Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T12:11:37.636Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Getting the ball rolling: the cross-linguistic conceptualization of caused motion*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 August 2016

GUILLERMO MONTERO-MELIS*
Affiliation:
Centre for Research on Bilingualism, Stockholm University
EMANUEL BYLUND
Affiliation:
Centre for Research on Bilingualism, Stockholm University; Department of Swedish, Linnaeus University; Department of General Linguistics, Stellenbosch University
*
Address for correspondence: Guillermo Montero-Melis, Centre for Research on Bilingualism, Stockholm University, SE-106 91 Stockholm. e-mail: montero-melis@biling.su.se

Abstract

Does the way we talk about events correspond to how we conceptualize them? Three experiments (N = 135) examined how Spanish and Swedish native speakers judge event similarity in the domain of caused motion (‘He rolled the tyre into the barn’). Spanish and Swedish motion descriptions regularly encode path (‘into’), but differ in how systematically they include manner information (‘roll’). We designed a similarity arrangement task which allowed participants to give varying weights to different dimensions when gauging event similarity. The three experiments progressively reduced the likelihood that speakers were using language to solve the task. We found that, as long as the use of language was possible (Experiments 1 and 2), Swedish speakers were more likely than Spanish speakers to base their similarity arrangements on object manner (rolling/sliding). However, when recruitment of language was hindered through verbal interference, cross-linguistic differences disappeared (Experiment 3). A compound analysis of all experiments further showed that (i) cross-linguistic differences were played out against a backdrop of commonly represented event components, and (ii) describing vs. not describing the events did not augment cross-linguistic differences, but instead had similar effects across languages. We interpret these findings as suggesting a dynamic role of language in event conceptualization.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © UK Cognitive Linguistics Association 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We thank Maya Hickmann and Henriette Hendriks for sharing their stimuli, Johannes Bjerva and Jaime Melis for programming help, and Maria Donoso and Arantxa Hurtado for data collection. We are grateful to Florian Jaeger, Guillaume Thierry, and an anonymous reviewer for valuable feedback on a previous version of this paper. Any remaining errors are entirely our own. The work for this paper was supported by the Swedish Research Council (grant no. 2015-01317).

References

references

Athanasopoulos, P., & Bylund, E. (2013). Does grammatical aspect affect motion event cognition? A cross-linguistic comparison of English and Swedish speakers. Cognitive Science, 37(2), 286309.Google Scholar
Athanasopoulos, P., Bylund, E., Montero-Melis, G., Damjanovic, L., Schartner, A., Kibbe, A., … Thierry, G. (2015). Two languages, two minds: flexible cognitive processing driven by language of operation. Psychological Science, 26(4), 518526.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390412.Google Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255278.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory & Cognition, 11(3), 211227.Google Scholar
Bates, D. M., Maechler, D., Bolker, B. M., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4 (Version R package version 1.1-7). Online: <http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4>..>Google Scholar
Bylund, E., & Athanasopoulos, P. (2014). Language and thought in a multilingual context: the case of isiXhosa. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(2), 431441.Google Scholar
Bylund, E., Athanasopoulos, P., & Oostendorp, M. (2013). Motion event cognition and grammatical aspect: evidence from Afrikaans. Linguistics, 51, 929955.Google Scholar
Cadierno, T., & Ruiz, L. (2006). Motion events in Spanish L2 acquisition. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 4(1), 183216.Google Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Lupyan, G. (2015). All concepts are ad hoc concepts. In Margolis, E. & Laurence, S. (Eds.), The conceptual mind: new directions in the study of concepts (pp. 543566). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Choi, S., & Bowerman, M. (1991). Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: the influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Cognition, 41(1/3), 83121.Google Scholar
Flecken, M., Gerwien, J., Carroll, M., & von Stutterheim, C. (2015). Analyzing gaze allocation during language planning: a cross-linguistic study on dynamic events. Language and Cognition, 7(1), 138166.Google Scholar
Flecken, M., von Stutterheim, C., & Carroll, M. (2014). Grammatical aspect influences motion event perception: findings from a cross- linguistic non-verbal recognition task. Language and Cognition, 6(1), 4578.Google Scholar
Gelman, A., & Loken, E. (n.d.). The garden of forking paths: why multiple comparisons can be a problem, even when there is no ‘fishing expedition’ or ‘p-hacking’ and the research hypothesis was posited ahead of time. Online: <http://www.stat.columbia.edu/∼gelman/research/unpublished/>..>Google Scholar
Gelman, A., & Su, Y.-S. (2014). arm: data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models (Version R package version 1.7-07). Online: <http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=arm>..>Google Scholar
Gennari, S. P., Sloman, S. A., Malt, B. C., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). Motion events in language and cognition. Cognition, 83(1), 4979.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L., & Papafragou, A. (2012). New perspectives on language and thought. In Holyoak, K. J. & Morrison, R. G. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 543568). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldstone, R. L. (1994). An efficient method for obtaining similarity data. Behavior Research Methods, 26(4), 381386.Google Scholar
Hendriks, H., Hickmann, M., & Demagny, A.-C. (2008). How adult English learners of French express caused motion: a comparison with English and French natives. Acquisition et Interaction en Langue Étrangère, 27, 1541.Google Scholar
Hickmann, M., & Hendriks, H. (2010). Typological constraints on the acquisition of spatial language in French and English. Cognitive Linguistics, 21(2), 189215.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 434446.Google Scholar
Ji, Y., Hendriks, H., & Hickmann, M. (2011). How children express caused motion events in Chinese and English: universal and language-specific influences. Lingua, 121(12), 17961819.Google Scholar
Kersten, A. W., Meissner, C. A., Lechuga, J., Schwartz, B. L., Albrechtsen, J. S., & Iglesias, A. (2010). English speakers attend more strongly than Spanish speakers to manner of motion when classifying novel objects and events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 638653.Google Scholar
Lai, V. T., Garrido Rodriguez, G., & Narasimhan, B. (2014). Thinking-for-speaking in early and late bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(1), 139152.Google Scholar
Loucks, J., & Pederson, E. (2011). Linguistic and non-linguistic categorization of complex motion events. In Bohnemeyer, J. & Pederson, E. (Eds.), Event representation in language and cognition (Vol. 11, pp. 108133). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G. (2012). Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: the label-feedback hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 54. Online: <http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00054>.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G., & Clark, A. (2015). Words and the World Predictive Coding and the Language-Perception-Cognition Interface. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(4), 279284.Google Scholar
Malt, B. C., Ameel, E., Imai, M., Gennari, S. P., Saji, N., & Majid, A. (2014). Human locomotion in languages: constraints on moving and meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 74, 107123.Google Scholar
Papafragou, A. (2008). Space and the language-cognition interface. In Carruthers, P., Laurence, S., & Stich, S. P. (Eds.), The innate mind. Volume 3: foundations and the future (pp. 272289). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Papafragou, A., Hulbert, J., & Trueswell, J. (2008). Does language guide event perception? Evidence from eye movements. Cognition, 108(1), 155184.Google Scholar
Papafragou, A., Massey, C., & Gleitman, L. (2002). Shake, rattle, ‘n’ roll: the representation of motion in language and cognition. Cognition, 84(2), 189219.Google Scholar
Papafragou, A., & Selimis, S. (2010). Event categorisation and language: a cross-linguistic study of motion. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(2), 224260.Google Scholar
R Development Core Team. (2013). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Online: <http://www.R-project.org/>..>Google Scholar
Ragnarsdóttir, H., & Strömqvist, S. (2004). Time, space, and manner in Swedish and Icelandic: narrative construction in two closely related languages. In Strömqvist, S. & Verhoeven, L. (Eds.), Relating events in narrative. Vol. 2: Typological and contextual perspectives (pp. 113141). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Regier, T., & Kay, P. (2009). Language, thought, and color: Whorf was half right. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(10), 439446.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime reference guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc. Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1996). From ‘thought and language’ to ‘thinking for speaking’. In Gumperz, J. J. & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 7096). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (2003). Language and thought online: cognitive consequences of linguistic relativity. In Gentner, D. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (Eds.), Language in mind: advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 157191). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I., Ibarretxe-Antuñano, I., Kopecka, A., & Majid, A. (2014). Manners of human gait: a crosslinguistic event-naming study. Cognitive Linguistics, 25(4), 701741.Google Scholar
Stringer, D. (2005). Paths in first language acquisition: motion through space in English, French and Japanese. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Durham.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (1985). Lexicalization patterns: semantic structure in lexical forms. In Shopen, T. (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description. Vol. III: Grammatical categories and the lexicon (pp. 57149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (2000a). Toward a cognitive semantics: concept structuring systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (2000b). Toward a cognitive semantics: typology and process in concept structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., & Papafragou, A. (2010). Perceiving and remembering events cross-linguistically: evidence from dual-task paradigms. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(1), 6482.Google Scholar
von Stutterheim, C., Andermann, M., Carroll, M., Flecken, M., & Schmiedtová, B. (2012). How grammaticized concepts shape event conceptualization in language production: insights from linguistic analysis, eye tracking data, and memory performance. Linguistics, 50(4), 833867.Google Scholar
Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality: selected writings, ed. Carroll, J. B.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wolff, P. (2003). Direct causation in the linguistic coding and individuation of causal events. Cognition, 88(1), 148.Google Scholar
Wolff, P., & Holmes, K. J. (2011). Linguistic relativity. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(3), 253265.Google Scholar
Wolff, P., Jeon, Ga-hyun, & Li, Yu (2009). Causers in English, Korean, and Chinese and the individuation of events. Language and Cognition, 1(2), 167196.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: PDF

Montero-Melis and Bylund supplementary material

Montero-Melis and Bylund supplementary material 1

Download Montero-Melis and Bylund supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 517.9 KB