Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T21:02:58.249Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of cognitive reflection in decision making: Evidence from Pakistani managers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Muhammad Sajid*
Affiliation:
Royal Holloway, University of London and Government College University Faisalabad, Pakistan
Matthew C. Li
Affiliation:
Royal Holloway, University of London
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Assessing how managers discount and evaluate risks is crucial in designing effective managerial policies. In this work, we examine whether risk preferences (RP; both in the domains of gain and loss) and time preferences (TP) are related to managers’ cognitive reflection (CR). To achieve this, the current study focuses on the responses of 601 corporate decision-makers, such as CEO and CFO, of 200 non-financial firms listed at the Pakistan Stock Exchange. Using the three-item of Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT; Frederick, 2005) as a measure of CR, we observe that males perform better on this test than females. Correlation analysis reveals that individuals’ RP in the gain domain are positively associated with their TP, implying that risk-taking individuals are more patient. Our evidence further shows that higher CR is associated with a higher likelihood of increased patience and a lower likelihood of willingness to take risks in the domain of loss. Greater CR is also linked to a higher likelihood of risk-taking in the domain of gain. These findings have important implications regarding the ability of managers to make financial decisions that involve uncertainty and delayed rewards but maximize firm value.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2019] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

Managers make many decisions in their everyday life involving time discounting and a large degree of uncertainty. Empirical evidence from experimental economics, neuroeconomics and cognitive psychology suggests that risk preferences (RP), preferences for risky versus safe outcomes, and time preferences (TP), preferences for immediate versus deferred outcomes, are related to decision making in many critical real-life domains, such as economics, finance, health and wealth (Reference Anderson and MellorAnderson and Mellor (2008); Reference Allen, Weeks and MoffittAllen, Weeks and Moffitt (2005); Barsky et al. (1997); Boyle et al. (2012); Cohn et al. (1975); Reference Guiso and PaiellaGuiso and Paiella (2008); James et al. (2015); Jarmolowicz et al. (2014); Harrison, Lau and Rutström (2007)). For example, people who are risk-averse prefer to invest in safe, low-yield options, such as Treasury bonds, rather than risky, high-yield ones, such as stocks (Reference Cohn, Lewellen, Lease and SchlarbaumCohn et al., 1975). Greater risk aversion is also associated with poor financial and healthcare decision making (Reference Boyle, Yu, Segawa, Wilson, Buchman, Laibson and BennettBoyle, Yu, Buchman, et al., 2012), and dangerous health behavior, such as cigarette smoking, heavy drinking and being overweight (Reference Anderson and MellorAnderson & Mellor, 2008). Likewise, impatience is significantly associated with lower level of income and education (Reference Reimers, Maylor, Stewart and ChaterReimers et al., 2009), poorer school performance (Reference James, Boyle, Yu, Han and BennettJames et al., 2015), being overweight or obese (Reference Chabris, Laibson, Morris, Schuldt and TaubinskyChabris et al., 2008; Reference Jarmolowicz, Cherry, Reed, Bruce, Crespi, Lusk and BruceJarmolowicz et al., 2014; Reference Reimers, Maylor, Stewart and ChaterReimers et al., 2009), smoking (Reference Chabris, Laibson, Morris, Schuldt and TaubinskyChabris et al., 2008; Reference Reimers, Maylor, Stewart and ChaterReimers et al., 2009), alcohol consumption (Reference MacKillop, MirandaJr, Monti, Ray, Murphy, Rohsenow and GwaltneyMacKillop et al., 2010; Reference PetryPetry, 2001; Reference Vuchinich and SimpsonVuchinich & Simpson, 1998), craving (Reference MacKillop, MirandaJr, Monti, Ray, Murphy, Rohsenow and GwaltneyMacKillop et al., 2010), drug addiction (Reference Bickel and MarschBickel & Marsch, 2001; Reference Kirby, Petry and BickelKirby, Petry & Bickel, 1999; Reference Kirby and PetryKirby & Petry, 2004), engaging in unsafe sex (Reference Reimers, Maylor, Stewart and ChaterReimers et al., 2009), less exercise (Reference Chabris, Laibson, Morris, Schuldt and TaubinskyChabris et al., 2008), higher amounts of credit card debt (Reference Meier and SprengerMeier & Sprenger, 2010) and under-utilization of health insurance (Reference Hsu, Lin, McNamara, Houser and McCabeHsu, Lin & McNamara, 2008). Thus, understanding how managers discount and evaluate risks is essential for making optimal financial decisions such as investment and risk-taking.

Several experimental studies have investigated the role of inter-individual differences, with specific reference to cognitive abilities, in individual risk and time preferences for decision-making. Perhaps importantly, these studies suggest that high-ability individuals tend to reveal preferences that differ from their counterparts. More precisely, the literature demonstrates that higher cognitive ability (CA) is significantly associated with more pronounced patience (Reference Białek and SawickiBiałek & Sawicki, 2018; Reference Booth and KaticBooth & Katic, 2013; Reference FrederickFrederick, 2005; Reference James, Boyle, Yu, Han and BennettJames et al., 2015; Reference MelikianMelikian, 1959; Reference Nofsinger and VarmaNofsinger & Varma, 2007; Reference Shamosh and GrayShamosh & Gray, 2008), more risk-seeking in the domain of gain (Reference Byrnes, Miller and SchaferByrnes, Miller & Schafer, 1999; Reference Croson and GneezyCroson & Gneezy, 2009; Reference Donkers, Melenberg and VanSoestDonkers, Melenberg & Van Soest, 2001; Reference Eckel, Grossman, Plott and SmithEckel & Grossman, 2008; Reference Ioannou and SadehIoannou & Sadeh, 2016; Reference Weber, Blais and BetzWeber, Blais & Betz, 2002) and greater risk aversion in the domain of loss (Reference Burks, Carpenter, Goette and RustichiniBurks et al., 2009; Reference FrederickFrederick, 2005; Reference Kirchler, Andersson, Bonn, Johannesson, Sørensen, Stefan and VästfjällKirchler et al., 2017; Reference Nofsinger and VarmaNofsinger & Varma, 2007; Reference NooriNoori, 2016). However, not all researchers have reached the same conclusion; for example, Andersson, Holm, Tyran and Wengström (2016) suggest both a negative and a positive correlation between risk aversion and CA, while Brañas-Garza, Guillen and del Paso (2008) and Reference Booth and KaticBooth and Katic (2013) find no relationship between CA and risk attitudes. Similarly, Kirby, Winston and Santiesteban (2005) document a negative correlation between students’ grades and delay-discount rates, whereas Monterosso et al. (2001) and Noori (2016) do not find any relationship between patient behavior and CA.

Results from recent literature of student subjects and general population suggest that CA seems to be the robust predictor of risk attitudes and intertemporal choices (Reference Basile and ToplakBasile & Toplak, 2015; Białek & Sawicki (2018); Boyle et al., 2011; Cueva et al., 2016; Park, 2016). However, until now, to the authors’ best knowledge, no research has been conducted to test the effects of cognitive reflection (CR) on risk and time preferences among managers.

With this backdrop, the present study utilizes data from a sample of 601 managers of 200 non-financial firms listed at the Pakistan Stock Exchange to examine: 1) the associations of risk attitudes with intertemporal choices; 2) gender differences in the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT); and 3) the correlations between RP (both in the domains of gain and loss), TP and CR. We use Frederick’s CRT to measure CR. RP are assessed using standard behavioral finance questions in which subjects were asked to choose between a certain payoff or a gamble in which they could gain more or gain nothing at all. Similarly, to measure TP, we asked subjects to choose between a smaller-sooner reward versus a larger-later one. These (and other) questions that we asked in our survey were purely hypothetical, and no compensation was offered for the participation; hence, the reality might not be reflected truthfully. This could be seen as a weakness of the data. Fortunately, the literature suggests that, for simple choice tasks, subjects do not need monetary incentives to elicit their preferences (Reference Beattie and LoomesBeattie & Loomes, 1997; Reference Brañas-Garza, Kujal and LenkeiBrañas-Garza, Kujal & Lenkei, 2015; Reference Campos-Vazquez, Medina-Cortina and Velez-GrajalesCampos-Vazquez, Medina-Cortina and Velez-Grajales, 2018; Reference Donkers, Melenberg and VanSoestDonkers et al., 2001; Reference Jullien and SalaniéJullien & Salanié, 2000).

Our results indicate that managers’ risk-taking in the gain domain is positively associated with their patience. In accordance with previous research (e.g., Frederick, 2005; Reference NooriNoori, 2016), we find that males perform better on the CRT than females. Finally, we observe that better CRT performance is positively correlated with risk-seeking in the gain domain, and negatively with risk-seeking in the loss domain and impatient behavior.

The remainder of the paper is organized as follows. Section 2 describes, in relative detail, our method. Section 3 presents our key empirical results and discusses important findings. Finally, Section 4 concludes and draws out some implications and limitations of this research.

2 Method

2.1 Subjects

This study uses survey data. In total, 601 Pakistani corporate financial decision-makers of 200 non-financial firms listed at the Pakistan Stock Exchange participated in the present study. 63 of the subjects are female. The average age of the sample is 37.62 years (STDEV = 10.50; range: 24–72). The sample comprises 21 Board Directors, 17 Chief Executive Officers (CEOs), 7 Vice Presidents, 71 Chief Financial Officers (CFOs), 19 Finance Directors, 47 General Managers (GMs), 16 Financial Controllers, 45 Senior Managers, 11 Chief Accountants, 11 Heads of Accounts, 220 Managers, 22 Accountants, 21 Executives and 75 Officers. The mean value of tenure (i.e., the number of years an individual has been in the current position) is 5.67 (STDEV = 3.55; range: 1.6-12). The sample contains 116 lower- 272 middle- and 213 top-level managers. The average values of past experience (in years) and highest qualification are 6.17 (STDEV = 4.19; range: 0-12) and 1.84 (STDEV = 0.57; range: 1-4), respectively. 350 of the subjects hold a business degree (e.g., MBA). Of the participants, 154, 221 and 120 indicate accounting, finance and both accounting & finance, respectively, as their academic major, and the remaining 106 subjects are those who indicated “other” as their academic major.

2.2 Procedure

Financial managers were surveyed by the investigators at their respective companies between September 1, 2017 and January 23, 2018. The survey was conducted by employing a three-stage approach: e-mail, telephone, and face-to-face. We downloaded the companies’ address bookFootnote 1 from the Pakistan Stock Exchange data portal section. The address book contains all the required information, such as company address, telephone number, e-mail address and name of the company representative, which were needed for reaching the target subjects. E-mails containing the questionnaire and participant’s information sheet were sent to the potential subjects inviting them to take part in the enclosed survey. In each e-mail, we provided our contact details and explained the purpose of conducting the survey. Survey invitations were sent to 372 non-financial companies’ e-mail addresses, which were extracted from the address book. Out of the 372 firms, just three firms filled out the questionnaires, which yielded a response rate of less than 1%. To get the required responses, we then used the telephonic approach and contacted the potential subjects using firms’ contacts, which were extracted from the address book. In addition to obtaining a reasonable response rate, we also used this method to reach the subjects who were geographically dispersed.Footnote 2 Compared to the first technique, this method was relatively successful because approximately twenty percent of the all responses were collected through this approach.Footnote 3 The last approach used to collect the data was a face-to-face mode of administering the questionnaire. The researchers personally visited the target firms that were located in the big cities of Pakistan and distributed the questionnaires to three levels of management. Compared to the previous two modes, this technique was very helpful because around eighty percent of the total responses were collected by this approach.

Potential subjects were informed of their right to withdraw from participating at any time without giving a reason. All of the questionnaires were filled out individually. The instructions for survey filling were given in the questionnaire. It was emphasized that all the items had to be answered. A small number of subjects returned the questionnaires with a few items blank. The researchers asked these subjects to fill out the unanswered questions. Because of this procedure, there are no missing data. No time limit was imposed to complete the survey; on average, a survey lasted 25 minutes. Subjects were not paid.

2.3 Materials

Subjects were asked to fill in a six-section survey questionnaireFootnote 4 including the following tasks: (i) demographic and socio-economic characteristics; (ii) optimism (not reported here) and risk & time preferences; (iii) mindfulness (not reported here); (iv) financial literacy (not reported here); (v) CRT; and (vi) behavioral biases (not reported here). The titles given in the questionnaire for each of the sections were: “Demographics”, “Life Attitudes”, “Day-to-Day Experiences”, “Financial Literacy”, “Cognitive Reflection” and “Behavior and Attitudes”. The tasks (CRT, risk and time preferences, and demographic and socio-economic characteristics) that are used in the current study are described in detail below.

2.3.1 Cognitive Reflection (CR)

In our survey, we measured CR by employing the three items in the CRT (Reference FrederickFrederick, 2005) to gauge an individual’s mode of reasoning and CR. The same test is used by Reference Albaity, Rahman and ShahidulAlbaity, Rahman and Shahidul (2014), Andersson et al. (2016), Białek and Sawicki (2018), Reference Campitelli and LabollitaCampitelli and Labollita (2010), Cueva et al. (2016), Reference Nofsinger and VarmaNofsinger and Varma (2007), Noori (2016), Reference Oechssler, Roider and SchmitzOechssler, Roider and Schmitz (2009), Taylor (2013) (2016) and Reference Thomson and OppenheimerThomson and Oppenheimer (2016) to study the relationships between economic preferences and CR. The following three problems are extracted from Frederick’s (2005, p. 27) paper that constitute our CRT score:

  1. 1. A bat and a ball together cost 110 cents. The bat costs 100 cents more than the ball. How much does the ball cost? (____cents). [Intuitive (incorrect) answer: 10 cents; Correct answer: 5 cents]

  2. 2. If it takes 5 machines 5 minutes to make 5 widgets, how long would it take 100 machines to make 100 widgets? (____min). [Intuitive (incorrect) answer: 100 min; Correct answer: 5 min]

  3. 3. In a lake, there is a patch of lily pads. Every day, the patch doubles in size. If it takes 48 days for the patch to cover the entire lake, how long would it take for the patch to cover half of the lake? (____days). [Intuitive (incorrect) answer: 24 days; Correct answer: 47 days]

Figure 1 presents the distribution of CRT responses of the sample subjects. The figure shows that for the Bat & Ball question, the “impulsive” answer (10) is more frequent than the “reflective” one (5). Whereas, for the Machines and Lily Pads questions, the reflective answers (5 and 47, respectively) are much more frequent than the impulsive ones (100 and 24, respectively). These statistics suggest that the majority of the subjects’ answers are either reflective or impulsive. However, Figure 1 also indicates that some of answers differ from the reflective or impulsive ones.

Figure 1: Answer distribution of the three CRT problems.

Furthermore, in our sample, 24.8% of the managers solved all of the three problems correctly, 22% solved two problems correctly, 18.6% solved one problem correctly, and the remaining 34.6% solved none of the problems correctly. On average, the managers have solved 1.37 (STDEV = 1.19; range: 0–3) of the CRT problems correctly. Cronbach’s alpha for our sample is 0.72, which is higher than that reported in Reference Campitelli and GerransCampitelli and Gerrans (2014), Liberali et al. (2012, Study 2), Reference Morsanyi, Busdraghi and PrimiMorsanyi, Busdraghi and Primi (2014), Primi et al. (2016) and Weller et al. (2013), and lower than that in Liberali et al. (2012, Study 1). Further, averages of intuitive and non-intuitive errors are 1.39 (STDEV = 1.14; range: 0–3) and 0.24 (STDEV = 0.50; range: 0–3), respectively. This finding corroborates Frederick’s (2005) view that the CRT problems prompt intuitive, but incorrect answers.

Moreover, average score of correct responses for males is 1.43 (STDEV = 1.20; range: 0–3) and for females is 0.83 only (STDEV = 0.99; range: 0–3; p < 0.001 for the difference, by Mann-Whitney U test). This gender difference is perfectly in line with past results in the literature (Reference Albaity, Rahman and ShahidulAlbaity et al., 2014; Reference Cueva, Iturbe-Ormaetxe, Mata-Pérez, Ponti, Sartarelli, Yu and ZhukovaCueva et al., 2016; Reference FrederickFrederick, 2005; Reference NooriNoori, 2016; Reference Oechssler, Roider and SchmitzOechssler et al., 2009; Reference Thomson and OppenheimerThomson & Oppenheimer, 2016).

2.3.2 Risk Preferences (RP)

With the aim of assessing the relationship between CR and RP, we included the following two gamble items in the survey. For both of the questions, participants were having the option of choosing either a certain payment of x or a lottery choice with a 75% chance of getting 2x and a 25% chance of getting nothing. The first question of RP is in the domain of gain (x = +10 lakhs Rs.)Footnote 5, while the second item as a measure of RP is in the domain of loss (x = −1m Rs.).Footnote 6

  1. 1 Assume you have the choice between two alternatives. Alternative 1: You receive Rs. 10 lakhs. Alternative 2: You receive a lottery ticket that yields a 75% chance of winning Rs. 20 lakhs. With a 25% probability, it is worthless. Which alternative do you choose? (a) Alternative 1 (b) Alternative 2

  2. 2 Suppose you have to pay Rs. 1m as your debt due. Would you prefer to replace this payment through the following alternative: With a probability of 75% you must pay Rs. 2m. With a 25% probability, you do not have to pay anything. (a) Yes (b) No

2.3.3 Time Preferences (TP)

To test the presumption that people with higher cognitive abilities are more patient, the researchers use the following item for measuring TP, which is a slightly modified version of a question used by Oechssler et al. (2009, as well as Albaity et al., 2014, and Noori, 2016).

Presume that you won Rs. 2m as a prize in a lottery and there are two options, which one do you choose: Take the prize immediately (b) Take the prize after 1 month with 5% premium

The test item gives two hypothetical choices to check whether subjects prefer to take the prize immediately (impatient option) or after a month with five percent increment (patient option).

2.3.4 Other Covariates

Existing research (e.g., Andersson et al., 2016; Reference Boyle, Yu, Buchman, Laibson and BennettBoyle et al., 2011; Reference Burks, Carpenter, Goette and RustichiniBurks et al., 2009; Reference Croson and GneezyCroson & Gneezy, 2009; Reference Donkers, Melenberg and VanSoestDonkers et al., 2001; Reference Eckel, Grossman, Plott and SmithEckel & Grossman, 2008; Reference James, Boyle, Yu, Han and BennettJames et al., 2015; Taylor, 2013, 2016) have suggested that demographic characteristics (e.g., gender, age and education) and contextual factors (such as past experience) also influence individuals’ economic preferences. Accordingly, in the present study, subjects were also asked to answer a set of background questions, which we include as covariates to perform secondary analyses. These covariates are: gender (Male = 0, Female = 1); age in years (range: 24–72); CEO title (CEO = 1, Non-CEO = 0); tenure, i.e., the number of years an individual has been in the current position (range: 1.6–12); level of management (Lower = 1, Middle = 2, Top = 3); past experience in years (range: 0–12); highest qualification (Bachelor = 1, Master = 2, MPhil = 3, PhD = 4); business degree (No = 0, Yes = 1); and academic major (Accounting, Finance or Both Accounting & Finance = 1, “Other” = 0). Age, tenure and past experience are treated as ordered (categorical) variables,Footnote 7 level of management and highest qualification as (discrete) ordinal variables, and the remaining background characteristics are treated as nominal variables.

3 Results

The present study follows, among others, Reference Campitelli and LabollitaCampitelli and Labollita (2010), Reference Shenhav, Rand and GreeneShenhav, Rand and Greene (2017) and Reference Thomson and OppenheimerThomson and Oppenheimer (2016) in using the CRT score as a continuous measure of CR instead of high-low split (Reference FrederickFrederick, 2005; Reference Oechssler, Roider and SchmitzOechssler et al., 2009) because dichotomous measures have been criticized for sacrificing statistical power (Reference Altman and RoystonAltman and Royston, 2006; Fitzsimons, 2008; González-Vallejo & Phillips (2010); Irwin & McClelland, 2001,, 2003; MacCallum, Zhang, Preacher and Rucker, 2002) and creating spurious effects (Reference Altman and RoystonAltman and Royston (2006); Fitzsimons (2008); Reference Maxwell and DelaneyMaxwell and Delaney (1993)).

3.1 Risk and Time Preferences and Cognitive Reflection (CR)

As discussed above, we asked the subjects two risky choice questions. For the first question, which was in the gain domain, 66.1% of the sample (397) chose the option of certain payment of Rs. 10 lakhs, while remaining 33.9% of the respondents (204) preferred the lottery option (that yields a 75% chance of winning Rs. 20 lakhs) [Panel A in Figure 2]. Similarly, for the second item, which was in the loss domain, three hundred eighty-five subjects (64.1%) took the certain option while the remaining two hundred and sixteen subjects (35.9%) picked the risky gamble option (Panel B in Figure 2). Therefore, the sample of the current research could be considered safe (both in the domains of gain and loss).

Figure 2: Frequency (%) of risk and time preferences.

To measure temporal preferences, we confronted the subjects with a single intertemporal choice question in which they were asked to choose between an immediate, smaller payoff and a deferred, larger one. At the aggregate level, 312 managers (51.9%) chose to “take the prize immediately” while the remaining 289 managers (48.1%) preferred to “take the prize after one month with 5% premium” (Panel C in Figure 2). These preliminary results suggest that slightly over half of the sample in the present research is impatient.

To investigate the relationships between CR and decision tasks, we calculate the bivariate correlations among these measures using both Pearson and Spearman correlation coefficients. As seen in Table 1, CRT score is positively correlated with RP in the gain domain, suggesting that good performance in the CRT is positively related to risk-taking in the gain condition. This finding is perfectly in line with the results of previous studies, namely, Reference Benjamin, Brown and ShapiroBenjamin, Brown and Shapiro (2013), Dohmen et al. (2010), Frederick (2005), James et al. (2015), Kirchler et al. (2017), Monterosso et al. (2001), Reference Nofsinger and VarmaNofsinger and Varma (2007), Oechssler et al. (2009) and Park (2016). For the second item of RP, which is in the loss domain, the obtained results indicate that CRT score is negatively correlated with this item, implying that CRT performance is inversely linked to risk-seeking in the loss condition. This finding is similar to the results of Burks et al. (2009), Frederick (2005), Kirchler et al. (2017), Noori (2016) and Oechssler et al. (2009). As expected, CRT score shows a significant negative correlation with (impatient) TP, which suggests that high performance in the CRT is negatively linked to impatient behavior. This result is consistent with several other studies (e.g., Albaity et al., 2014; Reference Basile and ToplakBasile and Toplak, 2015; Dohmen et al. (2010); Frederick, 2005; Hirsh et al., 2010; Melikian, 1959; Shamosh et al., 2008; Reference Slonim, Carlson and BettingerSlonim, Carlson and Bettinger, 2007), suggesting that subjects with higher CR are more patient. To sum up, our results reveal that managers who are high on CR are more (less) likely to take risks in the gain (loss) domain and are less impatient.

Table 1: Intercorrelations among CR and risk and time preferences

N = 601. All reported values are Pearson’s (Spearman’s) correlation coefficients below (above) the diagonal. RP in gains and RP in losses are binary variables, coded as 1 if a subject chooses a risky option and 0 otherwise. TP is also a binary variable, coded as 1 if a subject chooses an impatient option and 0 otherwise. See the Method section for a detailed description of the variables.

*** p < 0.01, two-tailed;

** p < 0.05.

Our results in Table 1 remain unchanged when we use high-low (CR) classification (Reference FrederickFrederick, 2005; Reference Oechssler, Roider and SchmitzOechssler et al., 2009) instead of the CRT score as a continuous measure of CR. The findings in Table 1 are also robust to the application of alternative methods of estimation, such as OLS and (Binary) Logistic regressions, instead of inspections of the correlation coefficients.

Figure 3 reports the frequencies (%) of the two decision-making tasks (RP and TP) when broken down by the four CRT scores. Panel A shows a majority of the sample with zero and one CRT scores selected the sure payment of Rs. 10 lakhs instead of the risky gamble (38% vs. 29% and 20% vs. 15%, respectively), while this pattern is exactly opposite for the sample with two and three CRT scores. For the loss domain, Panel B demonstrates that the majority of the zero and one CRT scorers preferred the risky gamble alternative on paying a sure debt (of Rs. 1 million) (44% vs. 30% and 20% vs. 17%, respectively), while this pattern is again exactly opposite for the managers with two and three CRT scores. Finally, it is apparent from Panel C that a substantial number of the subjects having zero and one CRT scores preferred to take the prize immediately instead of taking it after one month with 5% increase (40% vs. 29% and 21% vs. 16%, respectively), while this pattern is exactly opposite for the subjects having two and three CRT scores. Overall, Figure 3 suggests that the higher proportions of the managers with zero and one CRT scores chose a certain payoff in the gain domain, a risky gamble in the loss domain and an impatient option, while this pattern is precisely opposite for the managers having two and three CRT scores.

Figure 3: Frequency (%) of risk and time preferences.

3.2 Associations between Risk and Time Preferences

Part of the reason for discounting future rewards depends on the risk factor resulted by the delay period. It is possible that preferences towards risk and time are motivated by similar processes (Reference Ioannou and SadehIoannou & Sadeh, 2016). The literature on the relationship between risk and time preferences is scant, and the results are mixed. For instance, Burks et al. (2009) find evidence that subjects’ patience and willingness to take risks are significantly correlated with each other, both in short- and long-run. Reference Menon and PeraliMenon and Perali (2009) also find that impatient subjects are also more risk-averse. Reference Campitelli and LabollitaCampitelli and Labollita (2010) report no significant correlation between intertemporal and risky choices. Recently, James et al. (2015) document that risk aversion is weakly correlated with (both large and small stakes) temporal discounting. In a more recent paper, Ferecatu and Önçüler (2016) note that risk aversion is negatively correlated with impatience. Finally, Ioannou and Sadeh (2016), find that individuals’ intertemporal choices are not correlated with their risk aversion. Therefore, we next assess whether our measures derived from the two decision making characteristics (RP and TP) are correlated. These results are also presented in Table 1.

As can be seen from Table 1, managers’ RP in the gain domain are negatively and weakly correlated with their (impatient) TP. In other words, our result implies that less patient managers are more risk-averse in the gain domain. This is consistent with previous results in the literature (e.g., Burks et al., 2009; Reference James, Boyle, Yu, Han and BennettJames et al., 2015; Reference Menon and PeraliMenon and Perali, 2009). However, we find no significant correlation between risk-taking in the loss domain and impatient TP (p > 0.24) (Reference Campitelli and LabollitaCampitelli & Labollita, 2010), as well as between both risk-taking measures (p > 0.51) (Reference Thomson and OppenheimerThomson & Oppenheimer, 2016).

3.3 Associations between Risk and Time Preferences and Other Covariates

We now turn our attention to the analysis on how demographic characteristics and contextual factors are related to risk and time attitudes, using both Pearson and Spearman correlation coefficients, Table 2 reports the bivariate correlations among these measures. The obtained results show that RP in the gain domain have a significant positive correlation with CEO title, but not with any other covariates. This finding suggests that CEOs are more likely to take risks in the gain domain as compared to non-CEOs. RP in the loss domain show a significant positive correlation with gender (Reference Thomson and OppenheimerThomson & Oppenheimer, 2016), age and tenure, and a significant negative correlation with academic major. In other words, our results indicate that a manager’s gender, age and the number of years s/he has been in the current position are related to her/his risk-seeking behavior in the domain of loss. Further, managers who are specialized in accounting, finance or both accounting and finance are less likely to take risks in the loss domain as compared to those who have “other” academic specialization. Finally, TP are negatively correlated with CEO title, but not with any other covariates. This outcome implies that CEOs are less likely to take an immediately available inferior reward as compared to non-CEOs.

Table 2: Intercorrelations between risk and time preferences and other covariates

N = 601. All reported values are Pearson’s (Spearman’s) correlation coefficients. RP in gains and RP in losses are the binary variables, coded as 1 if a subject chooses a risky option and 0 otherwise. TP is also a binary variable, coded as 1 if a subject chooses an impatient option and 0 otherwise. Gender is a dummy variable that takes the value of 1 if the subject is female and 0 otherwise. Age, Tenure and Past Experience are treated as ordered (categorical) variables; see the Method section for operationalization of these variables. CEO Title is a dummy variable that takes the value of 1 if a subject has the job title CEO and 0 otherwise. Level of Management and Highest Qualification are treated as (discrete) ordinal variables; see the Method section for operationalization of these variables. Business Degree is a dummy variable that takes the value of 1 if a subject holds a business degree (e.g., MBA) and 0 otherwise. Academic Major is a dummy variable that takes the value of 1 if a subject indicated accounting, finance or both accounting & finance as his/her academic major and 0 otherwise. See the Method section for a detailed description of the variables.

*** p<0.01 two-tailed

** p<0.05 two-tailed.

4 Conclusion

Risk aversion and time discounting are the two key determinants of many important real-world outcomes, such as economic, finance and health, but, as yet, there lacks a study on risk aversion and time discounting among managers. Thus, the present research fills the gap and contributes to the subject matter. Through the use of a large sample (N = 601) of Pakistani financial decision-makers, such as CEO and CFO, of 200 non-financial firms listed at the Pakistani Stock Exchange, we test whether risk attitudes and time discounting, are correlated with CR as measured by the CRT (Reference FrederickFrederick, 2005). We find that individuals’ risk tendencies in the gain domain are positively associated with their patient behavior. In keeping with existing literature, we also find that male managers significantly out-perform female managers on the CRT. Our statistical tests further show that CR is positively related to the tendency to pick a lottery option in the domain of gain, and inversely linked to a higher likelihood of choosing a risky gamble in the domain of loss and a preference for a smaller but immediate reward. Notably, we find that CEOs are more likely to seek risks in the gain domain and are less likely to be impatient than non-CEOs. Further, we observe that risk-taking in the domain of loss is positively correlated with (female) gender, age and tenure (i.e., the number of years an individual has been in the current position), and is negatively correlated with academic major in accounting, finance or both accounting and finance.

Findings of this study are relevant for the development of better theories of human decision-making as well as for the formation of managerial policies. For example, our findings suggest that CR is an important predictor of economic preferences. Therefore, for firms, boosting CR can assist key decision-makers to improve their financial decision process. In fact, Reference Sala and GobetSala and Gobet (2017) document that our “cognition is extraordinarily malleable to training.” Thus, firms can provide reflective (and reasoning) training (Reference Willis, Tennstedt, Marsiske, Ball, Elias and KoepkeWillis et al., 2006; Reference Zhang, Fan, Xia, Guo, Jiang and YanZhang et al., 2017) to their managers to boost their CR, which will help them in making better financial decisions (Reference Ball, Berch, Helmers, Jobe, Leveck, Marsiske and GroupBall et al., 2002; Reference Donovan, Güss and NaslundDonovan, Güss and Naslund, 2015; Reference Kulason, Nouchi, Hoshikawa, Noda, Okada and KawashimaKulason et al., 2018). Another implication of the study is for the human resources of organizations. They can hire managers with higher CR because our findings together with other similar studies (e.g., Benjamin et al. (2013); Cueva et al., 2016; Dohmen et al., 2010; Frederick, 2005; Oechssler et al., 2009) show that more reflective individuals are less responsive to short-run TP and RP in the negative domain. Consequently, these high-CR managers can potentially promote firm’s growth and financial success by efficiently reducing the effects of risk aversion and impatience. Finally, evidence relating to CR and risk and time preferences advocates that managers with higher CR might save more, through patient behavior and get higher expected returns, through greater risk-seeking behavior in the gain domain, which possibly leading them to perform a more successful role in financial decision making than those with poor CR.

This work has a number of limitations that may influence the interpretations of the obtained findings. For instance, the data employed in the present study is cross-sectional, which is gathered through questionnaires from a single set of respondents at one specific point in time. A longitudinal study by other researchers could provide useful insights into the relationships between CR and decision-making traits. Besides, the questions used to measure the study variables are one measurement method, while there are other proxies which can be used to measure CA [such as GPA, SAT scores, Raven’s Matrices and WAISFootnote 8 (Reference Burks, Carpenter, Goette and RustichiniBurks et al., 2009; Reference Kirby, Winston and SantiestebanKirby et al., 2005; Reference Monterosso, Ehrman, Napier, O’Brien and ChildressMonterosso et al., 2001; Reference Slonim, Carlson and BettingerSlonim et al., 2007)], RP [like “multiple price list” approach (Reference Miller, Meyer and LanzettaMiller, Meyer and Lanzetta, 1969; Reference Holt and LauryHolt and Laury, 2002) and the “ordered lottery selection” method (Binswanger, 1980,, 1981; Reference BarrBarr, 2003)] and TP [e.g., the “convex time budget” method (Reference Andreoni and SprengerAndreoni & Sprenger, 2012), the “double multiple price list” approach (Reference Andersen, Harrison, Lau and RutströmAndersen et al., 2008), “time-tradeoff” sequences (Reference Attema, Bleichrodt, Rohde and WakkerAttema et al., 2010) and the “risk-free” intertemporal choice task (Reference Laury, McInnes and ToddSwarthoutLaury, McInnes & Todd Swarthout, 2012)]. Therefore, future research using alternative measures of these variables can verify and validate the findings of the present research. Finally, we evaluate RP (both in the domains of gain and loss) and TP through single-item scales. Single-item measures are simple, quick, and useful to target busy participants and easy to administer to large samples (Reference BowlingBowling, 2005; Reference LooLoo, 2002; Reference Waltz, Strickland and LenzWaltz, Strickland & Lenz, 2010); however, methodologists advocate the use of multi-scale instruments (Reference LooLoo, 2002; Reference NunnallyNunnally, 1978) because they are more stable, reliable and precise (Reference BowlingBowling, 2005; Reference LooLoo, 2002). Consequently, we encourage other researchers to examine the relationships between CR and decision-making using multi-item measures of RP and TP, such as the multiple price list (Reference Miller, Meyer and LanzettaMiller et al., 1969) and double multiple price list (Reference Andersen, Harrison, Lau and RutströmAndersen et al., 2008) approaches.

Appendix: Literature ReviewFootnote 9

Risk Preferences (RP) and Cognitive Reflection (CR)

A number of publications are available in the literature that discuss the link between RP and various facets of CA, though findings are mixed and also no one has focused on the managerial population. For instance, Monterosso et al. (2001) find that intelligence quotient (IQ) estimate is positively correlated with performance on the gambling task. Frederick (2005) reports that subjects with higher CRT scores are more willing to gamble in the domain of gains. He further documents that high-CRT subjects are less willing to seek risks for items involving loss and they prefer to accept a sure loss to avoid playing a gamble with a lower or negative expected value. However, Frederick also reveals that low-CRT subjects are more willing to seek risks in the domain of loss. Reference Nofsinger and VarmaNofsinger and Varma (2007) observe that intuitive planners are more risk-averse (in the gain domain, but not in the loss domain) than analytical planners. Oechssler et al. (2009) find that, in the positive domain, individuals in the higher CA group are significantly more willing to take risks. While, in the negative domain, they report that low-ability individuals are significantly more likely to gamble. Burks et al. (2009) conclude that participants with higher cognitive skills are more willing to take risks in the gain domain. However, they report that participants with worse cognitive skills are more willing to take gambles for the items involving loss than those with better cognitive skills. Similar to Frederick (2005), Burks et al. (2009) also find that subjects with greater cognitive skills are more willing to accept a small sure loss to avoid a lottery with a lower or more negative expected value. Dohmen et al. (2010) report that higher CA is associated with a greater willingness to take risks. In the gain domain, Reference Campitelli and LabollitaCampitelli and Labollita (2010) observe positively significant correlations between CRT and risky choice items. Boyle et al. (2011) reveal that a lower level of CA is associated with greater risk aversion among older persons without dementia. Benjamin et al. (2013) reveal that higher cognitive skills are correlated with less small-stakes risk aversion. James et al. (2015) note that more rapid cognitive deterioration predicts higher levels of risk aversion in community-based older adults. Recently, Noori (2016) states that participants with low CRT scores are more (less) likely to reveal risk aversion in the domain of gain (loss). In a related study, Cueva et al. (2016) document that reflective decision-makers are less risk-averse than impulsive ones. Another related work, by Park (2016), concludes that subjects with low cognitive skills being risk-averse when facing the high (low) probability of gain (loss), however being risk-seeking when facing the low (high) probability of gain (loss). Similarly, Kirchler et al. (2017), in the loss domain, indicate that scoring high in the CRT is linked to risk-neutral behavior, whereas in the gain domain, risk aversion is independent of participants’ CRT scores. Rather recently, Dohmen et al. (2018) suggest that CA is related to risk-taking behavior.

However, Brañas-Garza et al. (2008) find no relation between students’ scores on a GRE-like math test and risk attitudes. Reference Booth and KaticBooth and Katic (2013) display no effect of CA on RP. Albaity et al. (2014) also document no relationship between individuals’ CRT scores and RP. Similarly, Thomson and Oppenheimer (2016) observe no relationship between CR and RP (both in the domains of gain and loss). Recently, Ioannou and Sadeh (2016) conclude no relationship between subjects’ risk aversion and their cognitive abilities. In a more recent research, Campos-Vazquez et al. (2018) reveal that CA has no effect on RP.

Besides, Campitelli and Labollita (2010) observe a positive (but insignificant) correlation between CR and risky choices (in the loss domain). Andersson et al. (2016) suggest both a negative and a positive correlation between risk aversion and CA. Taylor (2013) finds that greater CA is related to lower risk aversion when individuals make choices in the hypothetical context, but CA is unrelated to risk aversion in the real-choice context. Likewise, Taylor (2016) reveals that the (inverse) relationship between risk aversion and CA is not robust. He further states that CA is not significantly correlated with RP when choices are real and are characterized by uncertainty, but it is significantly correlated when choices are hypothetical and the safe option is certain.

Collectively, the above-cited studies imply that various measures of CA and RP are connected with each other. Thus, it is expected that, as in most of past studies, subjects with higher CR will take more (less) risks in the positive (negative) domain.

Time Preferences (TP) and Cognitive Reflection (CR)

The relationship between different measures of RP and CA has been tested in several empirical studies, although, again, results are mixed and most of the previous research have targeted the convenient university population. Melikian (1959), for example, reports that subjects with higher “Goodenough” intelligence test scores tend to prefer a larger delayed reward rather than a smaller immediate one. Frederick (2005) finds that greater CR results in favoring the later larger reward. Slonim et al. (2007) find that subjects with higher Scholastic Assessment Test (SAT) scores are significantly more likely to be patient. Reference Nofsinger and VarmaNofsinger and Varma (2007) document that high-CRT financial advisors are more patient than the low-CRT ones. Shamosh et al. (2008) reveal that delay discountingFootnote 10 is negatively linked to general intelligence (g), as well as to working memory. In a meta-analysis, Reference Shamosh and GrayShamosh and Gray (2008) report that higher intelligence is associated with lower delay discounting. Reference Hirsh, Morisano and PetersonHirsh, Morisano and Peterson (2008) observe a significant negative relationship between discounting and CA. Oechssler et al. (2009) report that individuals with lower cognitive abilities are more impatient. Burks et al. (2009) state that subjects with better cognitive skills are more patient (in both short- and long-run). Hirsh et al. (2010) conclude that preferences for immediate gratification are negatively associated with CA. Dohmen et al. (2010) reveal that greater CA is associated with increased patience. Boyle, Yu, Segawa, et al. (2012) indicate that a lower level of CA is associated with greater temporal discounting. Benjamin et al. (2013) report that discounting over short-time horizons is more common among those subjects having a low CA. Albaity et al. (2014) document that subjects’ higher test scores on the CRT are significantly linked to a lower likelihood of being impatient. In a related study, James et al. (2015) observe that cognitive decline significantly predicts temporal discounting among older adults. Also, Basile and Toplak (2015) demonstrate that preference for a larger delayed reward is associated with higher cognitive abilities. Similarly, in a more recent work, Białek and Sawicki (2018) conclude that high-CRT individuals discount less strongly than low-CRT ones.

However, Monterosso et al. (2001) find no relationship between cocaine dependents’ IQ scores and their performance on delay discounting procedure (i.e., choosing between smaller-sooner and later-larger rewards). Noori (2016) also finds no relationship between TP and CRT scores. Likewise, Ioannou and Sadeh (2016) do not find any support of the hypothesis that TP are associated with subjects’ cognitive abilities. In addition, Reference Thomson and OppenheimerThomson and Oppenheimer (2016) report no consistent relationships between measures of CR and TP. In a more recent paper, Campos-Vazquez et al. (2018) find an insignificant relationship between CA and TP.

Besides, Kirby et al. (2005) document a negative correlation between students’ grades and delay-discount rates. Similarly, Campitelli and Labollita (2010) observe a negative (but insignificant) correlation between intertemporal choice and CRT. In a related research, Shenhav et al. (2017) demonstrate that subjects who prefer smaller sooner to later larger monetary payoffs are more likely to give intuitive, but wrong responses, on the CRT.

Collectively, the above-cited research indicate that different proxies of CA and TP are related to each other. Hence, it is expected that, as in much of earlier work, managers with higher cognition level will be more patient.

Table A1: A summary of the studies on risk and time preferences and CA

1 Amazon Mechanical Turk.

2 Online Recruitment Software for Economic Experiments.

Footnotes

We thank the 601 anonymous respondents to our survey, whose participation is truly invaluable. For their helpful comments, we also wish to thank the editor, Professor Jonathan Baron, two anonymous referees, Rizwan Mushtaq and participants at the British Accounting and Finance Association 2019 Annual Conference. Financial support from the Punjab Higher Education Commission, the Government College University Faisalabad, The Charles Wallace Pakistan Trust, and Royal Holloway, University of London, UK is also gratefully acknowledged.

1 Amazon Mechanical Turk.

2 Online Recruitment Software for Economic Experiments.

1 The address book can be found at https://dps.psx.com.pk/.

2 A majority of the target firms were in the largest cities of Pakistan, such as Karachi, Lahore, Islamabad/Rawalpindi, Faisalabad and Multan, but some of the firms (one or two in numbers) were in small cities of Pakistan, like Chakwal, Bannu, Haripur and Mardan. Therefore, due to time and budget constraints, it was not feasible for us to personally visit the small-city firms, and almost all of them were contacted using the e-mail and telephonic approaches.

3 Managers who showed a willingness to participate in the survey shared their e-mail addresses, and subsequently, we e-mailed them the questionnaires.

4 The subjects did all the tasks in English, which is their foreign language.

5 Rs. is the Pakistani rupee, and one lakh is equal to 100 thousand. In early September 2017, the exchange rate was approximately 105 Pakistani rupees/U.S. dollar, therefore 10 lakhs Rs. ≅ $9,500.

6 These two questions are based on Oechssler et al. (2009, p. 151–2). Albaity et al. (2014) and Noori (2016) used similar items.

7 We convert these interval measures to numerical variables by interpolation (Reference Bhandari and DeavesBhandari & Deaves, 2006). That is, we take interval midpoints (e.g., 35 in case of age 30–40); and to depart by 20% for an open-ended interval (e.g., 72 in case of age above 60).

8 WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale.

9 See Table A1 for a summary of the studies on risk and time preferences and cognitive ability.

10 Delay discounting is the tendency to prefer smaller, sooner payoffs to larger, later ones.

References

Albaity, M., Rahman, M., & Shahidul, I. (2014). Cognitive reflection test and behavioral biases in Malaysia. Judgment and Decision Making, 9(2), 149151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, D. G., Weeks, K. P., & Moffitt, K. R. (2005). Turnover intentions and voluntary turnover: The moderating roles of self-monitoring, locus of control, proactive personality, and risk aversion. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(5), 980990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.5.980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altman, D. G., & Royston, P. (2006). The cost of dichotomising continuous variables. BMJ, 332(7549), 1080. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.332.7549.1080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersen, S., Harrison, G. W., Lau, M. I., & Rutström, E. E. (2008). Eliciting risk and time preferences. Econometrica, 76(3), 583-618. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0262.2008.00848.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, L. R., & Mellor, J. M. (2008). Predicting health behaviors with an experimental measure of risk preference. Journal of Health Economics, 27(5), 12601274. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jhealeco.2008.05.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersson, O., Holm, H. J., Tyran, J.-R., & Wengström, E. (2016). Risk aversion relates to cognitive ability: Preferences or noise? Journal of the European Economic Association, 14(5), 11291154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jeea.12179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J., & Sprenger, C. (2012). Estimating time preferences from convex budgets. American Economic Review, 102(7), 33333356. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/aer.102.7.3333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Attema, A. E., Bleichrodt, H., Rohde, K. I. M., & Wakker, P. P. (2010). Time-tradeoff sequences for analyzing discounting and time inconsistency. Management Science, 56(11), 20152030. http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.1100.1219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ball, K., Berch, D. B., Helmers, K. F., Jobe, J. B., Leveck, M. D., Marsiske, M., … Group, f. t. A. S. (2002). Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 288(18), 22712281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.288.18.2271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, A. (2003). Risk pooling, commitment, and information: An experimental test of two fundamental assumptions. Centre for the Study of African Economies, Department of Economics, University of Oxford.Google Scholar
Barsky, R. B., Juster, F. T., Kimball, M. S., & Shapiro, M. D. (1997). Preference parameters and behavioral heterogeneity: An experimental approach in the Health and Retirement Study. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112(2), 537579. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/003355397555280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basile, A. G., & Toplak, M. E. (2015). Four converging measures of temporal discounting and their relationships with intelligence, executive functions, thinking dispositions, and behavioral outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(728). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beattie, J., & Loomes, G. (1997). The impact of incentives upon risky choice experiments. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 14(2), 155-168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/a:1007721327452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamin, D. J., Brown, S. A., & Shapiro, J. M. (2013). Who is ‘behavioral’? Cognitive ability and anomalous preferences. Journal of the European Economic Association, 11(6), 12311255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jeea.12055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhandari, G., & Deaves, R. (2006). The demographics of overconfidence. The Journal of Behavioral Finance, 7(1), 511. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15427579jpfm0701\_2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Białek, M., & Sawicki, P. (2018). Cognitive reflection effects on time discounting. Journal of Individual Differences, 39(2), 99106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickel, W. K., & Marsch, L. A. (2001). Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: delay discounting processes. Addiction, 96(1), 7386. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961736.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binswanger, H. P. (1980). Attitudes toward risk: Experimental measurement in rural India. American Journal of Agricultural Economics, 62(3), 395407. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1240194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binswanger, H. P. (1981). Attitudes toward risk: Theoretical implications of an experiment in rural India. The Economic Journal, 91(364), 867-890. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2232497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, A. L., & Katic, P. (2013). Cognitive skills, gender and risk preferences. Economic Record, 89(284), 1930. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1475-4932.12014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowling, A. (2005). Just one question: If one question works, why ask several? Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 59(5), 342-345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jech.2004.021204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyle, P. A., Yu, L., Buchman, A. S., & Bennett, D. A. (2012). Risk aversion is associated with decision making among community-based older persons. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(205). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, P. A., Yu, L., Buchman, A. S., Laibson, D. I., & Bennett, D. A. (2011). Cognitive function is associated with risk aversion in community-based older persons. BMC Geriatrics, 11(1), 53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2318-11-53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, P. A., Yu, L., Segawa, E., Wilson, R. S., Buchman, A. S., Laibson, D. I., & Bennett, D. A. (2012). Association of cognition with temporal discounting in community based older persons. BMC Geriatrics, 12(1), 48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2318-12-48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brañas-Garza, P., Guillen, P., & delPaso, R. L. (2008). Math skills and risk attitudes. Economics Letters, 99(2), 332336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2007.08.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brañas-Garza, P., Kujal, P., & Lenkei, B. (2015). Cognitive reflection test: Whom, how, when. Working Paper No. 68049. Munich Personal RePEc Archive. Retrieved from https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/68049/.Google Scholar
Burks, S. V., Carpenter, J. P., Goette, L., & Rustichini, A. (2009). Cognitive skills affect economic preferences, strategic behavior, and job attachment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(19), 77457750. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0812360106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological bulletin, 125(3), 367-383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campitelli, G., & Gerrans, P. (2014). Does the cognitive reflection test measure cognitive reflection? A mathematical modeling approach. Memory & Cognition, 42(3), 434447. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0367-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campitelli, G., & Labollita, M. (2010). Correlations of cognitive reflection with judgments and choices. Judgment and Decision Making, 5(3), 182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos-Vazquez, R. M., Medina-Cortina, E. M., & Velez-Grajales, R. (2018). Cognitive ability and economic preferences: Evidence from survey and experimental data in Mexico. Economics Bulletin, 38(3), 1406-1414.Google Scholar
Chabris, C. F., Laibson, D., Morris, C. L., Schuldt, J. P., & Taubinsky, D. (2008). Individual laboratory-measured discount rates predict field behavior. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 37(2), 237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11166-008-9053-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohn, R. A., Lewellen, W. G., Lease, R. C., & Schlarbaum, G. G. (1975). Individual investor risk aversion and investment portfolio composition. The Journal of Finance, 30(2), 605620. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.1975.tb01834.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 448474. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/jel.47.2.448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cueva, C., Iturbe-Ormaetxe, I., Mata-Pérez, E., Ponti, G., Sartarelli, M., Yu, H., & Zhukova, V. (2016). Cognitive (ir)reflection: New experimental evidence. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 64, 81-93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2015.09.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., & Sunde, U. (2010). Are risk aversion and impatience related to cognitive ability? American Economic Review, 100(3), 12381260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/aer.100.3.1238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., & Sunde, U. (2018). On the relationship between cognitive ability and risk preference. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 32(2), 115134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/jep.32.2.115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donkers, B., Melenberg, B., & VanSoest, A. (2001). Estimating Risk Attitudes using Lotteries: A Large Sample Approach. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 22(2), 165195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/a:1011109625844.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donovan, S. J., Güss, C. D., & Naslund, D. (2015). Improving dynamic decision making through training and self-reflection. Judgment and Decision Making, 10(4), 284295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, C. C., & Grossman, P. J. (2008). Men, women and risk aversion: Experimental evidence. In Plott, C. R. & Smith, V. L. (Eds.), Handbook of Experimental Economics Results (Vol. 1, pp. 1061-1073): Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferecatu, A., & Önçüler, A. (2016). Heterogeneous risk and time preferences. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 53(1), 128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11166-016-9243-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitzsimons, G. J. (2008). Editorial: A death to dichotomizing. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(1), 58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/589561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 2542. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/089533005775196732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
González-Vallejo, C., & Phillips, N. (2010). Predicting soccer matches: A reassessment of the benefit of unconscious thinking. Judgment and Decision Making, 5(3), 200206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guiso, L., & Paiella, M. (2008). Risk aversion, wealth, and background risk. Journal of the European Economic Association, 6(6), 1109-1150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/JEEA.2008.6.6.1109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, G. W., Lau, M. I., & Rutström, E. E. (2007). Estimating risk attitudes in Denmark: A field experiment. The Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 109(2), 341368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9442.2007.00496.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirsh, J. B., Guindon, A., Morisano, D., & Peterson, J. B. (2010). Positive mood effects on delay discounting. Emotion, 10(5), 717-721. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsh, J. B., Morisano, D., & Peterson, J. B. (2008). Delay discounting: Interactions between personality and cognitive ability. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(6), 16461650. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.07.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holt, C. A., & Laury, S. K. (2002). Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Review, 92(5), 16441655. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/000282802762024700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, M., Lin, H.-T., & McNamara, P. E. (2008). Neuroeconomics of decision-making in the aging brain: The example of long-term care. In Houser, D. & McCabe, K. (Eds.), Neuroeconomics (Advances in Health Economics and Health Services Research) (Vol. 20, pp. 203-225): Emerald Group Publishing Limited.Google Scholar
Ioannou, C. A., & Sadeh, J. (2016). Time preferences and risk aversion: Tests on domain differences. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 53(1), 2954. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11166-016-9245-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irwin, J. R., & McClelland, G. H. (2001). Misleading heuristics and moderated multiple regression models. Journal of Marketing Research, 38(1), 100109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.38.1.100.18835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irwin, J. R., & McClelland, G. H. (2003). Negative consequences of dichotomizing continuous predictor variables. Journal of Marketing Research, 40(3), 366371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.40.3.366.19237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, B. D., Boyle, P. A., Yu, L., Han, S. D., & Bennett, D. A. (2015). Cognitive decline is associated with risk aversion and temporal discounting in older adults without dementia. PLOS ONE, 10(4), e0121900. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121900.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarmolowicz, D. P., Cherry, J. B. C., Reed, D. D., Bruce, J. M., Crespi, J. M., Lusk, J. L., & Bruce, A. S. (2014). Robust relation between temporal discounting rates and body mass. Appetite, 78, 6367. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.02.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jullien, B., & Salanié, B. (2000). Estimating preferences under risk: The case of racetrack bettors. Journal of Political Economy, 108(3), 503-530. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/262127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirby, K. N., & Petry, N. M. (2004). Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. Addiction, 99(4), 461471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2003.00669.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(1), 7887. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirby, K. N., Winston, G. C., & Santiesteban, M. (2005). Impatience and grades: Delay-discount rates correlate negatively with college GPA. Learning and Individual Differences, 15(3), 213222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2005.01.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirchler, M., Andersson, D., Bonn, C., Johannesson, M., Sørensen, E. Ø., Stefan, M., … Västfjäll, D. (2017). The effect of fast and slow decisions on risk taking. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 54(1), 37-59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11166-017-9252-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kulason, K., Nouchi, R., Hoshikawa, Y., Noda, M., Okada, Y., & Kawashima, R. (2018). The beneficial effects of cognitive training with Simple Calculation and Reading Aloud (SCRA) in the elderly postoperative population: A pilot randomized controlled trial. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 10(68). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00068.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laury, S. K., McInnes, M. M., & ToddSwarthout, J. (2012). Avoiding the curves: Direct elicitation of time preferences. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 44(3), 181217. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11166-012-9144-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberali, J. M., Reyna, V. F., Furlan, S., Stein, L. M., & Pardo, S. T. (2012). Individual differences in numeracy and cognitive reflection, with implications for biases and fallacies in probability judgment. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(4), 361381. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bdm.752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loo, R. (2002). A caveat on using single-item versus multiple-item scales. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 17(1), 6875. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/02683940210415933.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 7(1), 1940. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKillop, J., MirandaJr, R., Monti, P. M., Ray, L. A., Murphy, J. G., Rohsenow, D. J., … Gwaltney, C. J. (2010). Alcohol demand, delayed reward discounting, and craving in relation to drinking and alcohol use disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(1), 106114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (1993). Bivariate median splits and spurious statistical significance. Psychological bulletin, 113(1), 181190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.113.1.181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, S., & Sprenger, C. (2010). Present-biased preferences and credit card borrowing. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 2(1), 193210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/app.2.1.193.Google Scholar
Melikian, L. (1959). Preference for delayed reinforcement: An experimental study among Palestinian Arab refugee children. The Journal of Social Psychology, 50, 8186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1959.9921980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menon, M., & Perali, F. (2009). Eliciting risk and time preferences in field experiments: Are they related to cognitive and non-cognitive outcomes? Are circumstances important? Rivista Internazionale di Scienze Sociali, 117(3), 593630.Google Scholar
Miller, L., Meyer, D. E., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1969). Choice among equal expected value alternatives: Sequential effects of winning probability level on risk preferences. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 79(3), 419423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0026968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monterosso, J., Ehrman, R., Napier, K. L., O’Brien, C. P., & Childress, A. R. (2001). Three decision-making tasks in cocaine-dependent patients: Do they measure the same construct? Addiction, 96(12), 18251837. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.9612182512.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morsanyi, K., Busdraghi, C., & Primi, C. (2014). Mathematical anxiety is linked to reduced cognitive reflection: a potential road from discomfort in the mathematics classroom to susceptibility to biases. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 10(1), 31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-10-31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nofsinger, J. R., & Varma, A. (2007). How analytical is your financial advisor? Financial Services Review, 16(4), 245260.Google Scholar
Noori, M. (2016). Cognitive reflection as a predictor of susceptibility to behavioral anomalies. Judgment and Decision Making, 11(1), 114-120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Oechssler, J., Roider, A., & Schmitz, P. W. (2009). Cognitive abilities and behavioral biases. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 72(1), 147152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2009.04.018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, N. Y. (2016). Domain-specific risk preference and cognitive ability. Economics Letters, 141, 14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2016.01.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petry, N. M. (2001). Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacology, 154(3), 243250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s002130000638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Primi, C., Morsanyi, K., Chiesi, F., Donati, M. A., & Hamilton, J. (2016). The development and testing of a new version of the cognitive reflection test applying item response theory (IRT). Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29(5), 453469. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reimers, S., Maylor, E. A., Stewart, N., & Chater, N. (2009). Associations between a one-shot delay discounting measure and age, income, education and real-world impulsive behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 973978. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.07.026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017). Is it possible to boost your intelligence by training? We reviewed three decades of research. Retrieved from https://theconversation.com/is-it-possible-to-boost-your-intelligence-by-training-we-reviewed-three-decades-of-research-86554.Google Scholar
Shamosh, N. A., DeYoung, C. G., Green, A. E., Reis, D. L., Johnson, M. R., Conway, A. R. A., … Gray, J. R. (2008). Individual differences in delay discounting: Relation to intelligence, working memory, and anterior prefrontal cortex. Psychological Science, 19(9), 904911. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02175.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shamosh, N. A., & Gray, J. R. (2008). Delay discounting and intelligence: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 36(4), 289305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.09.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shenhav, A., Rand, D. G., & Greene, J. D. (2017). The relationship between intertemporal choice and following the path of least resistance across choices, preferences, and beliefs. Judgment and Decision Making, 12(1), 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slonim, R., Carlson, J., & Bettinger, E. (2007). Possession and discounting behavior. Economics Letters, 97(3), 215221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2007.03.018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, M. P. (2013). Bias and brains: Risk aversion and cognitive ability across real and hypothetical settings. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 46(3), 299320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11166-013-9166-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, M. P. (2016). Are high-ability individuals really more tolerant of risk? A test of the relationship between risk aversion and cognitive ability. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 63, 136-147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2016.06.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomson, K. S., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2016). Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test. Judgment and Decision Making, 11(1), 99113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vuchinich, R. E., & Simpson, C. A. (1998). Hyperbolic temporal discounting in social drinkers and problem drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 6(3), 292305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1064-1297.6.3.292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waltz, C. F., Strickland, O. L., & Lenz, E. R. (2010). Measurement in nursing and health research (4th ed.). New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Weber, E. U., Blais, A.-R., & Betz, N. E. (2002). A Domain-specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(4), 263290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bdm.414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weller, J. A., Dieckmann, N. F., Tusler, M., Mertz, C. K., Burns, W. J., & Peters, E. (2013). Development and testing of an abbreviated numeracy scale: A rasch analysis approach. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 26(2), 198212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willis, S. L., Tennstedt, S. L., Marsiske, M., Ball, K., Elias, J., Koepke, K. M., … ACTIVE Study Group, f. t. (2006). Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. JAMA, 296(23), 28052814. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.296.23.2805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, C., Fan, H., Xia, J., Guo, H., Jiang, X., & Yan, Y. (2017). The effects of reflective training on the disposition of critical thinking for nursing students in China: A controlled trial. Asian Nursing Research, 11(3), 194200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anr.2017.07.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1: Answer distribution of the three CRT problems.

Figure 1

Figure 2: Frequency (%) of risk and time preferences.

Figure 2

Table 1: Intercorrelations among CR and risk and time preferences

Figure 3

Figure 3: Frequency (%) of risk and time preferences.

Figure 4

Table 2: Intercorrelations between risk and time preferences and other covariates

Figure 5

Table A1: A summary of the studies on risk and time preferences and CA

Supplementary material: File

Sajid and Li supplementary material

Sajid and Li supplementary material 1
Download Sajid and Li supplementary material(File)
File 35.9 KB
Supplementary material: File

Sajid and Li supplementary material

Invitation to Participate in a Reasearch Project
Download Sajid and Li supplementary material(File)
File 219.3 KB