Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T17:55:03.305Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The acquisition of routines in child language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2008

Jean Berko Gleason
Affiliation:
Boston University
Sandra Weintraub
Affiliation:
Boston University

Abstract

The acquisition of routines is one aspect of language development. Routines such as Bye-bye, in contrast to more referential language, appear to be among the earliest acquisitions and are congruent with the sensori-motor child's capacities. This study investigates performance of the highly constrained Hallowe'en Trick or treat routine in 115 children from 2 to 16 years of age. Changes in competence and the role of parental input are examined in relation to cognitive and social factors. (First routines; the Hallowe'en interaction; children's production; adult participation; adult metalanguage; implications for ethnographic research.)

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1976

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Broen, P. A. (1972). The verbal environment of the language learning child. Monograph of the American speech and hearing association 17.Google Scholar
Brown, R. & Hanlon, C. (1970). Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech. In Hayes, J. R. (ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York:Wiley. 1153.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1969). Sociolinguistics. In Berkowitz, L. (ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology 4. New York: Academic Press. 91165.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1966). How to learn to talk: some simple ways. In Smith, F. and Miller, G. A. (eds), The genesis of language. New York: Academic Press. 105–22.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. Berko (1973). Code switching in children's language. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press. 159–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hymes, D. (1973). The scope of sociolinguistics. In Shuy, R. W. (ed.), Sociolinguistics: state and prospect 23. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University Press. 313–33.Google Scholar
Remick, H.L.W. (1969). The maternal environment of linguistic development. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Davis.Google Scholar
Richards, A.I. (1956). Chisingu: a girls' initiation ceremony among the Bemba of Northern Rhodesia. London: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers' speech to children learning language. Child Development, 43. 549–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar