Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T17:36:22.921Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impact of coffee-derived chlorogenic acid on cognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 October 2024

Karen Johal*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading RG6 6DZ, UK
Dan J. W. Jones
Affiliation:
School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading RG6 6DZ, UK
Lynne Bell
Affiliation:
School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading RG6 6DZ, UK
Julie A. Lovegrove
Affiliation:
Human Department of Food and Nutritional Sciences, University of Reading, Reading RG6 6DZ, UK
Daniel Joseph Lamport
Affiliation:
School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading RG6 6DZ, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Karen Johal, email: k.johal@pgr.reading.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Coffee drinking has been associated with benefits for various health outcomes, with many attributed to the most prevalent family of polyphenols within coffee, chlorogenic acids (CGA). Whilst reviews of the association between coffee and cognition exist, evidence exploring effects of coffee-specific CGA on cognition has yet to be systematically synthesised. The purpose was to systematically review the current literature investigating the relationship between CGA from coffee and cognitive performance. A further objective was to undertake a meta-analysis of relevant randomised controlled trials (RCT). Observational and intervention studies were included if they considered coffee-based CGA consumption in human participants and applied a standardised measure of cognition. Furthermore, intervention studies were required to define the CGA content and include a control group/placebo. Studies were excluded if they examined CGA alone as an extract or supplement. A search of Scopus, PubMed, Web of Science, ScienceDirect and PsycINFO resulted in including twenty-three papers, six of which were interventions. The evidence from the broader systematic review suggests that CGA from coffee may need to be consumed chronically over a sustained period to produce cognitive benefits. However, the meta-analysis of RCT showed no benefits of coffee CGA intake on cognitive function (d = 0.00, 95% CI −0.05, 0.05). Overall, this review included a limited number of studies, the sample sizes were small, and a wide range of cognitive measures have been utilised. This indicates that further, good-quality interventions and RCT are required to systematically explore the conditions under which coffee CGA may provide benefits for cognitive outcomes.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Introduction

Polyphenols are phytochemicals that derive from plant-based foods such as fruits, vegetables, whole grains, tea and coffee and have been shown to affect human health(Reference Grosso1). Previous studies have suggested that polyphenols can help prevent or be beneficial to chronic conditions including heart disease, neurodegenerative disorders, glycaemic disorders and obesity(Reference Cory, Passarelli, Szeto, Tamez and Mattei2,Reference Russo, Spagnuolo, Russo, Tedesco, Moccia and Cervellera3) through a variety of mechanisms such as activating genes related to stress response(Reference Cory, Passarelli, Szeto, Tamez and Mattei2) and autophagy(Reference Russo, Spagnuolo, Russo, Tedesco, Moccia and Cervellera3), as well as upregulating pathways that reduce inflammation(Reference Russo, Spagnuolo, Russo, Tedesco, Moccia and Cervellera3,Reference Abbas, Kausar and Cui4) . Other actions include inhibiting enzymes related to carbohydrate digestion, thus affecting glucose absorption, modulation of hepatic glucose release and increased insulin sensitivity(Reference Leri, Scuto, Ontario, Calabrese, Calabrese, Bucciantini and Stefani5).

Coffee is a primary source of polyphenols for adults in many countries and has been associated with a positive impact on physical health(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6,Reference Ochiai, Saitou, Suzukamo, Osaki and Asada7) . Benefits associated with its consumption include reduced risk of diabetes and liver disease, as well as inverse associations with all-cause and cause-specific mortality(Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8Reference Eskelinen, Ngandu, Tuomilehto, Soininen and Kivipelto11). The impact of coffee on human health has been attributed to its bioactive compounds, the most prevalent of which is a family of phenolic acids called chlorogenic acids (CGA)(Reference Chuang, Lo, Wu, Wang and Pan12Reference Noguchi-Shinohara, Yuki and Dohmoto14). The biological activity of these compounds in coffee has been shown to be affected by a variety of factors including coffee bean species, roasting process and coffee brewing method(Reference Noguchi-Shinohara, Yuki and Dohmoto14Reference Ng, Zhong, Gao, Gwee, Chua and Larbi17). The processes of decaffeination and production of instant coffee appear to have negligible effects on CGA content, while the degree of roasting seems to have the largest impact(Reference Mills, Oruna-Concha, Mottram, Gibson and Spencer18). As coffee is roasted, CGA content decreases, but bioavailability increases until a certain point at which CGA content and activity both deteriorate(Reference Kamiyama, Moon, Jang and Shibamoto19). Light- and medium-roasted coffees have been suggested to reach the highest antioxidant capacity(Reference Bobková, Hudáček and Jakabová20Reference Zanin, Corso, Kitzberger, dos Santos Scholz and de Toledo Benassi22). Coffee’s country of origin has also been found to affect its polyphenol content(Reference Król, Gantner, Tatarak and Hallmann23), and though studies on the polyphenol content from specific coffee-producing regions are limited, one available study found coffees originating in Ethiopia and India contained more polyphenols, yet coffee from Colombia was most resistant to polyphenol loss during roasting(Reference Dybkowska, Sadowska, Rakowska, Debowska, Swiderski and Swiader21). Moreover, coffees with good cup quality are associated with higher levels of CGA(Reference Zanin, Corso, Kitzberger, dos Santos Scholz and de Toledo Benassi22). Cup quality is determined by a grading system standardised by the Specialty Coffee Association of America which has shown consistency through repeated use across countries. Using a standardised cupping form to ensure consistency, this grading system assesses taste, aroma and appearance of coffee beans before and after roasting, and after brewing(Reference Laukaleja and Kruma24). The association between cup quality and CGA content is consistent with findings that higher-quality coffees are rated as such due to the higher concentration of bioactive compounds lending to the taste, flavour and mouthfeel(Reference Bolka and Emire25,Reference Lingle and Menon26) . Polyphenols are known to have high antioxidant activity, and so this has been used as a surrogate marker for polyphenol content(Reference Elejalde, Villarán and Alonso27). Brewing coffee using a drip-filter method, which uses a cone lined with a paper or cloth filter and filled with ground coffee beans placed over a mug and then water poured over to extract brewed coffee, yielded the highest polyphenol content and antioxidant activity, while the convenient capsule-brewing method, which uses a pod filled with ground coffee inserted into a machine that then pushes hot water through the pod to extract brewed coffee, has the lowest antioxidant capacity(Reference Isac-Torrente, Fernandez-Gomez and Miguel28,Reference Miłek, Młodecki and Dżugan29) . These findings suggest that a filter-brewed, light- or medium-roasted coffee may be optimal for extracting bioactive compounds such as CGA.

CGA extract has been associated with beneficial physical health outcomes. These include improved blood pressure in people with normal blood pressure and mild hypertension, favourable changes in lipid and glucose metabolism in human participants and increased insulin sensitivity with doses ranging from 156 to 369 mg CGA(Reference Onakpoya, Spencer, Thompson and Heneghan30,Reference Bosso, Barbalho, de Alvares Goulart and Otoboni31) . Daily supplementation with a blend containing CGA was found to reduce basal blood glucose levels in adults with pre-existing type 2 diabetes(Reference Tajik, Tajik, Mack and Enck32,Reference Ahrens and Thompson33) . Risk of diabetes was reduced by 30% in people drinking up to four cups of decaffeinated coffee(Reference Tajik, Tajik, Mack and Enck32), emphasising the role of non-caffeine constituents in coffee(Reference Santana-Gálvez, Cisneros-Zevallos and Jacobo-Velázquez34).

Cognitive benefits related to CGA have also been observed. One in vitro study found that a coffee polyphenol extract, which included CGA, prevented cognitive dysfunction in rodent models of Alzheimer’s disease by degrading hippocampal amyloid β plaques, a hallmark of Alzheimer’s disease(Reference Santana-Gálvez, Cisneros-Zevallos and Jacobo-Velázquez34). Epidemiological studies have highlighted the reduced risk of neurodegenerative conditions such as Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease with three to five cups of coffee daily, which is considered to be a moderate level of coffee consumption(Reference Eskelinen, Ngandu, Tuomilehto, Soininen and Kivipelto11,Reference Wierzejska36,Reference Dostal, Roberts and Link37) . This same moderate coffee consumption may reduce the risk of cognitive decline in healthy participants as well, and one trial found improvements in motor speed, psychomotor speed and executive function for the group administered CGA when compared with placebo(Reference Fukutomi, Ohishi, Koyama, Pervin, Nakamura and Isemura38). CGA’s neuroprotective effects may arise from its ability to pass the blood–brain barrier(Reference Upadhyay, Ramalakshmi and Rao39,Reference Heitman and Ingram40) . CGA has been implicated in a number of mechanisms that reduce oxidative stress in neuronal cells, including reducing nuclear condensation, which is associated with apoptosis, and suppression of cytokine TNF-α, an inflammatory biomarker which has been associated with insulin resistance and poor glucose control(Reference Fukutomi, Ohishi, Koyama, Pervin, Nakamura and Isemura38). With respect to specific cognitive domains, rat models indicate that CGA may have the greatest effects on spatial learning, memory impairment and motor function(Reference Heitman and Ingram40), although it should be acknowledged that the range of cognitive domains which can be assessed in rodent models is limited.

Most coffee studies focus on the cognitive benefits of caffeine, but evidence is available that caffeine does not act alone in affecting cognitive function. An animal model of Alzheimer’s disease demonstrated that non-caffeine compounds in coffee such as cafestol and kahweol may be responsible for neuroprotective activity(Reference Dostal, Roberts and Link37) and may be acting in synergy with caffeine and other polyphenols(Reference Shukitt-Hale, Miller, Chu, Lyle and Joseph41). A neuronal culture study by Kim et al.(Reference Kim, Lee and Shim42) found that exposing cells to 0.05 mg of caffeinated coffee and decaffeinated coffee, and 0.05 mg CGA as an extract, was neuroprotective against reactive oxygen species. Moreover, caffeinated and decaffeinated coffee showed similar efficacy, emphasising that non-caffeine bioactives may be driving these protective effects(Reference Kim, Lee and Shim42). Cropley et al.(Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10) conducted a randomised controlled trial with older adults and manipulated CGA content in caffeinated and decaffeinated coffee to investigate effects on brain function using single dosages of 300–521 mg CGA. Cognitive function was tested 40 min after ingestion, with a 1-week washout period between dosages. This study found that decaffeinated coffee fortified with CGA positively impacted mood and behaviour more than regular caffeinated coffee(Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10), again suggesting the action of non-caffeine compounds in cognitive function.

There is a growing body of evidence examining the impact of CGA on cognitive function. However, evidence exploring the specific cognitive impact of CGA from coffee allowing for consideration of the food matrix, as opposed to an extract in which the bioactive properties of CGA may be different, has yet to be systematically synthesised(Reference Tajik, Tajik, Mack and Enck32,Reference Wierzejska36,Reference Arab, Khan and Lam43,Reference Farah and de Paula Lima44) . Therefore, the aims of this systematic review and meta-analysis were (i) to explore whether changes in cognitive function could be attributed to CGA when consumed in coffee and (ii) to understand if certain cognitive domains are affected by CGA.

Method

Methods for conducting this review were pre-specified in a registered protocol on PROSPERO (registration number CRD42021242345). This systematic literature review was performed using the following search strategy. The Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions and Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines were used to guide this analysis, and the PRISMA flowchart is illustrated in Figure 1.

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow diagram for identifying studies for inclusion.

Search terms

Literature was searched using the search terms Coffee*, Chlorogenic acid* and Cognit* applying the Boolean operator ‘and’. Truncations were used to ensure capture of all possible terms related to the search. Combinations of terms were as follows: coffee* and cognit*; chlorogenic acid* and cognit*; coffee* AND chlorogenic acid* and cognit*; coffee AND cognitive function NOT caffeine; coffee AND cognition NOT caffeine; chlorogenic acid AND cognition; chlorogenic acid AND cognition NOT caffeine; chlorogenic acid AND cognitive function; chlorogenic acid AND cognitive function NOT caffeine; coffee intake AND cognition NOT caffeine. The searches yielded 445 results from the databases below.

Databases

The following databases were searched: Scopus (1823 to April 2023), PubMed (1948 to April 2023), Web of Science (1986 to April 2023), ScienceDirect (1995 to April 2023), PsycINFO (1866 to April 2023). A hand search of references from the included studies was also conducted, yielding one result. This review focused on formally published papers; thus, grey literature databases were not searched.

Searches were run through ‘all fields’ for all databases, and then filtered on the basis of the high number of results. In Scopus, only articles were included in the search, and studies using animal or disease models were excluded. In PubMed, only human trials were included; and books, documents, meta-analysis and reviews were excluded. In Web of Science, human studies and articles were included in the search, and for cases where results were ample, a search within the results using the keyword ‘chlorogenic acid’ was performed. In ScienceDirect, the following keywords were applied to the initial search results before being filtered to include only research articles: coffee, cognition, cognitive function, chlorogenic acid. In PsycINFO, search results were limited to peer-reviewed journals and journal articles, and human participants.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Studies were included if they examined the effect of coffee on cognitive function in both healthy and clinical adult human participants; this included caffeinated, decaffeinated and CGA-fortified coffee. Intervention studies, such as RCT, and observational cross-sectional or longitudinal studies were included. A further inclusion criterion was a standardised measure of cognition, meaning a (validated/reliable) measurement of cognitive function not a diagnostic tool, and therefore, studies with a clinical diagnosis of cognitive function only were excluded. The Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) is often used as a diagnostic tool; however, it is a screening tool that measures cognitive impairment, and thus, studies using this questionnaire were retained(Reference Monroe and Carter45). Only papers available in English were included. For interventions, only studies stating the dosage of CGA were included; however, this criterion did not apply to cohort studies. Exclusion criteria were systematic reviews or meta-analyses and studies which examined CGA alone as an extract or supplement, or if they examined CGA from non-coffee food items. For intervention trials, absence of a control group or placebo was an exclusion criterion.

Data extraction

Study selection was initially performed by K.J. Papers were excluded on the basis of the title if it was evident that the research fell outside of the inclusion criteria specified, e.g. animal studies. Selected studies were then read in full to determine eligibility for inclusion.

Studies selected for inclusion were assessed for overall methodology quality and potential risk of bias using the Evidence Analysis Manual Quality Criteria Checklist (QCC) from the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics (2016). Studies were assessed independently by K.J. and J.L. with disagreements resolved with a third party as needed.

Data were then extracted from the included studies independently by K.J. and J.L. Only data relevant to cognitive outcomes associated with CGA intake were extracted for analysis (for example, data relating to other measures such as physical, psychological and biochemical outcome measures were not extracted). The following data were collated from each study: the first author’s last name, publication year, participants, CGA source and dose for RCT, study design, and standardised measures of cognition and their means and standard deviations.

Data synthesis

STATA (Version 17.0; StataCorp, College Station, TX) was used to perform the meta-analysis with the six RCT to explore the impact of CGA on cognitive function. Pre- and post-intervention means and standard deviations or standard errors were extracted for both control and treatment groups. One of the randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover trials, Ochiai et al.(Reference Ochiai, Saitou, Suzukamo, Osaki and Asada7), had only baseline participant data for pre-intervention data, used an average of both groups to calculate a baseline mean and did not differentiate between control or intervention data until after the intervention.

Change-from-baseline scores were calculated for the control group and treatment group. The study by Camfield et al.(Reference Camfield, Silber, Scholey, Nolidin, Goh and Stough9) had multiple treatment arms, so data from two treatment groups were combined as they used the same dosage of CGA(Reference Higgins and Green46). For another case(Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10) with multiple treatment arms using different dosages of CGA, one arm was selected on the basis of best practice to only combine relevant intervention groups or to otherwise choose one group to analyse(Reference Higgins and Green46). This study used CGA in dosages of 224, 244 and 521 mg daily for 3 d; the 521 mg arm was chosen for analysis as the other included RCT used at least 300 mg of CGA(Reference Higgins and Green46). These cases were discussed and agreed upon by all authors. For change-from-baseline means, the end score was subtracted from the baseline score. Change from baseline standard deviations were imputed using the following formula:

$$\eqalign{{\rm SD_{Echange}}= & \surd[{\rm SD^2_{Ebaseline}}+{\rm SD^2_{Efinal}}\cr &-(2\times{\rm{Correlation \,Coefficient\ }}\cr &\hskip12pt{\times\ S}{{\rm{D}}_{{\rm{Ebaseline}}}\times {\rm{ SD}}_{{\rm{Efinal}}})]}}$$

A correlation coefficient of 0.5 was assumed as recommended by Cochrane and others since this value was not provided and could not be calculated from the given data (e.g. Refs. Reference Higgins and Green46Reference Garg49).

Once change scores had been calculated, the data were imported into STATA, allowing the forest plots to be generated using a random effects model and Cohen’s d. Heterogeneity was evaluated using the I 2 statistic (%), with ≤30%, between 30% and 50%, between 50% and 75%, and ≥75% indicating low, moderate, substantial and considerable heterogeneity, respectively(Reference Farah and de Paula Lima44). Risk of bias was assessed with the AND’s QCC.

Two subgroup meta-analyses were performed to understand the effect of CGA on cognitive function by study and then the effect by cognitive domain. For the former, tests were grouped by study and analysed by STATA; effect sizes were reversed for individual tests where lower scores reflected better performance (e.g. reaction time) to account for directionality.

For the cognitive domain analysis, each cognitive test used in each of the six RCT studies for the meta-analysis was classified into cognitive domains according to the Cattell–Horn–Carrol (CHC) model of cognition. The CHC model is a factor analysis-based taxonomy of cognitive abilities that attempts to create a common structure and vocabulary for communicating cognitive research by bringing together multiple theories of cognition(Reference Keith and Reynolds50,Reference Schneider and McGrew51) . It has been supported for cognitive assessment in healthy and clinical populations. The CHC model categorises cognitive function into broad abilities, narrow abilities and specific abilities. Specific abilities can be measured directly with tasks, and these are clustered by correlation into narrow abilities, and highly correlated narrow abilities are then clustered into broad abilities(Reference Schneider and McGrew51). Broad abilities were used to categorise tasks into cognitive domains and include visuospatial ability, working memory, long-term memory encoding and retrieval, acquired knowledge, processing speed and fluid reasoning(Reference Jewsbury, Bowden and Duff52). One major area of critique is the lack of executive function in this model. However, the argument has been made that executive function is poorly defined and generally refers to a range of other cognitive functions. The CHC model has been tested and shown to capture these functions embedded within other domains(Reference Jewsbury, Bowden and Duff52). This categorisation allowed composite scores to be calculated for each cognitive domain represented in each study, allowing for each cognitive domain to be represented in the meta-analysis. Composite scores were calculated by combining change from baseline means and standard deviations for treatment and control groups in each study.

Results

Initial searches yielded 445 studies, 25 of which were duplicates, leaving 445 studies. After screening and checking for inclusions/exclusion criteria, twenty-three studies remained. Additional hand-searching yielded one further RCT.

Study characteristics

In total, twenty-three studies were obtained, six of which were clinical trial interventions(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6, Reference Ochiai, Saitou, Suzukamo, Osaki and Asada7, Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8, Reference Camfield, Silber, Scholey, Nolidin, Goh and Stough9, Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10, Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53) and seventeen of which were observational; of the latter, nine were longitudinal studies(Reference Eskelinen, Ngandu, Tuomilehto, Soininen and Kivipelto11,Reference Fischer, Melo van Lent and Wolfsgruber13,Reference Noguchi-Shinohara, Yuki and Dohmoto14,Reference Zupo, Griseta and Battista16,Reference Arab, Khan and Lam43,Reference Cho, Choi, Kim and Kim54Reference Gardener, Rainey-Smith and Villemagne57) and eight were cross-sectional studies(Reference Chuang, Lo, Wu, Wang and Pan12, Reference Huang, Huang, Lee, Chang and Doong15, Reference Ng, Zhong, Gao, Gwee, Chua and Larbi17, Reference Valls-Pedret, Lamuela-Raventós and Medina-Remon58Reference Heggy, Masoumi, Al-Yafei, Al-Bader, Al-Abdi and Shi62) (Tables 1 and 2).

RCT

Five RCT were crossover trials(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6,Reference Ochiai, Saitou, Suzukamo, Osaki and Asada7,Reference Camfield, Silber, Scholey, Nolidin, Goh and Stough9,Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10,Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53) , and one was a parallel-groups trial(Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8), all of which lasted between 4 and 48 weeks. Papers were published between the years 2012 and 2020. Five RCT included healthy adults(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6,Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10,Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53) , and one included participants with mild cognitive impairment(Reference Ochiai, Saitou, Suzukamo, Osaki and Asada7). Four RCT included aging adults, aged 50 or older(Reference Ochiai, Saitou, Suzukamo, Osaki and Asada7Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10), and two included younger participants aged 18–35 years(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6,Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53) . Three trials investigated the immediate (acute) effects of CGA ingestion and measured outcomes at intervals up to 3 h(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6,Reference Camfield, Silber, Scholey, Nolidin, Goh and Stough9,Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10) . These were all crossover trials and had each condition separated by a washout period(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6,Reference Camfield, Silber, Scholey, Nolidin, Goh and Stough9,Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10) . The remaining two studies were long-term (chronic) trials with regular CGA ingestion over the course of 4–16 weeks; these studies measured outcomes at baseline, midway and end point(Reference Ochiai, Saitou, Suzukamo, Osaki and Asada7,Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8,Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53) .

The dosage of CGA ranged from 224 to 553 mg. CGA was administered as a beverage in all trials, with one trial using coffee flavour(Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10) and the remaining trials using another flavour(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6Reference Camfield, Silber, Scholey, Nolidin, Goh and Stough9,Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53) .

Twenty-one different cognitive tests were used including two distinct global batteries, and data were reported across nine cognitive domains, as defined by the CHC model(Reference Jewsbury, Bowden and Duff52).

Observational studies

From the seventeen observational studies selected, nine were longitudinal cohort studies(Reference Eskelinen, Ngandu, Tuomilehto, Soininen and Kivipelto11,Reference Fischer, Melo van Lent and Wolfsgruber13,Reference Noguchi-Shinohara, Yuki and Dohmoto14,Reference Zupo, Griseta and Battista16,Reference Arab, Khan and Lam43,Reference Cho, Choi, Kim and Kim54Reference Gardener, Rainey-Smith and Villemagne57) and eight were cross-sectional studies(Reference Chuang, Lo, Wu, Wang and Pan12,Reference Huang, Huang, Lee, Chang and Doong15,Reference Ng, Zhong, Gao, Gwee, Chua and Larbi17,Reference Valls-Pedret, Lamuela-Raventós and Medina-Remon58Reference Heggy, Masoumi, Al-Yafei, Al-Bader, Al-Abdi and Shi62) . The studies were published between 2002 and 2021. These observational studies took place over 1–20 years.

It was understood that quantifying CGA content would be difficult in cohort studies, and indeed no cohort study was found that measured CGA content and instead coffee intake was measured using self-reports and food frequency questionnaires (FFQ). Most observational studies considered older adults(Reference Eskelinen, Ngandu, Tuomilehto, Soininen and Kivipelto11Reference Ng, Zhong, Gao, Gwee, Chua and Larbi17,Reference Arab, Khan and Lam43,Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53,Reference Araújo, Mirza and Bos55,Reference Gardener, Rainey-Smith and Villemagne57,Reference Kim, Yu, Choi, Nam, Kim, Oh and Yang60,Reference Dong, Li, Sun, Li and Zhang61) , while one had a lower mean age of 38.5 years(Reference Heggy, Masoumi, Al-Yafei, Al-Bader, Al-Abdi and Shi62). Three observational studies looked at clinical populations including older adults with cardiovascular risks(Reference Valls-Pedret, Lamuela-Raventós and Medina-Remon58), participants recently diagnosed with Parkinson’s disease(Reference Cho, Choi, Kim and Kim54) and adults with HIV(Reference Bragança, Marinho, Marques, Moreira, Palha, Marques-Teixeira and Esteves59).

Effects on cognitive domain

Cognitive domains were explored with the aforementioned CHC model of cognition. Nine studies looked at memory: four RCT(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6,Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8,Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10,Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53) and five observational(Reference Araújo, Mirza and Bos55,Reference Johnson-Kozlow, Kritz-Silverstein, Barrett-Connor and Morton56,Reference Valls-Pedret, Lamuela-Raventós and Medina-Remon58,Reference Bragança, Marinho, Marques, Moreira, Palha, Marques-Teixeira and Esteves59,Reference Dong, Li, Sun, Li and Zhang61) . The four RCT found limited effects of coffee on cognition. However, four of the five observational studies found positive effects of coffee intake on memory(Reference Johnson-Kozlow, Kritz-Silverstein, Barrett-Connor and Morton56,Reference Valls-Pedret, Lamuela-Raventós and Medina-Remon58,Reference Bragança, Marinho, Marques, Moreira, Palha, Marques-Teixeira and Esteves59,Reference Dong, Li, Sun, Li and Zhang61) . These observational studies looked at populations over periods ranging from 1 to 16 years. The absence of effects for the RCT with contrasting benefits observed in studies of greater than 1 year duration may indicate that long-term coffee CGA intake is needed to see beneficial effects on memory.

Six studies explored working memory, three of which were observational studies(Reference Johnson-Kozlow, Kritz-Silverstein, Barrett-Connor and Morton56,Reference Valls-Pedret, Lamuela-Raventós and Medina-Remon58,Reference Bragança, Marinho, Marques, Moreira, Palha, Marques-Teixeira and Esteves59) and three of which were RCT(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6,Reference Camfield, Silber, Scholey, Nolidin, Goh and Stough9,Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53) . All three observational studies found positive correlations between working memory and coffee intake, with two of those studies finding that women who drank coffee regularly performed especially well on memory tasks(Reference Johnson-Kozlow, Kritz-Silverstein, Barrett-Connor and Morton56,Reference Valls-Pedret, Lamuela-Raventós and Medina-Remon58) . One RCT(Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53) found a beneficial impact on working memory; however, the remaining two RCT found no difference between the control group or the CGA(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6) or in fact a negative impact of CGA on memory(Reference Camfield, Silber, Scholey, Nolidin, Goh and Stough9). This latter study tested working memory using the serial 3’s and 7’s task(Reference Camfield, Silber, Scholey, Nolidin, Goh and Stough9), while Jackson et al.(Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53) worked with a younger population with a mean age of 23 years and measured memory with a word recall. Two observational studies which did find an association used a digit-based recall task(Reference Valls-Pedret, Lamuela-Raventós and Medina-Remon58,Reference Bragança, Marinho, Marques, Moreira, Palha, Marques-Teixeira and Esteves59) , whilst cross-sectional assessment with a word-based task(Reference Johnson-Kozlow, Kritz-Silverstein, Barrett-Connor and Morton56) showed weaker benefits. The difference in cognitive measures could potentially explain these varying outcomes for memory, with some tests such as digit-based tasks being more sensitive than others, although there is not currently enough evidence here to systematically examine this. As mentioned, one possible emerging pattern is that consumption over a longer duration is required for observable benefits to memory.

Processing speed was explored in twelve studies, six of which found associations with coffee drinking(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6,Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8,Reference Araújo, Mirza and Bos55Reference Gardener, Rainey-Smith and Villemagne57,Reference Bragança, Marinho, Marques, Moreira, Palha, Marques-Teixeira and Esteves59,Reference Dong, Li, Sun, Li and Zhang61) . Two of the six RCT in this group found significant effects of coffee-derived CGA on processing speed(Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8,Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53) Five of six observational studies found positive associations between coffee intake and processing speed(Reference Araújo, Mirza and Bos55Reference Gardener, Rainey-Smith and Villemagne57,Reference Bragança, Marinho, Marques, Moreira, Palha, Marques-Teixeira and Esteves59,Reference Dong, Li, Sun, Li and Zhang61) . Dong et al.(Reference Dong, Li, Sun, Li and Zhang61) found that processing speed, measured by performance on the digit symbol substitution test, was better preserved in people who regularly consumed caffeinated coffee, but not decaffeinated, while Johnson-Kozlow et al.(Reference Johnson-Kozlow, Kritz-Silverstein, Barrett-Connor and Morton56) found that decaffeinated coffee drinking was associated with better processing speed as measured with the trail making test part B. These contrasting findings on the effects of decaffeinated coffee may warrant further investigation and lend support to the suggestion that there are effects of coffee which are independent of any caffeine effects. Furthermore, these findings may indicate that long-term intake is needed to observe effects, and that coffee CGA may act synergistically with other compounds such as caffeine to influence processing speed.

The majority of observational studies looked at global cognitive function. Eleven of twelve observational studies found positive associations between coffee drinking and global cognitive function. Two studies found positive associations specifically for women(Reference Arab, Khan and Lam43,Reference Johnson-Kozlow, Kritz-Silverstein, Barrett-Connor and Morton56) , and one found positive effects only for caffeinated coffee(Reference Dong, Li, Sun, Li and Zhang61). Kim et al.(Reference Kim, Yu, Choi, Nam, Kim, Oh and Yang60) found negative effects from coffee intake but explored coffee as part of a food pattern rather than exploring coffee on its own. This study created food patterns based on regular intake of participants; the authors found that people who regularly drank coffee also ate refined grains more often and, thus, investigated coffee as part of a ‘white rice–noodles–coffee’ food pattern, which was the dietary pattern associated with poorer cognitive function(Reference Kim, Yu, Choi, Nam, Kim, Oh and Yang60). This diet pattern can be considered as similar to a Western diet which is high in refined grains, which has previously been associated with poorer cognitive functioning(Reference Torres, Lautenschlager, Wattanapenpaiboon, Greenop, Beer, Flicker, Alfonso and Nowson63). Furthermore, this study did not investigate if coffee was consumed with or without additions, such as milk and sugar, which could be important as a typical Western diet tends to be higher in sugar-sweetened beverages which can negatively impact cognitive function(Reference Torres, Lautenschlager, Wattanapenpaiboon, Greenop, Beer, Flicker, Alfonso and Nowson63,Reference Sievenpiper and de Souza64) . Moreover, only one observational study in this review assessed additions to coffee, such as milk and sugar, but did not analyse this data as a covariate(Reference Dong, Li, Sun, Li and Zhang61) despite findings that adding dairy to coffee decreases urinary output of CGA and its metabolites, implying poorer absorption of these bioactive compounds(Reference Duarte and Farah66). Additions to coffee may also affect the volume of coffee being consumed, which itself could decrease exposure and absorption of polyphenols. A recent review concluded that adding cow’s milk to coffee may have a negative effect on the functionality of polyphenols, but outcomes of consuming milk and coffee together may be dose dependent(Reference Rashidinejad, Tarhan and Rezaei67). However, there is limited evidence available at present to understand the optimal ratio of coffee to milk(Reference Rashidinejad, Tarhan and Rezaei67). This finding may indicate that the neuroprotective effects of coffee are helped or hindered by other dietary factors(Reference Socała, Szopa, Serefko, Poleszak and Wlaź68,Reference Iranpour, Saadati, Koohi and Sabour69) . In support, Jackson et al.(Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53) reported that a combination of 440 mg CGA and 275 mg of apple polyphenols significantly improved performance on the peg and ball task. Investigating how foods commonly added to and consumed along with coffee impact cognition may be worthwhile in understanding how to maximise the potential cognitive benefits of CGA-rich coffee.

Some cognitive domains had limited data. Motor function was explored by only two observational studies with no associations found. Reaction time was explored by only two RCT(Reference Camfield, Silber, Scholey, Nolidin, Goh and Stough9,Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10) , which presented similar findings in terms of improvement with intake of coffee-based CGA, and one observational study(Reference Heggy, Masoumi, Al-Yafei, Al-Bader, Al-Abdi and Shi62), which found an inverse relationship between coffee intake and mean reaction time. Visual processing was explored by one RCT and found to be positively impacted by CGA derived from coffee(Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8,Reference Draheim, Mashburn, Martin and Engle70) . These may be worthwhile domains to explore as they all are important in retaining quality of life, and visual processing and reaction time have been shown to be affected by age and chronic disease(Reference Sreenivasan and Kusumadevi71).

Sex differences

Differences between males and females were observed in three(Reference Eskelinen, Ngandu, Tuomilehto, Soininen and Kivipelto11,Reference Arab, Khan and Lam43,Reference Johnson-Kozlow, Kritz-Silverstein, Barrett-Connor and Morton56) of the five studies looking at sex differences(Reference Eskelinen, Ngandu, Tuomilehto, Soininen and Kivipelto11,Reference Ng, Zhong, Gao, Gwee, Chua and Larbi17,Reference Arab, Khan and Lam43,Reference Johnson-Kozlow, Kritz-Silverstein, Barrett-Connor and Morton56,Reference Valls-Pedret, Lamuela-Raventós and Medina-Remon58) , and indeed previous studies have reported declines in the majority of cognitive domains for both sexes over the lifespan(Reference Gurvich, Hoy, Thomas and Kulkarni72). One observational study suggested the protective effects of midlife coffee drinking were more evident in males(Reference Eskelinen, Ngandu, Tuomilehto, Soininen and Kivipelto11). Gurvich et al.(Reference Gurvich, Hoy, Thomas and Kulkarni72) reported that healthy males showed greater decline in specific domains such as motor speed and visuospatial processing while females showed resistance to steep declines in cognitive function. This susceptibility to age-related changes may allow for greater observable differences. Another study found that current male coffee drinkers, mean age 73 years, scored poorer than females on backwards spelling tests(Reference Johnson-Kozlow, Kritz-Silverstein, Barrett-Connor and Morton56), suggesting that the time of life that coffee is consumed may also be a factor in coffee’s protective effects. Two observational studies both looking at older adults suggested more favourable associations of lifetime coffee drinking in females than males on global cognitive measures and memory tasks(Reference Arab, Khan and Lam43,Reference Johnson-Kozlow, Kritz-Silverstein, Barrett-Connor and Morton56) . Physiological differences exist between males and females that have been found to interact with the activity of polyphenols. For example, the polyphenols in red wine relaxed aortic rings in female rodents, and this was attributed to the polyphenols increasing the expression of endothelial nitric oxide synthase (eNOS), an important factor regulating the cardiovascular system, which was higher in female rats(Reference Kane, Anselm, Rattmann, Auger and Schini-Kerth73). Another example is the antihypertensive effects soy was found to have in men and postmenopausal women(Reference Campesi, Marino, Cipolletti, Romani and Franconi74). Even oxidative damage may differ between sexes, as one rodent study found that levels of reactive oxygen species scavengers such as superoxide dismutase was lower in male rodents and higher in females while levels of hydrogen peroxide were higher in males and lower in females, which implies an unequal baseline(Reference Campesi, Marino, Cipolletti, Romani and Franconi74). Further exploration into cognitive differences and changes over the lifespan based on sex and health status could reveal specific domains that could be bolstered by CGA-rich coffee.

Meta-analysis

The meta-analyses looking at changes in cognitive function from CGA-based interventions from the six RCT included in this review(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10,Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster53) showed no significant effects of coffee CGA on cognition (d = 0.00, 95% CI −0.05, 0.05; Figure 2). All studies included in the meta-analysis used standardised measures of cognition to evaluate differences in cognitive function before and after the CGA intervention, and specified the dosage of CGA used in that intervention.

Fig. 2. Random-effects DerSimonian–Laird model.

Some important differences amongst these RCT exist, such as the population studied. The study by Saitou et al.(Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8) enrolled participants complaining of memory changes, while Cropley et al.(Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10) included participants on the basis of MMSE scores above 24. Moreover, Saitou et al.(Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8) provided 300 mg of CGA as opposed to 521 mg of CGA in Cropley et al.’s study(Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10). Therefore, the absence of benefits in Saitou et al.’s(Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8) study could be explained by the cognitively impaired population and a relatively low dose. Furthermore, the negative outcome for CGA may be accounted for by general poorer functioning in the CGA group compared with the placebo group in this parallel-groups design. In this meta-analysis, the analyses by cognitive domain showed no significant effects of CGA (Supplemental Tables 24–29).

Table 1. Characteristics of randomised controlled trials (RCT)

Table 2. Characteristics of observational studies

Risk of bias

All papers were assessed using the QCC and deemed ‘positive’ or ‘neutral’ (Supplemental Tables 1–23). Overall, the risk of bias from participant selection and blinding was low, but potential areas for higher risk of bias included funding and withdrawal management. Implications are explored in the discussion below.

Discussion

Overall, the evidence from the meta-analysis with the six, good-quality RCT does not support an effect of coffee-based CGA on cognition, nor was there evidence from the meta-analysis showing effects on specific cognitive domains. Inconsistencies between studies and methodological factors may have contributed to the null findings in the meta-analysis, particularly given that there was some supportive evidence from the wider systematic review. Findings from the broader systematic review include positive effects of coffee-derived CGA and coffee consumption on memory, attention, executive function, alertness and motor activity, and a lower risk of cognitive impairment, dementia and Alzheimer’s disease, after chronic coffee intake.

All studies included in this review had a strong design; however, there were differences in the population selection criteria. Cropley et al.(Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10) found significant results in their population who scored 24 and above on the MMSE at screening, whereas Camfield et al.(Reference Camfield, Silber, Scholey, Nolidin, Goh and Stough9) included participants scoring 27 and higher on the MMSE at screening and found no significant results. Moreover, Ochiai et al.(Reference Ochiai, Saitou, Suzukamo, Osaki and Asada7) found significant effects in a population with mild cognitive impairment. This may indicate that the effects of CGA could be more important for those already experiencing some cognitive impairment, whereas effects in cognitively healthy populations may be more difficult to observe. The length of the intervention may also have an impact. For example, positive effects of CGA were found in two RCT of 12 and 16 weeks duration(Reference Ochiai, Saitou, Suzukamo, Osaki and Asada7,Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8) whereas two RCT with an acute design assessing cognition after a single dose of CGA found no effects(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6, Reference Camfield, Silber, Scholey, Nolidin, Goh and Stough9). Interestingly, Jackson et al.(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey6) used 440 mg and tested participants up to 3 h after ingestion and found no effect; however, benefits were found when participants were tested up to 6 h after ingestion with the same CGA dosage. Multiple studies have found that the majority of CGA is metabolised in the colon by microflora after being hydrolysed by esterases, and that the metabolites resulting from hydrolysis vary depending on an individual’s unique microflora and other compounds or foods consumed with CGA(Reference Li, Su and Chen75, Reference Zeng, Xiang, Fu, Qu and Liu76). Once absorbed, CGA can cross the blood–brain barrier(Reference Zeng, Xiang, Fu, Qu and Liu76). It is known that digestion through the colon can take up to 36 h; thus, it is possible that effects will not be observed in the 0–5 h postprandial period that the aforementioned acute studies tested within(Reference Li, Su and Chen75). This may warrant further exploration into the time-related effects of CGA, as well as the effects of microflora on the bioavailability of CGA. Additionally, the QCC was helpful in highlighting potential bias based on funding, but there were no apparent differences in outcomes based on a positive or neutral rating.

This review had some limitations. Firstly, it contained a limited number of studies each with small sample sizes, a wide range of cognitive measures and an overall small magnitude of effect (Cohen’s d = 0.00). The small degree of variability between studies (I 2 = 0.00%) will likely be biased in this meta-analysis as there are a small number of studies(Reference von Hippel77). One RCT chose to take a group average at baseline prior to randomisation, which can be considered a significant limitation as this approach would prevent researchers from assessing any differences between the intervention and control groups(Reference Ochiai, Saitou, Suzukamo, Osaki and Asada7). Moreover, the small number of studies included in the meta-analysis explored a wide range of CGA dosages, from 300 to 1106 mg, with some of these studies sourcing the CGA from unroasted, green coffee beans and others sourcing from roasted coffee beans. Despite green coffee beans having higher concentrations of CGA, roasted coffee has shown increased bioactivity of CGA(Reference Wu, Gu, Amrit, Nawaz, Barrow, Dunshea and Suleria78,Reference Upadhyay and Mohan Rao79) . Beneficial effects were found by one RCT looking at coffee with regular CGA content (224 mg) versus coffee with high CGA content (521 mg), with the higher-CGA coffee participants reporting improvements in mood(Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10). Unfortunately, there is a lack of data on dose–response effects of CGA on cognitive function. This would be a worthwhile area to explore based on past studies that have demonstrated a dose–response relationship of coffee-derived CGA on plasma content of CGA and blood pressure(Reference Renouf, Marmet, Giuffrida, Lepage, Barron, Beaumont, Williamson and Dionisi80,Reference Yamaguchi, Chikama, Mori, Watanabe, Shioya, Katsuragi and Tokimitsu81) . More studies are needed to explore dose–response effects of CGA derived from unroasted and roasted coffee to better understand how degree of roasting may be affecting cognitive outcomes and human health more widely.

Another limitation to consider is that the positive effects from the observational studies may have been impacted by confounding factors such as dietary choices and lifestyle behaviours. Moreover, the lack of data on CGA dosages available from observational studies prevented the narrative portion of the review from exploring the effects of coffee-derived CGA, allowing only an exploration of coffee drinking on cognitive function. This brings forth the limitation of the non-standardised measure of coffee intake. The observational studies employed different food frequency questionnaires administered at various intervals that captured coffee as daily, weekly or monthly intake. Other studies opted for 24-h diet recalls, or even used an interview schedule to simply categorise participants as coffee drinkers or not, regardless of intake frequency. This may have led to overestimations or underestimations of coffee intake and an inability to capture nuances of coffee drinking as daily differences may arise. These non-standardised measures create an overall lack of accuracy when trying to associate coffee intake to outcomes. Future studies should endeavour to use the same measure to capture coffee intake and perhaps develop a coffee intake questionnaire that captures multiple aspects of coffee intake such as daily and weekly volume, additions, caffeination and brew type. One observational study explored food patterns and associated dietary patterns higher in coffee with poorer cognitive function; however, coffee was not investigated on its own(Reference Kim, Yu, Choi, Nam, Kim, Oh and Yang60). As mentioned earlier in this review, coffee’s neuroprotective effects may be influenced by other dietary factors and thus would be worthwhile to explore individually and in combination with other foods or additions. Moreover, other coffee compounds such as trigonelline, cafestol, theobromine and kahweol will also be important factors to consider as these have been found to exert effects including decreasing inflammation and influencing lipid metabolism(Reference Romualdo, Rocha, Vinken, Cogliati, Moreno, Chaves and Barbisan82,Reference Carneiro, Oliveira and Alves83) . Theobromine in particular has been associated with protective cognitive effects in older adults, possibly by inhibiting amyloid-β production pathway(Reference Carneiro, Oliveira and Alves83,Reference Zhao, Zhan, Wang, Song, Han and Shen84) . Only two observational studies and two RCT differentiated between caffeinated and decaffeinated coffee intake, which is an important point for future studies to consider in order to better understand the effects of non-caffeine components in coffee(Reference Camfield, Silber, Scholey, Nolidin, Goh and Stough9,Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10,Reference Arab, Khan and Lam43,Reference Dong, Li, Sun, Li and Zhang61) . Differentiating caffeination status in coffees can be especially helpful for large-scale studies where testing for CGA content may be impractical.

A strength of this review is the use of random-effects models to include a range of studies. Moreover, this review used high-quality studies with human participants and standardised measures of cognition. This is one of the first systematic reviews and meta-analyses to explore the cognitive impacts of CGA derived specifically from coffee. The findings of this review indicate that more research is needed to explore the relationship between CGA from coffee and cognitive function. Future studies could explore coffee as a whole food to consider the synergistic effects of the food matrix as only one RCT compared coffees with varying levels of CGA(Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10). As mentioned at the start of this review, considering a coffee’s country of origin may also help predict CGA content, and should be a point for future studies to consider. Recent studies exploring the effects of origin have found country of origin to be an important factor affecting coffee’s polyphenol content, with some studies even finding differences based on different regions of a single country(Reference Alnsour, Issa, Awwad, Albals and Al-Momani85Reference Mehari, Chandravanshi, Redi-Abshiro, Combrinck, McCrindle and Atlabachew87). Future studies should also control for caffeine. Only one RCT(Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10) and two observational studies(Reference Farah and de Paula Lima44,Reference Dong, Li, Sun, Li and Zhang61) considered caffeine, and as discussed earlier, the effects of CGA may be influenced by other compounds in coffee. Currently, no good-quality studies exist that control for caffeine. Only two RCT used CGA sourced from decaffeinated coffee beans(Reference Ochiai, Saitou, Suzukamo, Osaki and Asada7,Reference Saitou, Ochiai, Kozuma, Sato, Koikeda, Osaki and Katsuragi8) , but this was not studied against CGA sources from caffeinated coffee beans. Only one RCT compared caffeinated versus decaffeinated coffees with varying levels of CGA in their intervention(Reference Cropley, Croft, Silber, Neale, Scholey, Stough and Schmitt10). Exploring the effects of caffeinated and decaffeinated coffees may help increase understanding about possible interactions of caffeine and CGA and the impact on cognitive function.

Conclusions

In summary, the evidence from the meta-analysis does not provide support for coffee-derived CGA benefitting cognition. However, the evidence from the wider systematic review provides some support for a benefit of CGA-rich coffee on cognition in the domains of memory, attention, executive function, alertness and motor activity, and also may decrease the risk of cognitive impairment, dementia and Alzheimer’s. Sex differences may exist, with observational studies finding more benefits for women who drink coffee, particularly for memory. Long-term intake of coffee may be needed to see clear effects of coffee CGA as indicated by two RCT that provided CGA for either 12 or 16 weeks, and the observational studies looking at coffee intake over many years. Further RCT studies are warranted to explore the effects of CGA intake duration on cognitive function, as well as dose–response effects. Considering differences in CGA from unroasted, green coffee and roasted coffee while controlling for caffeine could also be useful to investigate.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954422424000209.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interests

All authors declare no financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. J.A.L. is Deputy Chair of the UK Government Scientific Advisory Committee for Nutrition.

Author contributions

All authors contributed to this manuscript. K.J., D.L., D.J. and J.A.L. contributed to conception, design and revisions. All authors contributed to analysis and interpretation of data, as well as reviewing, editing and approving the final version to be published.

References

Grosso, G (2018) Effects of polyphenol-rich foods on human health. Nutrients 10(8), 1089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cory, H, Passarelli, S, Szeto, J, Tamez, M, Mattei, J (2018) The role of polyphenols in human health and food systems: a mini-review. Front Nutr 5, 87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russo, GL, Spagnuolo, C, Russo, M, Tedesco, I, Moccia, S, Cervellera, C (2020) Mechanisms of aging and potential role of selected polyphenols in extending healthspan. Biochem Pharmacol 173, 113719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abbas, MN, Kausar, S, Cui, H (2020) Therapeutic potential of natural products in glioblastoma treatment: targeting key glioblastoma signaling pathways and epigenetic alterations. Clin Transl Oncol 22(7), 963977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leri, M, Scuto, M, Ontario, ML, Calabrese, V, Calabrese, EJ, Bucciantini, M, Stefani, M (2020) Healthy effects of plant polyphenols: molecular mechanisms. Int J Mol Sci 21(4), 1250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, PA, Wightman, EL, Veasey, R, et al. (2020) A randomized, crossover study of the acute cognitive and cerebral blood flow effects of phenolic, nitrate and botanical beverages in young, healthy humans. Nutrients 12(8), 2254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ochiai, R, Saitou, K, Suzukamo, C, Osaki, N, Asada, T (2019) Effect of chlorogenic acids on cognitive function in mild cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled crossover trial. J Alzheimer’s Dis 72(4), 12091216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saitou, K, Ochiai, R, Kozuma, K, Sato, H, Koikeda, T, Osaki, N, Katsuragi, Y (2018) Effect of chlorogenic acids on cognitive function: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Nutrients 10(10), 1337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camfield, DA, Silber, BY, Scholey, AB, Nolidin, K, Goh, A, Stough, C (2013) A randomised placebo-controlled trial to differentiate the acute cognitive and mood effects of chlorogenic acid from decaffeinated coffee. PLoS One 8(12), e82897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cropley, V, Croft, R, Silber, B, Neale, C, Scholey, A, Stough, C, Schmitt, J (2012) Does coffee enriched with chlorogenic acids improve mood and cognition after acute administration in healthy elderly? A pilot study. Psychopharmacology 219, 737749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eskelinen, MH, Ngandu, T, Tuomilehto, J, Soininen, H, Kivipelto, M (2009) Midlife coffee and tea drinking and the risk of late-life dementia: a population-based CAIDE study. J Alzheimer’s Dis 16(1), 8591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chuang, SY, Lo, YL, Wu, SY, Wang, PN, Pan, WH (2019) Dietary patterns and foods associated with cognitive function in Taiwanese older adults: the cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. J Am Med Directors Assoc 20(5), 544550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, K, Melo van Lent, D, Wolfsgruber, S, et al. (2018) Prospective associations between single foods, Alzheimer’s dementia and memory decline in the elderly. Nutrients 10(7), 852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noguchi-Shinohara, M, Yuki, S, Dohmoto, C, et al. (2014) Consumption of green tea, but not black tea or coffee, is associated with reduced risk of cognitive decline. PLoS One 9(5), e96013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, WC, Huang, YC, Lee, MS, Chang, HY, Doong, JY (2021) Frailty severity and cognitive impairment associated with dietary diversity in older adults in Taiwan. Nutrients 13(2), 418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zupo, R, Griseta, C, Battista, P, et al. (2022) Role of plant-based diet in late-life cognitive decline: results from the Salus in Apulia Study. Nutr Neurosci 25(6), 13001309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ng, TP, Zhong, X, Gao, Q, Gwee, X, Chua, DQ, Larbi, A (2020) Socio-environmental, lifestyle, behavioural, and psychological determinants of biological ageing: the Singapore longitudinal ageing study. Gerontology 66(6), 603613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, CE, Oruna-Concha, MJ, Mottram, DS, Gibson, GR, Spencer, JP (2013) The effect of processing on chlorogenic acid content of commercially available coffee. Food Chem 141(4), 33353340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamiyama, M, Moon, JK, Jang, HW, Shibamoto, T (2015) Role of degradation products of chlorogenic acid in the antioxidant activity of roasted coffee. J Agric Food Chem 63(7), 19962005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bobková, A, Hudáček, M, Jakabová, S et al. (2020) The effect of roasting on the total polyphenols and antioxidant activity of coffee. J Environ Sci Health Part B 55(5), 495500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dybkowska, E, Sadowska, A, Rakowska, R, Debowska, M, Swiderski, F, Swiader, K (2017) Assessing polyphenols content and antioxidant activity in coffee beans according to origin and the degree of roasting. Roczniki Państwowego Zakładu Higieny 68(4), 347353.Google Scholar
Zanin, RC, Corso, MP, Kitzberger, CS, dos Santos Scholz, MB, de Toledo Benassi, M (2016) Good cup quality roasted coffees show wide variation in chlorogenic acids content. LWT 74, 480483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Król, K, Gantner, M, Tatarak, A, Hallmann, E (2020) The content of polyphenols in coffee beans as roasting, origin and storage effect. Eur Food Res Technol 246, 3339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laukaleja, I & Kruma, Z (2019) Influence of the roasting process on bioactive compounds and aroma profile in specialty coffee: a review. In Proceedings of the Baltic Conference on Food Science and Technology and North and East European Congress on Food, Jelgava, Latvia, pp. 23.Google Scholar
Bolka, M & Emire, S (2020) Effects of coffee roasting technologies on cup quality and bioactive compounds of specialty coffee beans. Food Sci Nutr 8(11), 61206130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lingle, TR & Menon, SN (2017) Cupping and grading—Discovering character and quality. In The craft and science of coffee, pp. 181203. Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elejalde, E, Villarán, MC, Alonso, RM (2021) Grape polyphenols supplementation for exercise-induced oxidative stress. J Int Soc Sports Nutr 18(1), 3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isac-Torrente, L, Fernandez-Gomez, B, Miguel, M (2020) Coffee capsules: implications in antioxidant activity, bioactive compounds, and aluminum content. Eur Food Res Technol 246, 23352347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miłek, M, Młodecki, Ł, Dżugan, M (2021) Caffeine content and antioxidant activity of various brews of specialty grade coffee. Acta Sci Polonorum Technol Aliment 20(2), 179188.Google ScholarPubMed
Onakpoya, IJ, Spencer, EA, Thompson, MJ, Heneghan, CJ (2015) The effect of chlorogenic acid on blood pressure: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized clinical trials. J Human Hypertens 29(2), 7781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosso, H, Barbalho, SM, de Alvares Goulart, R, Otoboni, AM (2023) Green coffee: economic relevance and a systematic review of the effects on human health. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr 63(3), 394410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajik, N, Tajik, M, Mack, I, Enck, P (2017) The potential effects of chlorogenic acid, the main phenolic components in coffee, on health: a comprehensive review of the literature. Eur J Nutr 56, 22152244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahrens, MJ & Thompson, DL (2013) Effect of emulin on blood glucose in type 2 diabetics. J Med Food 16(3), 211215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Santana-Gálvez, J, Cisneros-Zevallos, L, Jacobo-Velázquez, DA (2017) Chlorogenic acid: recent advances on its dual role as a food additive and a nutraceutical against metabolic syndrome. Molecules 22(3), 358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ishida, K, Yamamoto, M, Misawa, K, Nishimura, H, Misawa, K, Ota, N, Shimotoyodome, A. (2020) Coffee polyphenols prevent cognitive dysfunction and suppress amyloid β plaques in APP/PS2 transgenic mouse. Neurosci Res 154, 3544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wierzejska, R (2017) Can coffee consumption lower the risk of Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease? A literature review. Arch Med Sci 13(3), 507514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dostal, V, Roberts, CM, Link, CD (2010) Genetic mechanisms of coffee extract protection in a Caenorhabditis elegans model of β-amyloid peptide toxicity. Genetics 186(3), 857866.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fukutomi, R, Ohishi, T, Koyama, Y, Pervin, M, Nakamura, Y, Isemura, M (2021) Beneficial effects of epigallocatechin-3-O-gallate, chlorogenic acid, resveratrol, and curcumin on neurodegenerative diseases. Molecules 26(2), 415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Upadhyay, R, Ramalakshmi, K, Rao, LJ (2012) Microwave-assisted extraction of chlorogenic acids from green coffee beans. Food Chem 130(1), 184188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heitman, E & Ingram, DK (2017) Cognitive and neuroprotective effects of chlorogenic acid. Nutr Neurosci 20(1), 3239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shukitt-Hale, B, Miller, MG, Chu, YF, Lyle, BJ, Joseph, JA (2013) Coffee, but not caffeine, has positive effects on cognition and psychomotor behavior in aging. Age 35, 21832192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, J, Lee, S, Shim, J, et al. (2012) Caffeinated coffee, decaffeinated coffee, and the phenolic phytochemical chlorogenic acid up-regulate NQO1 expression and prevent H2O2-induced apoptosis in primary cortical neurons. Neurochem Int 60(5), 466474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arab, L, Khan, F, Lam, H (2013) Epidemiologic evidence of a relationship between tea, coffee, or caffeine consumption and cognitive decline. Adv. Nutr 4(1), 115122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farah, A & de Paula Lima, J (2019) Consumption of chlorogenic acids through coffee and health implications. Beverages 5(1), 11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monroe, T & Carter, M (2012) Using the Folstein Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE) to explore methodological issues in cognitive aging research. Eur J Ageing 9, 265274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, JP & Green, S. editors. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions.Google Scholar
Montgomery, SA, Thal, LJ, Amrein, R (2003) Meta-analysis of double blind randomized controlled clinical trials of acetyl-l-carnitine versus placebo in the treatment of mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 18(2), 6171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiu, S, Sievenpiper, JL, De Souza, RJ, et al. (2014) Effect of fructose on markers of non-alcoholic fatty liver disease (NAFLD): a systematic review and meta-analysis of controlled feeding trials. Eur J Clin Nutr 68(4), 416423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garg, A (1998) High-monounsaturated-fat diets for patients with diabetes mellitus: a meta-analysis. Am. J Clin Nutr 67(3), 577S582S.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keith, TZ & Reynolds, MR (2010) Cattell–Horn–Carroll abilities and cognitive tests: what we’ve learned from 20 years of research. Psychol Schools 47(7), 635650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, WJ & McGrew, KS (2018) The Cattell-Horn-Carroll theory of cognitive abilities. In Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues, pp. 73163.Google Scholar
Jewsbury, PA, Bowden, SC, Duff, K (2017) The Cattell–Horn–Carroll model of cognition for clinical assessment. J Psychoeducational Assess 35(6), 547567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, PA, Haskell-Ramsay, C, Forster, J, et al. (2022) Acute cognitive performance and mood effects of coffee berry and apple extracts: a randomised, double blind, placebo controlled crossover study in healthy humans. Nutr Neurosci 25(11), 23352343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cho, BH, Choi, SM, Kim, JT, Kim, BC (2018) Association of coffee consumption and non-motor symptoms in drug-naïve, early-stage Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Related Disord 50, 4247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Araújo, LF, Mirza, SS, Bos, D, et al. (2016) Association of coffee consumption with MRI markers and cognitive function: a population-based study. J Alzheimer’s Dis 53(2), 451461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson-Kozlow, M, Kritz-Silverstein, D, Barrett-Connor, E, Morton, D (2002) Coffee consumption and cognitive function among older adults. Am J Epidemiol 156(9), 842850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardener, SL, Rainey-Smith, SR, Villemagne, VL, et al. (2021) Higher coffee consumption is associated with slower cognitive decline and less cerebral Aβ-amyloid accumulation over 126 months: data from the Australian imaging, biomarkers, and lifestyle study. Front Aging Neurosci 13, 681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valls-Pedret, C, Lamuela-Raventós, RM, Medina-Remon, A, et al. (2012) Polyphenol-rich foods in the Mediterranean diet are associated with better cognitive function in elderly subjects at high cardiovascular risk. J Alzheimer’s Dis 29(4), 773782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bragança, M, Marinho, M, Marques, J, Moreira, R, Palha, A, Marques-Teixeira, J, Esteves, M. (2016) The influence of espresso coffee on neurocognitive function in HIV-infected patients. AIDS Care 28(9), 11491153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J, Yu, A, Choi, BY, Nam, JH, Kim, MK, Oh, DH, Yang, YJ (2015) Dietary patterns derived by cluster analysis are associated with cognitive function among Korean older adults. Nutrients 7(6), 41544169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dong, X, Li, S, Sun, J, Li, Y, Zhang, D (2020) Association of coffee, decaffeinated coffee and caffeine intake from coffee with cognitive performance in older adults: national health and nutrition examination survey (NHANES) 2011–2014. Nutrients 12(3), 840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heggy, A, Masoumi, A, Al-Yafei, M, Al-Bader, F, Al-Abdi, T, Shi, Z (2021) Habitual tea and coffee consumption and mean reaction time among Qatari adults. J Alzheimer’s Dis 84(2), 767776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torres, SJ, Lautenschlager, NT, Wattanapenpaiboon, N, Greenop, KR, Beer, C, Flicker, L, Alfonso, H, Nowson, CA (2012) Dietary patterns are associated with cognition among older people with mild cognitive impairment. Nutrients 4(11), 15421551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sievenpiper, JL & de Souza, RJ (2013) Are sugar-sweetened beverages the whole story?. Am J Clin Nutr 98(2), 261263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muñoz-García, MI, Martínez-González, MA, Martín-Moreno, JM, Razquin, C, Cervantes, S, Guillén-Grima, F, Toledo, E (2020) Sugar-sweetened and artificially-sweetened beverages and changes in cognitive function in the SUN project. Nutr Neurosci 23(12), 946954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duarte, GS & Farah, A (2011) Effect of simultaneous consumption of milk and coffee on chlorogenic acids’ bioavailability in humans. J Agric Food Chem 59(14), 79257931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rashidinejad, A, Tarhan, O, Rezaei, A, et al. (2022) Addition of milk to coffee beverages; the effect on functional, nutritional, and sensorial properties. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr 62(22), 61326152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Socała, K, Szopa, A, Serefko, A, Poleszak, E, Wlaź, P (2020) Neuroprotective effects of coffee bioactive compounds: a review. Int J Mol Sci 22(1), 107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iranpour, S, Saadati, HM, Koohi, F, Sabour, S (2020) Association between caffeine intake and cognitive function in adults; effect modification by sex: data from National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2013–2014. Clin Nutr 39(7), 21582168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Draheim, C, Mashburn, CA, Martin, JD, Engle, RW (2019) Reaction time in differential and developmental research: a review and commentary on the problems and alternatives. Psychol Bull 145(5), 508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sreenivasan, S & Kusumadevi, MS (2020) Effect of duration of disease and glycemic control on attention, executive function and visual reaction time in type 2 diabetes mellitus patients of Bangalore. CHAIRMAN, EDITORIAL BOARD 8(1), 106.Google Scholar
Gurvich, C, Hoy, K, Thomas, N, Kulkarni, J (2018) Sex differences and the influence of sex hormones on cognition through adulthood and the aging process. Brain Sci 8(9), 163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kane, MO, Anselm, E, Rattmann, YD, Auger, C, Schini-Kerth, VB (2009) Role of gender and estrogen receptors in the rat aorta endothelium-dependent relaxation to red wine polyphenols. Vasc Pharmacol 51(2-3), 140146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campesi, I, Marino, M, Cipolletti, M, Romani, A, Franconi, F (2018) Put “gender glasses” on the effects of phenolic compounds on cardiovascular function and diseases. Eur J Nutr 57, 26772691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, L, Su, C, Chen, X, et al. (2020) Chlorogenic acids in cardiovascular disease: a review of dietary consumption, pharmacology, and pharmacokinetics. J Agric Food Chem 68(24), 64646484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeng, L, Xiang, R, Fu, C, Qu, Z, Liu, C (2022) The regulatory effect of chlorogenic acid on gut-brain function and its mechanism: a systematic review. Biomed Pharmacother 149, 112831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Hippel, PT (2015) The heterogeneity statistic I2 can be biased in small meta-analyses. BMC Med Res Methodol 15(1), 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, H, Gu, J, Amrit, BK, Nawaz, MA, Barrow, CJ, Dunshea, FR, Suleria, HA (2022) Effect of processing on bioaccessibility and bioavailability of bioactive compounds in coffee beans. Food Biosci 46, 101373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Upadhyay, R & Mohan Rao, LJ (2013) An outlook on chlorogenic acids—occurrence, chemistry, technology, and biological activities. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr 53(9), 968984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renouf, M, Marmet, C, Giuffrida, F, Lepage, M, Barron, D, Beaumont, M, Williamson, G, Dionisi, F. (2014) Dose–response plasma appearance of coffee chlorogenic and phenolic acids in adults. Mol Nutr Food Res 58(2), 301309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamaguchi, T, Chikama, A, Mori, K, Watanabe, T, Shioya, Y, Katsuragi, Y, Tokimitsu, I. (2008) Hydroxyhydroquinone-free coffee: a double-blind, randomized controlled dose–response study of blood pressure. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis 18(6), 408414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romualdo, GR, Rocha, AB, Vinken, M, Cogliati, B, Moreno, FS, Chaves, MA, Barbisan, LF. (2019) Drinking for protection? Epidemiological and experimental evidence on the beneficial effects of coffee or major coffee compounds against gastrointestinal and liver carcinogenesis. Food Res Int 123, 567589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carneiro, SM, Oliveira, MB, Alves, RC (2021) Neuroprotective properties of coffee: an update. Trends Food Sci Technol 113, 167179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhao, L, Zhan, R, Wang, X, Song, R, Han, M, Shen, X (2023) Correlation between dietary theobromine intake and low cognitive performance in older adults in the United States: a cross-sectional study based on the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 32(1), 120132.Google ScholarPubMed
Alnsour, L, Issa, R, Awwad, S, Albals, D, Al-Momani, I (2022) Quantification of total phenols and antioxidants in coffee samples of different origins and evaluation of the effect of degree of roasting on their levels. Molecules 27(5), 1591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Król, K, Gantner, M, Tatarak, A, Hallmann, E (2020) The content of polyphenols in coffee beans as roasting, origin and storage effect. Eur Food Res Technol 246, 3339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehari, B, Chandravanshi, BS, Redi-Abshiro, M, Combrinck, S, McCrindle, R, Atlabachew, M (2021) Polyphenol contents of green coffee beans from different regions of Ethiopia. Int J Food Prop 24(1), 1727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow diagram for identifying studies for inclusion.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Random-effects DerSimonian–Laird model.

Figure 2

Table 1. Characteristics of randomised controlled trials (RCT)

Figure 3

Table 2. Characteristics of observational studies

Supplementary material: File

Johal et al. supplementary material

Johal et al. supplementary material
Download Johal et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.2 MB