we can see in figure 2. As this sound varies considerably according to consonantal contexts and speaker, it presents a more complex situation for foreign speakers. It seems to vary from [ɜ/ɔ] to [ʌ/ɔ]. In this Illustration, [ʌ] is proposed as the basic norm and all others are considered phonetic variants of this phoneme.

Figure 2 The Nepali vowel chart based on isolated vowel productions of five male speakers.

All Nepali vowels except /o/ have distinctive nasal counterparts (see the examples above). The absence of a phonological /o˜/ (noted by Pokharel Reference Pokharel1989: 34) is quite striking. There is some free variation between [o] and [o˜], as in the case of [dzɦk] vs. [dzɦk] ‘irritate’ and [ɦtso] vs. [ɦtso] ‘short’, but there is no phonologically distinctive contrast. Also there may be nondistinctive nasalization after nasal consonants.

Though the written form of Nepali distinguishes long and short /i/ and /u/, there is no opposition of length in the spoken language. According to Pokharel (Reference Pokharel1989, p.c. 2007), due to intervocalic /ɦ/ deletion, there is an emergence of vowel length in limited data; for example, in some cases of spontaneous speech, /pʌɦaɖi/ ‘of mountain’ can be pronounced as [paːɽi] (< [paaɽi] < [paɦɽi]), creating a clear contrast with /paɖi/ ‘female buffalo calf’.

Almost all vowel sequences, including identical vowels, can be analysed as vowel clusters (Bandhu et al. Reference Bandhu, Dahal, Holzhausen and Hale1971).Footnote 4 It is not very easy to determine in many cases whether a given sequence is monosyllabic or dissyllabic.

Stress and pitch

Stress and pitch are non-distinctive in Nepali.

Transcription of the recorded passage

In this Illustration, a relatively narrow phonetic transcription was adopted, reflecting the pronunciation used in the recording of the passage

Orthographic version

Acknowledgements

I am grateful to Nick Clements, Boyd Michailovsky, Charalampos Karypidis, Takeki Kamiyama and John H. Esling for suggestions and assistance in the preparation of this Illustration, as well as Madhav P. Pokharel and two anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments and useful suggestions. I alone am responsible for any remaining defects and infelicities.

Footnotes

1 ‘As it turned out, these dialects [one spoken in Madi, representing the eastern dialect, and the other spoken in east Tansen, representing the western dialect – RK] are quite close to one another, phonologically speaking, and two distinct analyses at this early stage of analysis were not necessary.’ Bandhu et al. (Reference Bandhu, Dahal, Holzhausen and Hale1971: 1)

2 Pokharel (Reference Pokharel1989: 34) observed that the Nepali /ɦ/ was spectrographically breathy in the speech of his subjects. The speaker used in this illustration regularly produced voiceless [h]. Both types of /ɦ/ – voiceless and voiced – are found in our different recordings.

3 [r] and [ɾ] are two allophones of /r/ (see the subsection on rhotics below).

4 Pokharel (Reference Pokharel1989: 37–38) recognizes ten diphthongs in Nepali. They are shown in the following list:

References

Bandhu, Chudamani, Dahal, Ballabh Mani, Holzhausen, Andreas & Hale, Austin. 1971. Nepali segmental phonology. Kirtipur, Nepal: Summer Institute of Linguistics, Tribhuvan University.Google Scholar
Clements, George N. & Khatiwada, Rajesh. 2007. Phonetic realization of contrastively aspirated affricates in Nepali. 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Saarbrücken, Saarland University, 629–632.Google Scholar
Dahal, Ballabh Mani. 1974. A description of Nepali: Literary and colloquial. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Pune.Google Scholar
Dixit, R. Prakash. 1987. In defence of the phonation adequacy of the traditional term ‘voiced aspirated’. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 67, 49102.Google Scholar
Khatiwada, Rajesh. 2007. Nepalese retroflex stops: A static palatography study of inter- and intra-speaker variability. Interspeech-2007, 1422–1425. Antwerp: ISCA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladefoged, Peter & Bhaskararao, Peri. 1983. Non-quantal aspects of consonant production: A study of retroflex consonants. Journal of Phonetics 11, 291302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladefoged, Peter & Maddieson, Ian. 1996. The sounds of the world's languages. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Masica, Colin P. 1991. The Indo-Aryan languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pokharel, Madhav Prasad. 1989. Experimental analysis of Nepali sound system. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Pune.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 The above spectrograms and amplitude waveforms show the voiced retroflex /ɖ/ in two different positions in the word. This phoneme is produced as a plosive retroflex [ɖ] at the beginning of the word (left), whereas it is produced as a flap retroflex [ɽ] in intervocalic position (right). We can observe vocal fold vibration over a longer closure duration followed by a visible burst in the voiced retroflex [ɖ], whereas there is no closure duration or burst in the flap [ɽ].

Figure 1

Figure 2 The Nepali vowel chart based on isolated vowel productions of five male speakers.

Supplementary material: File

Khatiwada, Rajesh. 2009. Nepali. JIPA 39(3), 373–380.

Sound files

Download Khatiwada, Rajesh. 2009. Nepali. JIPA 39(3), 373–380.(File)
File 5 MB