Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T09:14:35.080Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Post-educator relaxation in the U-shaped curve: Evidence from a panel study of Tyneside (ing)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 October 2023

James Grama*
Affiliation:
University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany
Johanna Mechler
Affiliation:
University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany
Lea Bauernfeind
Affiliation:
University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany
Mirjam E. Eiswirth
Affiliation:
University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany
Isabelle Buchstaller
Affiliation:
University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany
*
Corresponding author: James Grama. Email: james.grama@uni-due.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Age-grading—a cornerstone of sociolinguistic theorizing—is hypothesized to follow a U-shaped pattern. Vernacular forms peak in adolescence, abate in middle age, and increase again in retirement, forming a vernacular tail. A complete understanding of age-grading has been hampered by a lack of empirical evidence across the entire adult trajectory and a relatively narrow understanding of speakers’ motivations to change. This paper presents data from a dynamic panel dataset of Tyneside English speakers, covering successive cohorts over the entire adult lifespan. An analysis of (ing) reveals that the U-shaped curve is occupationally niched; only professional educators demonstrate clear retrenchment followed by a tail. Drawing on educational policy research, we argue this effect is largely driven by institutional (and heavily policed) expectations of UK educational policies. We are the first to demonstrate the occupationally niched nature of the U-shaped curve and provide quantitative evidence of the effect of educational policy on linguistic production.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCSA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-ncsa/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is included and the original work is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press.

Age-grading—the pattern whereby individuals change their linguistic behavior throughout their lifetime while the community remains diachronically stable—is one of the theoretical cornerstones of the variationist paradigm. To explain the “regular association of a variant with a particular life-stage” that is characteristic of age-graded changes (Wagner, Reference Wagner2012a:371), sociolinguistic research has traditionally appealed to speakers’ changing relationship to marketplace pressures at various points across the lifespan (Sankoff & Wagner, Reference Sankoff and Wagner2020; Wagner & Sankoff, Reference Wagner and Sankoff2011). For example, the adolescent peak, an ontogenetic mainstay of western industrialized societies (Macaulay, Reference Macaulay1978; Van Hofwegen & Wolfram, Reference Hofwegen and Wolfram2010), is understood as an expression of rebellion against normative expectations “when peer group pressure not to conform to society’s norms is greatest” (Holmes, Reference Holmes1992:184). Much less is known about intraspeaker malleability after this adolescent “extremism” (Chambers, Reference Chambers2008:187). While the “middle years” have attracted little scrutiny (Eckert, Reference Eckert and Coulmas1997:158), sociolinguistic theory postulates retrenchment toward more prescriptively sanctioned forms, resulting in a middle-age trough, particularly for individuals in linguistically sensitive occupations. Following this retrenchment in middle age, older life stages are hypothesized to show a “tail” back toward vernacular forms (e.g., in retirement), as standardizing pressures abate (Buchstaller, Reference Buchstaller2006; Chambers, Reference Chambers2008; Downes, Reference Downes1998). Across the adult lifespan, this fluctuation in prescriptive expectations results in a U-shaped curve in individual realizations of vernacular forms, which repeats generation after generation, resulting in stable variability at the community level.

The best way to map speaker behavior across the lifespan is with panel studies, which follow speakers as they move into progressively older life stages. Panel research has explored the transition from secondary school to university (Wagner, Reference Wagner2012b), university to post-university life (Mechler, Grama, Bauernfeind, Eiswirth, & Buchstaller, Reference Johanna, James, Lea, Eiswirth and Buchstaller2022), and the effect of retirement (Beaman, Reference Beaman2021; Buchstaller, Krause, Auer, & Otte, Reference Buchstaller, Krause, Auer and Otte2017; Grama, Eiswirth, & Buchstaller Reference Grama, Eiswirth, Buchstaller, Skarnitzl and Volín2023). Individually, these studies shed light on specific aspects of post-adolescent linguistic behavior, but the diversity of both the variables studied and methods employed make directly comparing their findings difficult (see Bowie & Yaeger-Dror, Reference Bowie, Yaeger-Dror, Honeybone and Salmons2015; Pichler, Wagner, & Hesson, Reference Pichler, Wagner and Hesson2018). To date, the sociolinguistic enterprise lacks a coherent understanding of how individual linguistic trajectories are articulated throughout the adult lifespan.

The present paper brings a dynamic multicohort dataset to bear on intraspeaker malleability across the post-adolescent lifespan. The data come from twenty-eight speakers across six consecutive age-brackets collected between 1971 and 2022 on Tyneside in the North East of England. Comprehensive metadata allow us to draw on detailed information regarding individual life experiences. Here, we focus on the variable realization of a stable variable, (ing) (see Labov, Reference Labov2001). We address two research questions:

  • Does vernacular behavior follow a U-shaped curve, and, if so, how malleable are speakers across their lifespans with respect to the realization of (ing)?

  • Do we observe linguistic evidence that marketplace pressures are professionally niched?

Our findings indicate that most speakers show high and unchanging vernacular alveolar rates and exhibit linguistic conditioning in line with the existing literature (e.g., Schleef, Meyerhoff, & Clark, Reference Schleef, Meyerhoff and Clark2011). Most strikingly, the U-shaped curve is occupationally niched. Whereas speakers outside of educational spheres demonstrate no retrenchment as they move into older life stages, older educators tend to orient to formally prescribed forms during active teaching practice, exhibiting post-educator relaxation as they move out of in-session teaching. Crucially, the use of standard variants by educators is not monolithic; individuals teaching outside of their local neighborhoods show more pronounced effects than those teaching within their neighborhood. We contextualize these findings using sociolinguistic school research in the United Kingdom, which has drawn attention to the intense normative pressures that are placed on educators during teacher training and their ongoing educational practice (see Baratta, Reference Baratta2017; Snell & Cushing, Reference Snell and Cushing2022). In doing so, we provide the first panel study that explores the impact of language (self) policing on teachers in British educational settings.

Background

Panel research and lifespan change

Until recently, age-graded variability has been hypothesized largely on the basis of apparent time and trend data (e.g., Buchstaller, Reference Buchstaller2006; Downes, Reference Downes1998). Panel datasets—repeated observations of the same speakers as they age—are crucial to capturing the degree to which individuals change across their lifespans. However, the field continues to be hampered by two key shortcomings.

First, most panel studies cover short time frames, making predictions about the course of lifespan change challenging. Others rely on recordings that were conducted many years apart (e.g., forty-two years in Mechler & Buchstaller, Reference Mechler and Buchstaller2019), making it difficult to pinpoint exactly when linguistic changes occurred in a speaker’s life history. Other work relies on case studies of single speakers over several years (e.g., Harrington & Reubold, Reference Harrington, Reubold, Beaman and Buchstaller2021; MacKenzie, Reference MacKenzie2017; Rickford & McNair-Knox, Reference Rickford, McNair-Knox, Biber and Finegan1994), which makes it difficult to generalize results. Comparatively few sizeable studies cover longer stretches of the adult lifespan (see Beaman, Reference Beaman2021; Sankoff & Blondeau, Reference Sankoff and Blondeau2006). What is needed, then, are dynamic panel datasets that follow cohorts of speakers across their lifespans to capture the range of factors that impact on their linguistic choices as they age.

Second, panel research has largely relied on the concept of the linguistic marketplace (Bourdieu & Boltanski, Reference Bourdieu and Boltanski1975) to explain patterning across the lifespan. As individuals age, they are assumed to make their way into spaces where styles that reproduce hegemonic expectations of conformity to “standardness” are explicitly valued (especially in positions where authority is overtly reified, as in line managerial positions, corporate environments, and educational institutions; see Chambers, Reference Chambers2008:189–190). Indeed, retrenchment has primarily been observed in stable variants with low symbolic market value (e.g., vernacular variants of (ing), multiple negation; Downes, Reference Downes1998). After retirement from the professional marketplace, these models postulate that individuals retreat from prescribed norms and revert toward vernacular forms that characterized their speech at an earlier age. Early evidence for this “tail” in older age suggests that this pattern may be the result of the relaxation of prescriptive pressures as individuals leave professional marketplaces (Buchstaller et al., Reference Buchstaller, Krause, Auer and Otte2017). These models have unfortunately offered little insight into the onset and trajectory of when and how such corrective adjustments occur (but see Wagner, Reference Wagner2012b). This is partly due to the disproportionate attention that has been paid to adolescence and emergent adulthood, with relatively little focus on the behavior of middle-aged and older speakers (see Eckert, Reference Eckert and Coulmas1997; Pichler et al., Reference Pichler, Wagner and Hesson2018; Wahl & Kruse, Reference Wahl, Kruse, Willis and Martin2005). What is required in addition to models of marketplace pressures, are social-psychologically and sociodemographically realistic motivations for speakers’ linguistic behavior (see Buchstaller & Beaman, Reference Buchstaller, Beaman, Beaman and Buchstaller2021). Our dynamic panel corpus, covering speakers between nineteen and seventy-eight, is in a unique position to address the impact that professional choices have on speakers’ linguistic behavior across the entire adult lifespan. We focus specifically on the question of how educators react to the pressures of the linguistic marketplace (Bourdieu & Boltanski, Reference Bourdieu and Boltanski1975:20). Our findings provide evidence that educators correct toward the legitimized standard by producing more velar forms while actively employed in school settings. Once these individuals leave these jobs and prescriptive pressures ostensibly abate, they revert to more vernacular choices, a phenomenon we term post-educator relaxation.

(ing)

The variation between the velar nasal and alveolar nasal, as in walki[ŋ] versus walki[n], has long been characterized as a stable sociolinguistic marker. Apparent time research has highlighted its “long-term community stability” in a number of varieties (Wagner, Reference Wagner2012b:183), where it is stratified by age, class, occupation, socioeconomic aspirations, and gender (Hazen, Reference Hazen and Brown2006; Labov, Reference Labov2001; Trudgill, Reference Trudgill1974). Linguistic factors—including priming effects, phonological context, and grammatical category—have a well-documented effect on the realization of (ing) (see Schleef et al., Reference Schleef, Meyerhoff and Clark2011; Tamminga, Reference Tamminga2016).

Our research builds on four panel studies that have mapped changes in (ing) across different life stages. In early adolescence, Van Hofwegen and Wolfram (Reference Hofwegen and Wolfram2010) provided evidence of speakers’ sociolectal adjustments during primary and secondary schooling, demonstrating that pupils made more standard choices upon entering primary school, followed in adolescence by an upswing in vernacularity. Focusing on later adolescence, Wagner (Reference Wagner2012b) explored the linguistic consequences of educational choices at the juncture between high school and university and found that students anticipated the move toward prestigious national universities by producing standard velar variants at elevated rates, while students planning to attend regional schools showed no evidence of such retrenchment. Mechler and Buchstaller (Reference Mechler and Buchstaller2019) investigated middle-aged and older speakers with different socioeconomic orientations. While social risers (all of whom were educators) showed evidence of retrenchment to standard forms, stably working-class speakers only demonstrated small-scale movements away from the vernacular variant. These patterns are supported by Mechler et al. (Reference Johanna, James, Lea, Eiswirth and Buchstaller2022), who noted that retirement seemed to have a more pronounced effect on speakers’ selection of (ing) variants than the progression into early adulthood. Specifically, evidence for the U-shaped curve was restricted to older socially upwardly mobile speakers—again, all educators—who exhibited retrenchment, followed by a post-retirement “tail”; working-class speakers, by contrast, showed a clear preference for the vernacular variant across timepoint.

These panel studies provide a backdrop to lifespan research on a diachronically stable variable. However, because they offer mere snapshots of the lifespan as a whole, they further underscore the need for comprehensive panel datasets that contain data from multiple age groups in the same speech community, collected using the same method, and with similar time-gaps between recordings. Our dataset allows for the modeling of age-graded variables like (ing) across the entire adult lifespan in populations that differ according to both their positionality vis-à-vis superimposed norms, as well as the construction of age-specific social identities.

Sociohistorical context: the Tyneside community

Tyneside, the community considered here, has been historically associated with heavy industry. Coal-mining, shipbuilding, and manufacturing have dominated the economy of the North East (Beal, Burbano-Elizondo, & Llamas, Reference Beal, Burbano-Elizondo and Llamas2012). The industrial decline in the 1970s and 1980s saw seismic shifts in the economic backbone of the wider region, leading to upheavals in the social fabric of Tyneside (see Pike, O’Brien, & Tomaney, Reference Pike, O’Brien and Tomaney2006). Social reforms and investment in new economic sectors (e.g., the medical sciences, sustainable technology) have helped foster an economic upswing in the region. But while the Newcastle-Gateshead conurbation has successfully reinvented itself as the retail, cultural, and service economy center of the North of England (Vall, Reference Vall2007), a significant proportion of the population has been left behind by these recent changes. The North East continues to have some of the highest unemployment rates in the United Kingdom (29.1%; Office for National Statistics, 2022).

Due to a long tradition of sociolinguistic and dialectological research going back to the 1960s, we have a relatively good understanding of the broader socioindexical landscape of language variation on Tyneside (e.g., Allen, Beal, Corrigan, & Moisl, Reference Joan, Corrigan, Moisl, Beal, Corrigan and Moisl2007). Evidence points to changing social indexicalities associated with the local variety. Like other post-industrial vernaculars, the local dialect, Geordie, has historically been rated low on status and high on solidarity, but recent work has reported an increase in overall attractiveness ratings (see Montgomery, Reference Montgomery, Braber and Jansen2018) in line with the perceived attractiveness of the city and commodification of the dialect (Beal & Cooper, Reference Beal, Cooper and Hickey2015).

Data and Methods

The panel data

The present dataset draws on two heritage collections of Anglo speakers on Tyneside. The oldest speakers (aged 60+ in the 2010s, our calibration point) were initially recorded by the Tyneside Linguistic Survey corpus, which collected data in 1971 (Allen et al., Reference Joan, Corrigan, Moisl, Beal, Corrigan and Moisl2007). Six of these individuals were reinterviewed in 2013 as part of a small panel analysis (Buchstaller, Reference Buchstaller2016); three of these speakers were recorded again (yielding a third recording) in 2021. Speakers in the younger age brackets (aged 19-53 in the 2010s) were originally recorded as part of the DECTE project, a large monitor corpus of North East speakers that has been underway since 2006 (Corrigan, Buchstaller, Mearns, & Moisl, Reference Corrigan, Buchstaller, Mearns and Moisl2012). Although we aimed to re-record as many of these individuals as possible, typical challenges that befall panel data collection (see Sankoff, Reference Sankoff, Wagner and Buchstaller2017) restricted our pool to twenty-eight speakers, distributed across six age bands that represent consecutively older life-stages. Table 1 provides an overview over these data. Each cohort is named based on their age bracket at the time of the first recording (T1), except for the 60s+, who are so named because they were in their 60s in the mid-2010s (the T1 recordings of all other cohorts). The other cohorts were re-recorded an average of seven years after the T1 recording, allowing us to investigate their movement into and through stages that “give age meaning” (Eckert, Reference Eckert and Coulmas1997:167). People in their early 20s were captured moving into early adulthood from T1 to T2; a subset of them were recorded a third time after another seven years. Those in their late 20s were captured first in early adulthood, then again as they had moved into middle adulthood. The 30s and 40s, then, represent the period of middle adulthood, with the 50s capturing the move into older age and, for some, (early) retirement. The 60s+ offer a broader snapshot from T1 to T2, as they move from early adulthood to retirement, with T3 capturing speakers settling into post-retirement. Together, this collection represents speakers aged nineteen to seventy-eight in six subsequent age cohorts that form consecutively older life-stages. We call this panel dataset “dynamic” because it does not follow the same group of speakers through all life stages; instead, each cohort provides an independent slice of the lived human experience, the sum total of which can be viewed as a single trajectory. This panel corpus covers a large time-span, which allows us to explore a wide range of factors that contribute to the “emergence of age-relevant linguistic identities” (Bowie & Yaeger-Dror, Reference Bowie, Yaeger-Dror, Honeybone and Salmons2015:608) and to explore intraspeaker malleability across the entire adult lifespan. Οther social characteristics of the panel are discussed in the following section.

Table 1. Dynamic panel dataset across cohort and timepoint; age ranges at time of recording in parentheses

Great care was taken to ensure the comparability of the recordings. To the extent possible, the original speaker dyads were retained for each recording, and interviews were conducted in either the same or a comparable location using the same (or similar) questions and interview topics. In later recordings, interviewees were reminded of topics they had discussed in earlier recordings and asked whether their opinions had changed.

Data processing and coding

The data were orthographically transcribed in ELAN (version 6.5; ELAN, 2023; Lausberg & Sloetjes, Reference Lausberg and Sloetjes2009) and time-aligned using LaBB-CAT (Fromont & Hay, Reference Fromont and Hay2012). All candidate tokens of (ing) were tagged in LaBB-CAT using regular expressions. After extraction, tokens were double-blind coded (by the second and third authors) auditorily as [n], [ŋ], and two forms which included stop releases [ŋk, ŋɡ]. Intercoder reliability for the entire sample was 90%. Mismatches between coders were checked independently by the first author and excluded if all coders disagreed or the token was unclear.

The resulting dataset was coded in LaBB-CAT and R (version 4.0.2; R Core Team, 2021) for a range of linguistic factors known to influence (ing): preceding and following phonological environment, the number of syllables in the word, speech rate, the identity of the preceding (ing) token (Tamminga, Reference Tamminga2016), and grammatical category. Grammatical coding was performed by the third author, following established precedent (Huddlestone & Pullum, Reference Huddleston and Pullum2002; Mechler & Buchstaller, Reference Mechler and Buchstaller2019; Schleef et al., Reference Schleef, Meyerhoff and Clark2011). Initial coding included an eight-way contrast between grammatical categories; however, because categories that fill similar roles showed roughly equivalent alveolar rates (see patterns in Labov, Reference Labov2001:79–88; Schleef et al., Reference Schleef, Meyerhoff and Clark2011:212–219), grammatical category was reduced to a three-way distinction, comprising verbal (84.9% alveolar), nominal (70.2% alveolar), and pronominal (46.0% alveolar) tokens. Of note, [ŋk, ŋɡ] are minority variants that are restricted to pronouns (including those fulfilling a general extender function); these forms (n = 74) are excluded from analysis (see Schleef et al., Reference Schleef, Meyerhoff and Clark2011:215). Finally, tokens in repetitions, read speech, and those that were unclear or not syntactically incorporated were excluded. These processes yielded a total of 7,448 tokens for analysis.

We considered a range of social factors identified as potential axes of variation: sex/gender was self-assigned by the speaker (male/female); qualitative socioeconomic class assignment (working/middle) was assessed on the basis of occupation (following Baranowski & Turton, Reference Baranowski and Turton2018); social mobility (upward/stable) was established via comparisons of class assignments across timepoint (see Mechler et al., Reference Johanna, James, Lea, Eiswirth and Buchstaller2022); and educator status (no/previous/current) was categorized based on speaker occupations in formal schooling, including tertiary education. University students’ sociodemographic classification (n = 9) was assigned on the basis of their parents’ information. Due to the nature of the dataset, many of these factors were highly correlated, and it was unclear which of them would explain patterns in the panel data most effectively. We thus subjected the entire dataset to a random forest analysis using caret (Kuhn, Reference Kuhn2021) and randomForest (Liaw & Wiener, Reference Liaw and Wiener2002) in R to assess the relative importance of each social factor. The social factor with the highest variable importance (following decrease in the Gini coefficient) was educator status.

This weighting informed the prioritization of factors in subsequent logistic mixed-effects models described below. As we argue, educator status is essential to explaining effects in these data; however, it is highly collinear with other factors, in particular, socioeconomic status (since educators are or become middle class by virtue of their profession). With this caveat in mind, we move on to discuss the impact of all relevant factors.

Results

Our data suggest that malleability over the lifespan is mediated by grammatical category, cohort, and occupational orientation. We first turn to linguistic effects and then move to effects that emerge from an investigation of the intersection between individuals and their social characteristics, taking account of cohort- and speaker-specific lifespan orientation.

Broad cohort patterns

Cohort patterns were investigated via a logistic mixed-effects model using lme4 (Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015), fit to a subsample of the dataset that excluded T3 recordings (since only the early 20s and 60s+ cohorts include this timepoint); all instances of velar-plus-stop forms (including in priming contexts) were also excluded (see above). The model tested for the effect of normalized timepoint (operationalized as T1, T2), cohort (early 20s, late 20s, 30s, 40s, 50s, 60s+), educator status (here, collapsed into: yes [at some point], never), ternary grammatical category, preceding and following phonological context,Footnote 1 and the status of the prior (ing) token (alveolar, velar, no preceding token). Initially, the random effects structure included a random slope for speaker by timepoint, but this model failed to converge, and so it was simplified to include speaker and word as random intercepts. Two- and three-way interactions were tested between linguistic and social predictors, with only those interactions kept that improved model fit (by comparing AIC). The final model is reported in Table 2. The threshold for significance for this and subsequent models was set at 0.05.

Table 2. Logistic mixed-effects model fit to (ing) at T1 and T2 (n = 6175, 20.7% velar); we present odds ratios (OR) instead of log odds (OR<1 indicates disfavoring of [ŋ]; OR>1 indicates favoring of [ŋ])

Linguistic effects

The model reveals several global phonological and priming effects. Preceding dorsals favor alveolar realizations, indicating a dissimilation effect (Schleef et al., Reference Schleef, Meyerhoff and Clark2011:218), and following coronals favor the alveolar variant, indicating an assimilation effect. We also observe priming effects; specifically, velar forms are significantly more likely to appear when the preceding token is also velar, and numerically (but not significantly) more likely when the (ing) token is the first produced by the speaker. Phonological context and priming in interaction with timepoint and cohort did not significantly improve model fit; we thus interpret these as global effects in the current dataset.

Grammatical category is a robust predictor of variation in (ing). Both nominal and pronominal categories motivate higher rates of [ŋ], a relationship that persists across timepoint. The interaction effects structure reveals that the degree to which nouns and pronouns motivate more velar realizations varies by cohort, but is consistently higher than verbal contexts. Post hoc comparisons using emmeans (Lenth, Reference Lenth2022) in R with Tukey adjustment confirm that pronominals favor [ŋ] in each cohort, particularly in the late 20s cohort where alveolar rates in nominal and verbal tokens are nearly categorical. These findings suggest that pronouns form a natural class separate from verbs and nouns in their relatively high retention of [ŋ], an effect that is niched by cohort. We revisit this finding below. Due to the distinct behavior of pronominals, subsequent analyses exclude them from the variable context.

Age and timepoint effects

The dynamic nature of our sample allows us to view these six age cohorts as consecutive snapshots across progressively older lifespan stages. Conceived of in this way, we might expect to see evidence of a U-shaped curve across the age-bands; that is, we should observe a general increase in speaker adoption of prescribed forms until (post-)retirement, after which we would observe a reversal toward more vernacular forms. However, we see little evidence in the aggregate of such a pattern. The model in Table 2 and the raw proportions in Figure 1 suggest retrenchment occurs only in the 60s+ age group at T2 (noting that this cohort has the largest time gap between T1 and T2). By contrast, most age bands are stable across timepoint (the early and late 20s, 40s, 50s, and 60s+), with only one group (30s) producing significantly higher vernacular rates at T2. When conceptualized as successive age bands, then, these patterns do not support the generalized curvilinear prediction postulated in sociolinguistic theorizing for the adult lifespan.

Figure 1. Raw proportion of (ing) variants (excluding pronouns) across timepoint by cohort.

Of particular importance is the impact of educator status on (ing). A significant main effect of educator status indicates that individuals whose professional experience includes in-session teaching show an increased likelihood of the standard [ŋ] as compared with those who have never served as educators. Moreover, a significant interaction between educator status and timepoint (T1 versus T2) suggests that this preference for the standard variant increases relative to non-educators over timepoint. Together, these effects indicate that educators make less vernacular choices compared with non-educators, and that these choices become increasingly less vernacular over educators’ working lives. This pattern suggests that these choices are motivated by participation in (or exposure to) linguistic marketplace pressures in the context of the schooling environment. We investigate these findings below by focusing on the behavior of individuals.

Individual trajectories and the effect of (former) educator status

Panel research has demonstrated that the processes underpinning (linguistic) aging are characterized by considerable inter‐ and intragroup variability (e.g., Buchstaller et al., Reference Buchstaller, Krause, Auer and Otte2017; Sundgren, Buchstaller, & Beaman, Reference Buchstaller, Beaman, Beaman and Buchstaller2021) and complex post-adolescent trajectories (Pichler et al., Reference Pichler, Wagner and Hesson2018:3–5). We therefore focus on the individuals that underlie the aggregated data. Not only does this allow us to incorporate the third recording of our bookend panels, we can also undertake bottom-up analyses to locate trends that emerge across speakers along key axes of variability.

Figure 2 plots all twenty-eight speaker trajectories across available timepoints, organized by cohort. In line with previous panel research (e.g., Sankoff & Blondeau, Reference Sankoff and Blondeau2006:560), most participants exhibit no major changes in (ing) across timepoint, with several speakers showing functionally categorical alveolar realizations. The 20s cohort is a particularly striking example of this; all speakers show nearly invariable categorical alveolar use. Among the other cohorts, three trajectories stand out. First, some speakers (Jake, Nathan, Margaret, and Richard) show an increase in the rate of [n]. Second, one speaker, Aidan, shows retrenchment to the prescribed velar form. Finally, a subset of speakers for whom we have three recordings (Amelia, Fred, and Nelly) show evidence of a curvilinear U-shaped pattern. We contend that a single explanatory parameter—active employment as an educator—lies behind these findings.

Figure 2. (ing) proportions for all speakers (excluding pronominals) across timepoint.

As reported in Mechler et al. (Reference Johanna, James, Lea, Eiswirth and Buchstaller2022), the 60s+ cohort exhibits socially niched behavior. The three working-class-oriented speakers—Rob, Anne, and Edith—illustrate stable rates of the alveolar variant across their lifespans.Footnote 2 For the three middle-class-oriented speakers, by contrast, there is evidence of “post-adolescent adjustments under marketplace pressures” (Chambers, Reference Chambers2008:200). Aidan, Fred, and Nelly support the predictions from the age-grading literature for middle-class occupational trajectories, in that they exhibit modulation toward the standard during their working lives. Of particular note, all older middle-class-oriented speakers are educators: Aidan is a college lecturer, Fred a secondary teacher, and Nelly a nursery school nurse. These speakers fit Bourdieu and Boltanski’s (Reference Bourdieu and Boltanski1975:6) characterization of educators as professionals of the language,Footnote 3 and individuals for whom linguistic behavior that adheres to hegemonic expectations is highly prioritized. After this middle-age trough at T2, those 60s+ educators for whom we have a third time point (Fred and Nelly) relax their linguistic choices in retirement, resulting in an uptick of vernacularity.

Fred provides metalinguistic commentary at various points in his interviews suggesting that he is both aware that (ing) is socially marked and that he exhibits variation in (ing). When asked about his linguistic choices in professional and private domains at T1, he remarks that he is not currently using what he calls his “dole voice,” which is the way he spoke in his younger years when he worked at the local job center.Footnote 4 As illustrated in (1), Fred singles out (ing) as a feature that his wife would notice.

We can contrast this with what Fred says at T3, shown in (2), where he reflects on listening to his old recording and comments that he believes he sounded “pretentious” and likely also did as a young teacher. As we discuss in greater detail below, this sentiment is in line with pressures identified in critical school research that educators feel to perform the “legitimised” dialect (Chambers, Reference Chambers2008:195). In particular, this work has identified that teachers uphold the “linguistic conservatism as found within current…educational policy” (Cushing, Reference Cushing2020:426), where English is taught “not only as a set of skills but also as a set of values” (Clark, Reference Clark2001:32). And while (ing) is not the only variable implicated in Fred’s corrective behavior (see Bauernfeind, Ahrens, & Grama, Reference Bauernfeind, Ahrens, Grama, Skarnitzl and Volín2023; Buchstaller & Mearns, Reference Buchstaller, Mearns, Jansen and Braber2018; Buchstaller et al., Reference Buchstaller, Krause, Auer and Otte2017; Grama et al., Reference Grama, Eiswirth, Buchstaller, Skarnitzl and Volín2023), his comments at T3 suggest that he believes his younger self projected a professional, erudite persona, perhaps disingenuously in line with the hegemonic expectations of educator speech.

Indeed, interviews with UK teachers imply they feel pressure to “modify their accents to varieties deemed more ‘professional’” (Baratta, Reference Baratta2017:416). Fred’s behavior at T2, when he shows his highest rates of standard forms (92.4% velar), fully aligns with these prescriptive concerns. This is followed by notable relaxation after retirement in T3 (75.8% velar).

Nelly reflects on similar pressures. In (3), she describes the pervasive standard language ideologies she encountered while working in the “posh” Newcastle suburb of Gosforth. Nelly recounts feeling the need to adapt to her colleagues, who, like her, were expected to model standard language forms for the children. In line with Cushing’s (Reference Cushing2020:440) argument that schools reify conformist behavior as positive, Nelly manifests retrenchment toward the prescribed standard at T2, but, like Fred, shows relaxation from her high velar rates post-retirement in T3. The curvilinear behavior exhibited by these speakers (very high rates of [ŋ] when working as educators, followed by a drop in retirement) mirrors their changing positionalities to normative expectations.

What links these curvilinear patterns among the oldest educators with the middle-aged cohorts? While the majority of speakers remain linguistically stable across their lifespan, Richard, a retired head of school in the 50s cohort, shows a rise in [n] at T2. His withdrawal from prescribed forms after he left his educator role parallels Nelly and Fred’s behavior. A similar reversal after leaving teaching is also clear in Margaret (forty-nine at T1, sixty-one at T2), who took early retirement, and Nathan (thirty at T1, thirty-nine at T2), who left employment at a school to pursue a PhD in music.

We thus see a conspicuous effect: all speakers who exhibit elevated rates of [ŋ] followed by a noticeable drop have left occupations as educators. This is made clear in Figure 3, which plots the proportions of (ing) variants when these speakers were actively involved in teaching compared with when they had left. While (predominantly older and middle-aged) educators differ in their proportions of [ŋ], it is clear that each relaxes in their adherence to the prescribed velar form.

Figure 3. (ing) proportions in subsample of educators before and after leaving the teaching profession.

Proportional differences between active- and post-educator life among these older and middle-aged teachers are reinforced by a logistic mixed-effects regression analysis. Table 3 reports the output of a model fit to (ing) for this subsample of educators. Independent variables include the same linguistic predictors tested in the overall model, with speaker (Nathan, Margaret, Richard, Fred, Nelly) added as a dependent variable. All other operationalized socioeconomic indicators were tested (see above), but none improved model fit over educator status. Word was included as a random intercept. Initially, models were constructed with a random slope of speaker-by-educator status to account for speaker-specific trajectories. However, these models yielded singular fit, suggesting they were overly complex. Speaker was then included as a fixed effect in interaction with educator status. However, this, too, did not significantly improve model fit, suggesting that speakers exhibit roughly the same degree of retrenchment as they move out of their educator positions. The model was thus simplified to include speaker and educator status as fixed effects, outside of interaction. For speaker, we used deviation coding (or sum contrast coding; Brehm & Alday, Reference Brehm and Alday2022) to aid model interpretability, which compares the mean of each level of a factor to the overall group mean. Post hoc comparisons were again performed with emmeans, using Tukey adjustment.

Table 3. Logistic mixed-effects model fit to (ing) to a subsample of educators (n = 913, 71.4% velar; OR<1 indicates disfavoring of [ŋ]; OR>1 indicates favoring of [ŋ])

This model reveals that the subsample of former educators orient to the classic linguistic constraint system of (ing) outlined above. Predictably, we observe high levels of speaker variability. Nelly and Fred are far more likely to exhibit velar forms, followed by Richard, then Nathan, and finally Margaret, who is the most vernacular. Crucially, we again observe a significant effect of educator status, whereby speakers are less likely to produce [ŋ] after they have left their role as an educator.

The model supports our contention that middle-aged and older educators exhibit trajectories that are in line with linguistic marketplace predictions, but this behavior cannot be generalized across the entire panel dataset. Figure 4 plots the percentage of vernacular (ing) for each individual by age at the time of the interview. With a few notable exceptions (discussed below), these educators (in darker font) clearly stand out with depressed alveolar rates. Fred and Nelly again show evidence of the U-shaped curve, while Nathan, Margaret, and Richard show post-educator relaxation. Of note, Aidan exhibits the predicted retrenchment from T1 to T2 as he maintains his professional occupation as a college lecturer; though we have no T3 for him, we hypothesize that he, too, would undergo post-educator relaxation in retirement.

Figure 4. Proportion [n] for each participant across age at time of recording; educators (dark) and noneducators (light).

Contextualizing current educators

In addition to former educators, the panel sample includes speakers who currently hold posts as teachers. These four participants all show trajectories that appear to diverge from our hypothesis that educational settings reinforce adherence to prescribed norms. We discuss these participants in turn.

Sally, a middle-aged English teacher, reflects on the pressures of her linguistically sensitive profession, where she is expected to model “proper” English to her students. At T1, Sally in (4) expresses a stance of resistance toward these expectations, describing her strong dialectal pride and her refusal to engage in verbal hygiene (Cameron, Reference Cameron2012). Her linguistic behavior mirrors this resistance, with vernacular rates over 90%. While Sally’s defiance of prescriptive norms persists as she ages, she reflects on the beginning of her career when she was acutely aware of her “strong accent” (5) and admits she likely changed it to fit expectations. While we lack data on Sally’s speech as a young educator, her assessment matches her linguistic behavior, which has shifted even closer to being categorically vernacular (99%) at T2.

While Sally’s linguistic choices might appear unexpected for a secondary school teacher of English, they are locally unmarked (consider Mechler et al.’s [Reference Johanna, James, Lea, Eiswirth and Buchstaller2022:103] finding that the Tyneside community is almost categorically alveolar). Sally’s school is located in her own neighborhood, and she reports speaking with the same accent with colleagues and students. By engaging in shared linguistic practices, Sally models an authentic, local persona, which has been shown to promote feelings of solidarity and supportive learning environments (see Snell, Reference Snell2018). At the same time, and from a critical school research perspective, Sally’s intransigence can be seen as resisting “the imposition of macro-level linguicism, eschewing prescriptive language ideologies in a local context” (Cushing, Reference Cushing2020:443). Her linguistic behavior is also mirrored in her anti-elitist professional choices. At T2, she comments that she could not imagine being employed at a private school because of her localized accent (on the divide between private and public schools in the United Kingdom, see Green & Kynaston, Reference Green and Kynaston2019). While critical school-based research has shown that many non-fee-paying schools subscribe to policies where language is “disciplined, controlled and monitored” (Cushing, Reference Cushing2021:23), Sally’s school choice can also be viewed as a conscious decision that allows her to embrace her local dialect.

The same accretion of localized pride, professional choices, and accent patterns is observable in Lynn, an early-20s panel member who became a secondary school teacher who exhibits consistently high vernacular rates. Like Sally, Lynn lives and works in the same neighborhood, but she also teaches in the high school she once attended as a pupil. Lynn regularly encounters colleagues and students outside of school and remarks that some of her current colleagues were her teachers. In (6), Lynn frames her linguistic choices in this context. Her sustained high vernacular rates may be motivated in part by her orientation to shared vernacular norms, signaling her membership in the community (see Hall-Lew, Paiva Couceiro, & Fairs, Reference Hall-Lew, Couceiro, Blake and Buchstaller2019). Lynn’s avoidance of standard forms, then, eschews the pretentious (and non-local) connotations these forms carry, in spite of ideologies enshrined in UK educational policies.

Charlotte, a university lecturer, parallels Lynn in her consistent use of the vernacular. One possible explanation for this stability might be that teaching staff at institutions of higher learning are not bound by Ofsted-imposed exigencies.Footnote 5 Another possibility is that Charlotte’s professional background as a social scientist with expertise in ethnographic methods and data collection in the North East, where she also earned her PhD, has afforded her a deep understanding of the social indexicalities of vernacular choices. In her T3 interview, she comments that she “say[s] “walki[ŋ]” when she is teaching, suggesting the production of legitimized language in classroom settings. Though we lack recordings of Charlotte in classroom situations, metalinguistic comments like this implicate style-shifting between work environments and more informal interactions, a hypothesis we explore in future research.

The final teacher, Amelia, illustrates a pattern that we have discussed above for the older educators, namely a steep drop in alveolar realizations between T1 and T2, followed by increased vernacularity at T3. The initial downward trajectory suggests she, too, exhibits retrenchment toward the standard as she moves into the constraints of in-classroom teaching. How then do we explain Amelia’s reversal to vernacular forms in her 30s?

An important cue for Amelia’s behavior can be found in how she positions herself vis-à-vis professional expectations. At T2, Amelia is a substitute teacher and reports feeling insecure in her job, particularly due to pressures to adhere to Ofsted-imposed benchmarks. One such example is shown in (7), where she details her concerns surrounding an impending visit.

Amelia’s worries evoke the climate of “pre-emptive self-surveillance partly driven by fear of…the acute consequences of receiving a ‘satisfactory’ or ‘inadequate’ grading” (Cushing & Snell, Reference Cushing and Snell2023:364) identified in critical school-based research. Viewed in this light, Amelia’s shift from T1 to T2 (a 30% increase in [ŋ]) gels well with the patterns of retrenchment discussed for older speakers and underscores the effect language ideologies can have on language practices (see Snell, Reference Snell2013). Amelia’s later behavior clarifies the context-specific nature of this pressure. In her T3 interview, she shows little evidence of her previous concerns with institutional oversight. Having settled into her educational role, she emphasizes how much she enjoys working with “the little ones,” and her interview features lighthearted reflections of banter with her students. Amelia also reveals that she is studying toward a specialized degree as a speech and language therapist. This newfound confidence is reflected in her linguistic behavior, with vernacular rates returning to those she exhibited at T1.

These pieces of evidence together suggest that Amelia’s retrenchment at T2 reflects situationally specific pressures, which have not contributed to lasting change in her grammar. Amelia’s trajectory, especially in contrast to that of Lynn and Sally, brings to the fore the importance of speaker-specific linguistic choices resulting from the “continuous modification and reconstruction of…linguistic identity over the course of the lifespan” (Dickson & Hall-Lew, Reference Dickson and Hall-Lew2017:249).

Discussion

Reflecting on the course of lifespan change

Stability in post-adolescent adult grammars is traditionally assumed and frequently attested in panel studies, but a growing body of work on lifespan change has provided insights into the modifications some individuals make to their grammars as they age (see Sankoff, Reference Sankoff2019). The present study builds on this work with a unique panel dataset that follows six speaker cohorts as they move into progressively older life stages. This allows us to make a number of contributions. First, we corroborate the widespread finding that most speakers remain relatively stable post-adolescence. Second, for the speakers that do change, malleability can largely be contextualized in terms of professional expectations and individual positioning vis-à-vis prescriptive pressures. Most notably, the hypothesized U-shaped curve in our data is strongly tied to whether a speaker currently holds a job in education. These individuals demonstrate clear retrenchment, followed by an increase in vernacularity after leaving the education sector. While Mechler et al. (Reference Johanna, James, Lea, Eiswirth and Buchstaller2022) contend that this behavior characterized only the older middle-class cohort (all of whom were educators), we show that these patterns apply equally to speakers in middle adulthood. Finally, there is evidence of a cohort effect in our data: educators that do not exhibit retrenchment are all members of younger cohorts (30s and below). As evidenced by comments during the interviews, all educators in our panel sample show heightened sensitivity to the pressures associated with teaching professions in the United Kingdom. We argue these patterns are best encapsulated by the professional pressures of traditional language pedagogies that bear on these speakers, a discussion to which we now turn.

Educators and standardization pressures

The original conceptualization of the “marché linguistique” (‘linguistic marketplace’; Bourdieu & Boltanski, Reference Bourdieu and Boltanski1975:4) assigned educators to a special “marché scolaire” (‘marketplace of the school’; Bourdieu & Boltanski, Reference Bourdieu and Boltanski1975:7), who operate in especially linguistically sensitive spheres as professionals of the language. Research on language policies in UK schools suggests that teachers feel under pressure to perform the legitimized language (Cushing, Reference Cushing2020:428) and that adherence to hegemonic language forms is widely policed, with policies equally visible at the national, school, and teacher level. In particular, Cushing (Reference Cushing2020:438) pointed to the Teachers’ Standards, a set of benchmarks used to evaluate teachers during, for example, teacher education programs, appraisals, and Ofsted inspections. These benchmarks include the promotion of “high standards of literacy, articulacy and the correct use of standard English” (Department for Education, 2013:11). Cushing and Snell (Reference Cushing and Snell2023:363) pointed to another recent example of this institutionalized linguicism in an Ofsted (2019:3) report, which lamented that “[t]oo many staff make errors in their standard spoken English…teach incorrect grammar…[and] need to do more to correct pupil’s poor language or vocabulary.”

While the links between “language, ideology and behaviour are well established within critical educational linguistics” (Cushing, Reference Cushing2020:427), these connections have been mainly supported on the basis of interviews with teachers (see, in particular, Baratta, Reference Baratta2017). The present study is the first to demonstrate the effects these pressures have on individual linguistic production. Older and middle-aged speakers serving active roles as educators produce lower rates of vernacular (ing), despite the overwhelming local use of such forms, and show a clear rise in vernacularity after leaving active duty (largely irrespective of chronological age). Here, the tail of the U-shaped curve is a reflection of speakers exiting a system where English is most stringently monitored and disciplined. The exceptions to this are those younger educators who are embedded in the same local communities they teach in.

These quantitative data are supplemented with qualitative interviews that corroborate teachers’ awareness of their role as upholders of “language subordination” (Lippi-Green Reference Lippi-Green2012:66–77). In environments where hegemonically sanctioned English is endorsed as the only desirable variety, teachers function as both models and arbiters of these practices (Cushing, Reference Cushing2020). The consequences of these pressures are plainly expressed by Sally in (8).

Along with statements from other participants (especially Nelly and Fred above), Sally frames “standard English” as a requisite for her profession, where pressure to adhere to hegemonic expectations is disproportionately high. By juxtaposing “Geordie” with “standard English,” she balances her loyalty to her local dialect with her role as a “language policy [manager]” (Cushing, Reference Cushing2021:23).

However, the use of standard variants by educators is not monolithic. Occupation interacts with the educator’s relationship to the local context since individuals teaching outside their local neighborhoods feel more pressure to use standardized language than those teaching within their neighborhood. The regularity of these overall patterns provides support for Pichler and colleagues’ (Reference Pichler, Wagner and Hesson2018:5) call to move away from narrow conceptions of aging in favor of those that aim to understand “experiences and changes that are relevant or even exclusive to the…[professional] life-course.”

The stability of (ing) on Tyneside

Generally, age-grading is characterized as some degree of individual change over the lifespan in a variable that shows community-level stability diachronically (e.g., Wagner, Reference Wagner2012b:180). We submit, however, that this definition is somewhat open to interpretation. A strict interpretation of age-grading would hold that rates as well as linguistic and social constraints would need to remain stable across the community, although we are not aware of work that has taken such a rigid interpretation. Most studies treat variables as age-graded simply if they retain stable rates of use in real-time (e.g., Macaulay, Reference Macaulay1978; Wagner, Reference Wagner2012b). In our analysis, we identify that (ing) does indeed remain stable across the community over time for a professionally niched subsample of non-educators. This is evident in Figure 4, where non-educators (except for Jake and Andrew) produce high and stable rates of alveolar realizations. In terms of its linguistic constraints, (ing) may show some evidence of change (see Table 3), whereby younger cohorts show greater differentiation between pronominal and non-pronominal (ing) than older cohorts. However, this may be due to subtle shifts in weightings across cohort rather than a diachronic change in grammatical constraints. The sole factor that seems to have changed over time is the value ascribed to (ing)’s variants in educational settings. This is especially obvious when comparing teachers in older cohorts (like Nelly and Fred) as opposed to younger educators who teach in their local communities (like Sally and Lynn) and who are largely stable over time. Future re-recordings will tell whether the youngest panel members, too, succumb to the pressures to “modify their accents to varieties deemed more ‘professional’” (Baratta, Reference Baratta2017:416).

Conclusion

This paper brings dynamic panel data to bear on the linguistic and social mechanisms underlying age-grading, allowing us to map speaker trajectories across the entire adult lifespan. Individual trajectories are socially and linguistically niched (MacKenzie, Reference MacKenzie2017; Mechler & Buchstaller, Reference Mechler and Buchstaller2019). Choices in (ing) are linguistically conditioned as predicted by grammatical and phonological principles (e.g., Schleef et al., Reference Schleef, Meyerhoff and Clark2011).

Most importantly, our results suggest that retrenchment toward the standard and the subsequent “tail” is a function of the specific pressures exerted by educational occupations. Against a community backdrop of principally alveolar forms, in-session teachers show movement toward the velar variant, a type of retrenchment, we argue, that is a reaction of educator orientation to formally prescribed norms, and, as such, part of the “pervasive…practice [of] linguistic conservatism and linguicism of current UK curriculum policy” (Cushing, Reference Cushing2020:443). After leaving the teaching profession, whether by occupational change or retirement, we observe post-educator relaxation—a clear increase in vernacularity consistent with the hypothesized “tail” in the U-shaped curve. Until recently, accounts of speaker’s self-correction have been based on introspective commentary (Baratta, Reference Baratta2017; Cushing, Reference Cushing2020), or inferred on the basis of panel data from younger age brackets (Wagner, Reference Wagner2012b). Our data produces the first panel-based evidence that educators systematically correct toward the standard during their years of active service, in part, due to the hegemonic pressures enacted within educational institutions.

Acknowledgments

The analysis was funded by the DFG (#BU 2902/7-1) and the ERC (#321760). We are grateful to our fieldworkers, Adam Mearns and Alice Richardson, as well as to the students at the Sociolinguistics Lab at the University of Duisburg-Essen who helped with transcriptions, alignment, and coding: Paul Enninger, Marie Phillips, Hannah Sawall, Carina Ahrens, Deborah Veiter, and Anabel Büscher. Swetlana Herbrandt (TU Dortmund) provided invaluable statistical guidance. We also thank our panel speakers for generously sharing their speech with us not once, but twice and three times. Our analysis has benefitted from audience feedback at UKLVC13, ICLaVE11, and NWAV50. All remaining errors are our own.

Competing interests

The authors declare none.

Footnotes

1. Due to uneven token numbers, preceding phonological context was expressed binarily (dorsal tokens versus all other contexts); following phonological context was a three-level factor: coronal, non-coronal (comprising dorsal and labial tokens), and vocalic/glottal/final (which included tokens followed by a vowel, /h/, or in utterance-final position).

2. Mechler et al. (Reference Johanna, James, Lea, Eiswirth and Buchstaller2022) reported that these speakers become slightly more vernacular, a finding that holds only if pronominals are included.

3. “Qui doivent leur position à la maîtrise des instruments de l’expression” (‘those who owe their position to mastering the use of the instruments of expression’; our translation).

4. In UK social services, the “dole office” refers to the job center. Sundgren, Buchstaller, and Beaman (Reference Buchstaller, Beaman, Beaman and Buchstaller2021) discussed how young, especially male, public-sector employees rely on vernacular forms to index localized expertise. This would seem to apply to Fred before he was recorded at T1 and is equally relevant for Jake (early 20s), who, between T1 and T2, rose through the Newcastle city administration, from dole office clerk to lower management of the local authority.

5. Since 1992, the Office for Standards in Education, Children’s Services, and Skills (Ofsted) has monitored educational agencies and teacher training, which includes “regular inspections…which comment on various aspects of educational provision, including teachers’ and students’ spoken language” (Cushing & Snell, Reference Cushing and Snell2023:363). Although Ofsted is not involved in tertiary-level educational contexts, here, too, reports of accent discrimination can be found (see Paterson, Reference Paterson2019).

References

Allen, Will, Beal, Joan, C., Corrigan, Karen P., & Moisl, Hermann. (2007). A linguistic “time-capsule”: The Newcastle electronic corpus of Tyneside English. In Beal, J.C., Corrigan, K.P., & Moisl, H.L. (eds.), Creating and digitizing language corpora. London: Palgrave Macmillan. 1648.Google Scholar
Baranowski, Maciej, & Turton, Danielle. (2018). Locating speakers in the socioeconomic hierarchy: Toward the optimal indicators of social class. Paper presented at New Ways of Analyzing Variation (NWAV) 47. New York, October 18-21 .Google Scholar
Baratta, Alex. (2017). Accent and linguistic prejudice within British teacher training. Journal of Language, Identity & Education 16(6):416423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, Douglass, Mächler, Martin, Bolker, Ben, & Walker, Steve. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1):148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauernfeind, Lea, Ahrens, Carina, & Grama, James. (2023). Change across the lifespan in goat: Evidence from a panel study of Tyneside English. In Skarnitzl, R. & Volín, J. (eds.), Proceedings of the 20th International Congress of Phonetic Science. Guarant International. 2064–2068.Google Scholar
Beal, Joan, Burbano-Elizondo, Lourdes, & Llamas, Carmen. (2012). Urban north-eastern English: Tyneside to Teeside. Edinburgh: Edingburgh University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beal, Joan, & Cooper, Paul. (2015). The enregisterment of Northern English. In Hickey, R. (ed.), Researching Northern English. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 2750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaman, Karen V. (2021). Coherence in real- and apparent-time: A sociolinguistic variationist investigation of language change in Swabia. Doctoral dissertation, Queen Mary University College.Google Scholar
Bourdieu, Pierre, & Boltanski, Luc. (1975). Le fétichisme de la langue. Actes de la recherche en sciences sociales 4:232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowie, David, & Yaeger-Dror, Malcah. (2015). Phonological change in real time. In Honeybone, P. & Salmons, J. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of historical phonology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. .Google Scholar
Brehm, Laurel, & Alday, Phillip. (2022). Contrast coding choices in a decade of mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language 125:113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchstaller, Isabelle, & Beaman, Karen V. (2021). Introduction. Panel research: Theoretical and methodological implications. In Beaman, K.V. & Buchstaller, I. (eds.), Language variation and language change across the lifespan: Theoretical and empirical perspectives from panel studies. New York: Routledge. 1755.Google Scholar
Buchstaller, Isabelle, & Mearns, Adam. (2018). Sociolinguistic variation on Tyneside. In Jansen, S. & Braber, N. (eds.), Sociolinguistics in England. London: Palgrave Macmillan. 215-241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchstaller, Isabelle, Krause, Anne, Auer, Anja, & Otte, Stefanie. (2017). Levelling across the life-span? Journal of Sociolinguistics 21:333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchstaller, Isabelle. (2016). Investigating the effect of socio-cognitive salience and speaker-based factors in morpho-syntactic life-span change. Journal of English Linguistics 44(3):199229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchstaller, Isabelle. (2006). Diagnostics of age-graded linguistic behaviour. Journal of Sociolinguistics 10:330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, Deborah. (2012). Verbal hygiene (1st ed.). London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, Jack. (2008). Sociolinguistic theory: Linguistic variation and its social significance (3rd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Clark, Urszula. (2001). War words: Language, history and the disciplining of English. New York: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corrigan, Karen P., Buchstaller, Isabelle, Mearns, Adam, & Moisl, Hermann. (2012) The diachronic electronic corpus of Tyneside English. Newcastle University. https://research.ncl.ac.uk/decte.Google Scholar
Cushing, Ian. (2020). The policy and policing of language in schools. Language in Society 49(3):425450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushing, Ian. (2021). Language, discipline and ‘teaching like a champion.’ British Educational Research Journal 47:2341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushing, Ian, & Snell, Julia. (2023). The (white) ears of Ofsted: a raciolinguistic perspective on the listening practices of the schools inspectorate. Language in Society, 52(3):363386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Department for Education. (2013). Teachers’ standards. London: Department for Education.Google Scholar
Dickson, Victoria, & Hall-Lew, Lauren. (2017). Class, gender and rhoticity: The social stratification of non-prevocalic /r/ in Edinburgh speech. Journal of English Linguistics 45(3):229259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Downes, William. (1998). Language and society (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. (1997). Age as a sociolinguistic variable. In Coulmas, F. (ed.), The handbook of sociolinguistics. Oxford: Blackwell. 151167.Google Scholar
ELAN (Version 6.5) [Computer software]. (2023). Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, The Language Archive. Retrieved from https://archive.mpi.nl/tla/elan.Google Scholar
Fromont, Robert, & Hay, Jennifer. (2012). LaBB-CAT: An annotation store. Proceedings of the Australasian Language Technology Workshop 113117.Google Scholar
Grama, James, Eiswirth, Mirjam E., & Buchstaller, Isabelle. (2023). Tracking creak from early to late adulthood: A panel study from the North East of England. In Skarnitzl, R. & Volín, J. (eds.) Proceedings of the 20th International Congress of Phonetic Science. Guarant International. 2049–2053.Google Scholar
Green, Francis, & Kynaston, David. (2019). Engines of privilege: Britain’s private school problem. Bloomsbury: Bloomsbury Publishing.Google Scholar
Hall-Lew, Lauren, Couceiro, Paiva, Inês, & Fairs, Amie. (2019). Credibility without intelligibility. In Blake, R. & Buchstaller, I. (eds.), The Routledge companion to the work of John R. Rickford. New York: Routledge. 221231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, Jonathan, & Reubold, Ulrich. (2021). Accent reversion in older adults. In Beaman, K.V. & Buchstaller, I. (eds.), Language variation and language change across the lifespan. New York: Routledge. 119137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hazen, Kirk. (2006). IN/ING variable. In Brown, K. (ed.), Encyclopedia of language and linguistics (2nd ed). Amsterdam: Elsevier. 581584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, Janet. (1992). Women’s talk in public contexts. Discourse & Society 3(2):131150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huddleston, Rodney, & Pullum, Geoffrey. (2002). The Cambridge grammar of the English language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, Max. (2021). caret: Classification and regression training. R package v. . https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=caret.Google Scholar
Labov, William. (2001). Principles of linguistic change. Volume 2: Social factors. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lausberg, Hedda, & Sloetjes, Han (2009). Coding gestural behavior with the NEUROGES-ELAN system. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers 41(3):841849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenth, Russel V. (2022). emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. R package v. . https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans.Google Scholar
Liaw, Andy, & Wiener, Matthew. (2002). Classification and regression by randomForest. R News 2(3):1822.Google Scholar
Lippi-Green, Rosina. (2012). English with an accent. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macaulay, Ronald K.S. (1978). Language, social class and education. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
MacKenzie, Laurel. (2017). Frequency effects over the lifespan: A case study of Attenborough’s r’s. Linguistic Vanguard 3:112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mechler, Johanna, & Buchstaller, Isabelle. (2019). [In]stability in the use of a stable variable. Linguistic Vanguard 5:113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johanna, Mechler, James, Grama, Lea, Bauernfeind, Eiswirth, Mirjam E., & Buchstaller, Isabelle. (2022). Toward an empirically-based model of age-graded behaviour: Trac(ing) linguistic malleability across the entire adult life-span. Penn Working Papers in Linguistics 28(2):101110.Google Scholar
Montgomery, Chris. (2018). The perceptual dialectology of England. In Braber, N. & Jansen, S. (eds.), Sociolinguistics in England. London: Palgrave Macmillan. 127164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ofsted. (2019). Ofsted report of Cranberry Academy. London: Ofsted.Google Scholar
Paterson, Laura. (2019). Interview with Erin Carrie and Rob Drummond of The Accentism Project. Journal of Language and Discrimination 2(1):7684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pichler, Heike, Wagner, Suzanne Evans, & Hesson, Ashley. (2018). Old‐age language variation and change: Confronting variationist ageism. Language and Linguistics Compass 12(6):.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pike, Andy, O’Brien, Peter, & Tomaney, John. (2006). Devolution and the Trades Union Congress in North East England and Wales. Regional & Federal Studies 16(2):157178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Rickford, John, & McNair-Knox, Faye. (1994). Addressee- and topic-influenced style shift: A quantitative sociolinguistic study. In Biber, D. & Finegan, E. (eds.), Sociolinguistic perspectives on register. Cary: Oxford University Press. 235276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, Gillian, & Wagner, Suzanne. (2020). The long tail of language change: A trend and panel study of Québecois French futures. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 65(2):.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, Gillian. (2019). Language change across the lifespan: Three trajectory types. Language 95(2):197229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, Gillian. (2017). Before there were corpora: The evolution of the Montreal French Project as a longitudinal study. In Wagner, S.E. & Buchstaller, I. (eds.), Panel studies of variation and change (1st ed.). New York: Routledge. 2152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, Gillian, & Blondeau, Hélène. (2006). Language change across the lifespan: /r/ in Montreal French. Language 83:560588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schleef, Erik, Meyerhoff, Miriam, & Clark, Lynn. (2011). Teenagers’ acquisition of variation. English World-Wide 32:206236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snell, Julia. (2013). Dialect, interaction and class positioning at school. Language and Education 27(2):110128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snell, Julia. (2018). Solidarity, stance and class identities. Language in Society 47(5):.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snell, Julia, & Cushing, Ian. (2022). “A lot of them write how they speak”: Policy, pedagogy and the policing of “nonstandard” English. Literacy 56(3):199211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sundgren, Eva, Buchstaller, Isabelle, , & Beaman, Karen V. (2021). The beginnings of panel research: Individual language variation, change and stability in Eskilstuna. In Beaman, K.V. & Buchstaller, I. (eds.), Language variation and language change across the lifespan: Theoretical and empirical perspectives from panel studies. New York: Routledge. 1755.Google Scholar
Tamminga, Meredith. (2016). Persistence in phonological and morphological variation. Language Variation and Change 28(3):335356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trudgill, Peter. (1974). The social differentiation of English in Norwich. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vall, Natasha. (2007). Cities in decline?: A comparative history of Malmö and Newcastle after 1945. Malmö: Malmö University Press.Google Scholar
Hofwegen, Van, Janneke, & Wolfram, Walt. (2010). Coming of age in African American English. Journal of Sociolinguistics 14:2752.Google Scholar
Wagner, Suzanne Evans. (2012a). Age grading in sociolinguistic theory. Language and Linguistics Compass 6:371382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, Suzanne Evans. (2012b). Real-time evidence for age grad(ing) in late adolescence. Language Variation and Change 24:179202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, Suzanne Evans, & Sankoff, Gillian. (2011). Age grading in the Montréal French inflected future. Language Variation and Change 23(3):275313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wahl, Hans-Werner, & Kruse, Andreas. (2005). Historical perspectives of middle age within the life span. In Willis, S.L. & Martin, M. (eds.), Middle adulthood: A lifespan perspective. Thousand Oaks: Sage. 334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Dynamic panel dataset across cohort and timepoint; age ranges at time of recording in parentheses

Figure 1

Table 2. Logistic mixed-effects model fit to (ing) at T1 and T2 (n = 6175, 20.7% velar); we present odds ratios (OR) instead of log odds (OR<1 indicates disfavoring of [ŋ]; OR>1 indicates favoring of [ŋ])

Figure 2

Figure 1. Raw proportion of (ing) variants (excluding pronouns) across timepoint by cohort.

Figure 3

Figure 2. (ing) proportions for all speakers (excluding pronominals) across timepoint.

Figure 4

Figure 3. (ing) proportions in subsample of educators before and after leaving the teaching profession.

Figure 5

Table 3. Logistic mixed-effects model fit to (ing) to a subsample of educators (n = 913, 71.4% velar; OR<1 indicates disfavoring of [ŋ]; OR>1 indicates favoring of [ŋ])

Figure 6

Figure 4. Proportion [n] for each participant across age at time of recording; educators (dark) and noneducators (light).