Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T14:43:44.978Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Time heals all wounds: analysis of changes in temporal focus and implicit space–time mappings among survivors of the 2019 China earthquake over time

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2021

HENG LI*
Affiliation:
College of International Studies, Southwest University

Abstract

Accumulating evidence suggests that people’s sense of the spatial location of events in time is flexible across cultures, contexts, and individuals. Yet few studies have established whether time spatialization is correlated with traumatic experiences. Based on findings that people tend to demonstrate a past time orientation when suffering from disasters, the present research investigated how earthquake experience is associated with temporal focus and time spatialization. Study 1 compared responses of residents in an earthquake-hit area with those of residents in a non-disaster area about two weeks after the disaster had occurred. The results showed that participants in the disaster area were more past-focused and produced more past-in-front responses than participants in the non-disaster area. In Study 2, a follow-up survey was conducted in the same areas ten months after the earthquake to examine whether the impact of disasters on spatial conceptions of time would decay as time elapsed. The findings indicated that participants in these two areas showed no differences in temporal focus and implicit space–time mappings. Taken together, these findings provide support for the Temporal Focus Hypothesis. They also have implications for understanding fluctuation in temporal focus and the high malleability of temporal mappings across individuals.

Type
Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

references

Blix, I. & Brennen, T. (2011). Mental time travel after trauma: the specificity and temporal distribution of autobiographical memories and future-directed thoughts. Memory 19(8), 956967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology 43(1), 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L. (2018). Language and the construction of time through space. Trends in Neurosciences 41(10), 651653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bylund, E., Gygax, P., Samuel, S. & Athanasopoulos, P. (2020). Back to the future? The role of temporal focus for mapping time onto space. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 73(2), 174182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callizo-Romero, C., Tutnjević, S., Pandza, M., Ouellet, M., Kranjec, A., Ilić, S. … & Santiago, J. (2020). Temporal focus and time spatialization across cultures. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 27(6), 12471258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casasanto, D. (2016). Temporal language and temporal thinking may not go hand in hand. In Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (ed.), Conceptualizations of time (pp. 169186). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Casasanto, D. & Jasmin, K. (2012). The hands of time: temporal gestures in English speakers. Cognitive Linguistics 23(4), 643674.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 2763). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de la Fuente, J., Santiago, J., Román, A, Dumitrache, C. & Casasanto, D. (2014). When you think about it, your past is in front of you: how culture shapes spatial conceptions of Time. Psychological Science 25(9), 16821690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dong, P., Huang, X. & Zhong, C. B. (2015). Ray of hope: hopelessness increases preferences for brighter lighting. Social Psychological and Personality Science 6(1), 8491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forest, A. L., Kille, D. R., Wood, J. V. & Stehouwer, L. R. (2015). Turbulent times, rocky relationships: relational consequences of experiencing physical instability. Psychological Science 26(8), 12611271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, R. W. Jr (2020). How metaphors shape the particularities of illness and healing experiences. Transcultural Psychiatry. doi:10.1177/1363461520965424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gu, Y., Zheng, Y. & Swerts, M. (2019). Which is in front of Chinese people, past or future? The effect of language and culture on temporal gestures and spatial conceptions of time. Cognitive Science 43(12), e12804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holman, E. A. & Silver, R. C. (1998). Getting ‘stuck’ in the past: temporal orientation and coping with trauma. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74(5), 11461163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holman, E. A., Silver, R. C., Mogle, J. A. & Scott, S. B. (2016). Adversity, time, and well-being: a longitudinal analysis of time perspective in adulthood. Psychology and Aging 31(6), 640651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, Z. L. (2010). Earthquake blamed for divorce rise. China Daily. Online <http://www.chinadaily.com.cn/china/2010-11/08/content_11513173.htm>..>Google Scholar
Janet, P. (1925). Psychological healing: a historical and clinical study. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Jordan, H. T., Osahan, S., Li, J., Stein, C. R., Friedman, S. M., Brackbill, R. M. … & Farfel, M. R. (2019). Persistent mental and physical health impact of exposure to the September 11, 2001 World Trade Center terrorist attacks. Environmental Health 18(1), 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lai, J., Ma, S., Wang, Y., Cai, Z., Hu, J., Wei, N.Tan, H. (2020). Factors associated with mental health outcomes among health care workers exposed to coronavirus disease 2019. JAMA Network Open 3(3), e203976e203976.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lavi, T. & Solomon, Z. (2005). Palestinian youth of the Intifada: PTSD and future orientation. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 44(11), 11761183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S. W. & Schwarz, N. (2020). Grounded procedures: a proximate mechanism for the psychology of cleansing and other physical actions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 44, doi:10.1017/S0140525X20000308.Google ScholarPubMed
Levasseur, L., Shipp, A. J., Fried, Y., Rousseau, D. M. & Zimbardo, P. G. (2020). New perspectives on time perspective and temporal focus. Journal of Organizational Behavior 41(3), 235243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, H., Bui, V. Q. & Cao, Y. (2018). One country, two cultures: implicit space–time mappings in Southern and Northern Vietnamese. European Journal of Social Psychology 48(4), 560565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, H. & Cao, Y. (2017). Personal attitudes toward time: the relationship between temporal focus, space–time mappings and real-life experiences. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 58(3), 193198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, H. & Cao, Y. (2018). Karma or immortality: Can religion influence space–time mappings? Cognitive Science 42(3), 10411056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, H. & Cao, Y. (2019a). Hands occupied: Chinese farmers use more non-manual pointing than herders. Lingua 222, 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, H. & Cao, Y. (2019b). Losing your footing, losing your morality: the embodied effect of physical slant on moral judgment. Review of Cognitive Linguistics 17(2), 497510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, H. & Cao, Y. (2020a). Best wishes for the future: the link between dispositional optimism and mental sagittal space–time mappings. Personality and Individual Differences 166, doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, H. & Cao, Y. (2020b). Time for politics: the relationship between political attitude and implicit space–time mappings. Current Psychology doi.org/10.1007/s12144-020-00640-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, H. & Cao, Y. (2021). In times of illness: Covid-19 threat influences temporal focus and implicit space–time mappings. Personality and Individual Differences 171, doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, J. Z., Li, S. & Liu, H. (2011). How has the Wenchuan earthquake influenced people’s intertemporal choices? Journal of Applied Social Psychology 41(11), 27392752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, B. P., Schnall, S., Schwarz, N. & Bargh, J. A. (2012). Embodiment in social psychology. Topics in Cognitive Science 4(4), 705716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohamed, M. T. (2018). Is up always good and down always bad? From linguistic to conceptual orientational metaphors in Arabic. Pragmatics & Cognition 25(2), 203275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, F. H., Friedman, M. J. & Watson, P. J. (2002). 60,000 disaster victims speak: Part II. Summary and implications of the disaster mental health research. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes 65(3), 240260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Núñez, R. & Cooperrider, K. (2013). The tangle of space and time in human cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17(5), 220229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfaltz, M. C., Plichta, M. M., Bockisch, C. J., Jellestad, L., Schnyder, U. & Stocker, K. (2021). Processing of an ambiguous time phrase in posttraumatic stress disorder: eye movements suggest a passive, oncoming perception of the future. Psychiatry Research 299, e113845.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rockman, A. (2010). Earthquake boosts divorce rate in southern China: experts. Huffington Post. Online <http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2010/11/09/earthquake-boostsdivorce_n_780462.html>..>Google Scholar
Rodríguez, L. (2019). ‘Time is not a line.’ Temporal gestures in Chol Mayan. Journal of Pragmatics 151, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shipp, A. J. & Aeon, B. (2019). Temporal focus: thinking about the past, present, and future. Current Opinion in Psychology 26, 3743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shipp, A. J., Edwards, J. R. & Lambert, L. S. (2009). Conceptualization and measurement of temporal focus: the subjective experience of the past, present, and future. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 110(1), 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinha, C., Sinha, V. D. S., Zinken, J. & Sampaio, W. (2011). When time is not space: the social and linguistic construction of time intervals and temporal event relations in an Amazonian culture. Language and Cognition 3(1), 137169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stocker, K. (2020). Mental perspectives during temporal experience in posttraumatic stress disorder. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 19(2), 321334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uskul, A. K., Kitayama, S. & Nisbett, R. E. (2008). Ecocultural basis of cognition: farmers and fishermen are more holistic than herders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105(25), 85528556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Kolk, B. A. & van der Hart, O. (1989). Pierre Janet and the breakdown of adaptation in psychological trauma. American Journal of Psychiatry 146(12), 15301540.Google ScholarPubMed
Vardy, T. & Atkinson, Q. D. (2019). Property damage and exposure to other people in distress differentially predict prosocial behavior after a natural disaster. Psychological Science 30(4), 563575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, W., Chen, H., Jiang, X., Ma, L. & Qu, M. (2020). Lessons learnt from analysing the causes of casualties in the Sichuan Changning Ms 6.0 earthquake. Natural Hazards and Earth System Sciences Discussions, 1–13.Google Scholar
Williams, L. E. & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science 322, 606607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, L. E., Huang, J. Y. & Bargh, J. A. (2009). The scaffolded mind: higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of the physical world. European Journal of Social Psychology 39, 12571267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, J., Zhao, G., Li, X., Hong, Y., Fang, X., Barnett, D.Zhang, L. (2009). Positive future orientation as a mediator between traumatic events and mental health among children affected by HIV/AIDS in rural China. AIDS Care 21(12), 15081516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimbardo, P. G. & Boyd, J. N. (2008). The time paradox. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar