Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T07:32:16.441Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Out-foxing the red fox: how best to protect the nests of the Endangered loggerhead marine turtle Caretta caretta from mammalian predation?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 December 2011

David J. Kurz*
Affiliation:
Princeton University, Frist Campus Center, Box 1219, Princeton, New Jersey 08544, USA.
Katherine M. Straley
Affiliation:
Ithaca College, Ithaca, New York, USA
Brett A. DeGregorio
Affiliation:
Savannah River Ecology Laboratory, Aiken, South Carolina, USA
*
*Princeton University, Frist Campus Center, Box 1219, Princeton, New Jersey 08544, USA. E-mail dave.kurz@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Recovery plans for the Endangered loggerhead marine turtle Caretta caretta cite mammalian predation as a major threat, and recommend nest protection efforts, already present at many rookery beaches, to protect eggs and hatchlings. Nest protection techniques vary but wire box cages and plastic mesh screens are two common tools used to deter predation by a host of beach-foraging, opportunistic mammalian predators. We empirically tested the efficacy of wire cages and plastic mesh screens in preventing red fox Vulpes vulpes predation on artificial nests. Both techniques averted fox predation (0%), whereas unprotected control nests suffered 33% predation under conditions of normal predator motivation, or a level of motivation stimulated by loggerhead turtle egg scent. However, in side-by-side comparisons under conditions of presumed high predator motivation, 25% of mesh screens were breached whereas no cage-protected nests were successfully predated. In addition to effectiveness at preventing predation, factors such as cost, ease of use, deployment time, and magnetic disturbance were evaluated. Our study suggests that the efficacy of plastic screens and the potential disadvantages associated with galvanized wire should influence selection of mechanical barriers on beaches where fox predation threatens loggerhead nests.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Fauna & Flora International 2011

Introduction

Predation by mammals is perhaps the most significant biotic threat to the hatching success of the loggerhead marine turtle Caretta caretta. The devastating impact of predators such as raccoons Procyon lotor, red foxes Vulpes vulpes, feral pigs Sus scrofa, coyotes Canis latrans and armadillos Dasypus novemcinctus on loggerhead turtle nests is well-documented (Stancyk, Reference Stancyk and Bjorndal1982; Erk’akan, Reference Erk’akan1993; Mroziak et al., Reference Mroziak, Salmon and Rusenko2000). On some beaches predation by mammals has partially or fully destroyed up to 97% of nests annually (Hopkins & Murphy, Reference Hopkins and Murphy1980; Talbert et al., Reference Talbert, Stancyk, Dean and Will1980; Schroeder, Reference Schroeder1981). Although population models for loggerhead turtles show that eggs and hatchlings are generally the least-responsive life stage to decreases in mortality, they also show that significant decreases (e.g. 50%) in first-year survival cause populations to decline more swiftly (Crouse et al., Reference Crouse, Crowder and Caswell1987; Crowder et al., Reference Crowder, Crouse, Heppell and Martin1994). Furthermore, all life stages of threatened or declining populations merit consideration for conservation efforts (Marchand & Litvaitis, Reference Marchand and Litvaitis2004). The reduction of nest predation is also one of 13 goals cited in the Northwest Atlantic Loggerhead Sea Turtle Recovery Plan, with the specific target of reducing the annual rate of mammalian predation on nests to < 10% (NMFS & USFWS, 2008).

Several mechanical nest protection methods have been proposed: wire cages (Addison & Henricy, Reference Addison and Henricy1994; Jordan, Reference Jordan, Bjorndal, Bolten, Johnson and Eliazar1994; Ratnaswamy et al., Reference Ratnaswamy, Warren, Kramer and Adam1997), wire screens (Yerli et al., Reference Yerli, Canbolat, Brown and Macdonald1997), flags (Longo et al., Reference Longo, Pazeto, de Abreu and Floeter2009), and relocation of nests to fenced hatcheries (Stancyk et al., Reference Stancyk, Talbert and Dean1980; Talbert et al., Reference Talbert, Stancyk, Dean and Will1980). Each method has proved successful in preventing nest predation in relation to unguarded nests. However, some of these methods may be site-specific (e.g. flags require windy beaches) or cost and labour prohibitive (e.g. relocation to hatcheries). Cages and screens are often the most popular choices in the south-eastern USA. In 2002 > 90% of loggerhead turtle nests in North Carolina and Georgia were protected with a plastic or wire cage or screen (NMFS & USFWS, 2008). However, the widespread use of galvanized metal screen or wire to protect loggerhead turtle nests is cause for concern, as recent research indicates that galvanized wire cages may disorient marine turtle hatchlings (Irwin et al., Reference Irwin, Horner and Lohmann2004). The potential risk posed by galvanized wire to the magnetic compass of hatchlings necessitates re-evaluation of this common management strategy. If other mechanical barriers prove as effective in protecting nests as wire cages, large-scale implementation of these new designs could eliminate concern about hatchling disorientation while still increasing annual hatchling emergence success.

Although wire cages have been successful in preventing predation (Addison & Henricy, Reference Addison and Henricy1994; Adamany et al., Reference Adamany, Salmon and Witherington1997; Ratnaswamy et al., Reference Ratnaswamy, Warren, Kramer and Adam1997) and have been implemented on many nesting beaches, no published study has empirically tested their effectiveness in comparison to plastic mesh screens. The objectives of our study were to (1) compare the rates of predation among unprotected nests and nests protected with wire cages or plastic mesh screens, (2) compare the effectiveness of wire cages and mesh screens in preventing predation when predator motivation was high because of attractive baiting, and (3) make management recommendations based on the efficacy, cost, ease of implementation and potential risk of hatchling disorientation for each of the competing methods.

Study area

Bald Head Island is a forested barrier island in south-eastern North Carolina at the confluence of the Cape Fear River and the Atlantic Ocean (Fig. 1). Available nesting habitat consists of 15.3 km of sandy beach on the west, south, and east sides of the island (Hawkes et al., Reference Hawkes, Broderick, Godfrey and Godley2005). Marine turtle monitoring and nest protection have been ongoing at Bald Head since 1983, with a mean of 89 ± SE 8.2 (range 36–198; 1983–2009) loggerhead turtle nests deposited annually. Galvanized metal cages (122 cm long × 61 cm wide × 61 cm high, with 30-cm flaps on each side) are deployed at each nest location immediately following egg deposition and nest covering. Nest predation on the island is predominantly by red foxes (BAD, pers. obs.).

Fig. 1 Bald Head Island, North Carolina, USA (map modified with permission from Hawkes et al., Reference Hawkes, Broderick, Godfrey and Godley2005). Approximately 15 km of beach on the west, south and east of Bald Head Island provide nesting habitat for loggerhead Caretta caretta marine turtles annually. Broken lines represent nesting beaches on Bald Head Island and nearby Oak Island (not included in our study). Bald Head Island is located at the confluence of the Cape Fear River and Atlantic Ocean. The rectangle on the inset indicates the location of the main map on the east coast of the USA.

Methods

Experiment 1

Two trials were conducted to compare fox predation on unprotected (control), cage-protected, and screen-protected artificial nests; chicken eggs were placed inside each artificial nest and covered with sand to mimic loggerhead turtle nests. We prepared six nests of each treatment and six control nests along a 4.25-km transect that ran 5 m from the edge of dune habitat (a typical nesting area for C. caretta on Bald Head Island). Wire cages were made from galvanized wire fencing (Red Brand Welded Wire Fence, Peoria, USA). Cages were 122 cm long × 61 cm wide × 61 cm high, with 30-cm flaps extending horizontally from the bottom in all four directions. Sides were fastened with 20-cm plastic cable ties and the cages were buried 30 cm into the sand.

Mesh screens were made of 5 × 5 cm mesh plastic fencing (Tenax Plastic Home Fence, Baltimore, USA). Two 2.44 × 1.22 m sections were cut and connected with 20-cm cable ties to make a square screen with side lengths of 2.44 m. Screens were centred over the nests, staked down using 12 15-cm wire stakes (16-gauge wire coil) per screen, and covered with 1–2 cm of sand both for aesthetic reasons and to avoid providing foxes with visual cues. Control nests were marked discreetly with a plain stake 5 m from the nest.

Based on our knowledge of fox foraging habits on Bald Head Island (BAD, pers. obs.), we spaced nests 250 m apart from one another to ensure that multiple beach-foraging foxes had access to at least one nest. We placed nests in a regular alternating pattern (cage, screen, unprotected) to avoid bias. The second trial, although identical in design, was conducted 8 km from the first, as individual foxes and family groups patrol distinct areas of beach (DJK, pers. obs.). In each nest, we buried five chicken eggs to a depth of 29 cm from ground level to the top of the eggs (Tiwari & Bjorndal, Reference Tiwari and Bjorndal2000) to replicate the distance from the surface to the top of the egg chamber in a real loggerhead turtle nest. This experiment was intended to incite fox predation efforts similar to those on loggerhead turtle eggs in the first few days after deposition.

We scented chicken eggs by pouring 15 ml of a dilute loggerhead egg yolk solution on the eggs as well as on top of the filled-in nest. The solution was prepared by vigorously mixing one loggerhead turtle egg yolk (already sacrificed for a genetics project; Shamblin, Reference Shamblin2007) with 300 ml of water. Nests were checked daily for signs of predator activity, and each nest was classified according to the following categories: predation (some or all eggs removed), attempted predation (visible effort exerted by predator to breach egg chamber), fox presence (footprints or scat present within 30 cm of cage or screen), or no interest (no visible signs of fox presence). Each day signs of fox activity were cleared. When substantial predation attempts disturbed the area near nests (e.g. via holes next to cages), we smoothed the sand to simulate the daily efforts of a real marine turtle nest protection programme. Every 2 days 15 ml of egg yolk mixture was re-applied to keep the scent fresh, and daily checks halted when attempted predation events ceased (6–7 days after the start of the trials). Differences in predator attendance, predation attempts, and successful predation events were compared between protected and unprotected nests, and then between caged and screen-protected nests. Because of the non-parametric nature of the data sets we used a 2 × 2 contingency table test. All trials took place during the summer of 2010.

Experiment 2

Two trials were conducted to compare empirically the performance of wire cages and mesh screens placed side by side under conditions of attractive baiting and therefore presumed high predator motivation. The two nest protection techniques were deployed in the same way as those in the first two trials; however, in this experiment we placed nests in pairs to ensure equal predation pressure. Thus, each cage-protected nest was placed 5 m from a mesh-protected nest, allowing the same individual foxes to attempt predation on each paired treatment. Four pairs of nests were scattered along 9 km of beach in areas with resident foxes (DJK, pers. obs.), with at least 1 km between each pair to increase the likelihood of independent sampling.

To test the effectiveness of each mechanical barrier we selected bait with a high probability of stimulating vigorous fox predation attempts. In the first trial, we used high-fat bacon and rotten eggs. We placed 0.25 kg of bacon and ten rotten chicken eggs in each nest. Bacon grease was used to scent the sand on top of the nest, and for re-scenting the top of the nest every 3 days.

The second trial was constructed the same way, using different areas of beach. For this trial we placed 0.35 kg of chicken breast and leftover bacon scraps (< 0.05 kg) in each nest. Chicken grease was used to scent the sand on top of the nest, and nests were re-scented after 3 days. Both trials were continued for a minimum of 5 days and were removed after 24 hours with no new predation attempts (each trial happened to run for 6 days). Because of the paired nature of the data we used McNemar’s test to compare the predation rates of foxes on caged and screen-protected nests.

Additional analysis

In addition to testing for protection success we evaluated the drawbacks of each technique in terms of installation time and the financial cost of each individual barrier. For all cages and screens (four of each) in the second trial of Experiment 2 we used a stopwatch to record the time required for two people to deploy the mechanical barrier while working at a moderate pace. Cost was assessed by summing the aggregate price of all materials required to make a cage or screen. Finally, we considered the qualitative features of each technique, including aesthetics, portability and potential disorientation of hatchlings.

Results

Experiment 1

During trials one and two of Experiment 1 predators were motivated by loggerhead turtle egg scent. No difference in predator attendance was detected between control and protected nests (χ2 = 1.04, P = 0.31), with fox attendance (footprints or scat) at all 12 (100%) control nests and 11 of 12 (92%) nests of each treatment (Table 1). Predation attempts were more successful at control nests (four of eight attempts) than at protected nests (χ2 = 5.0, P = 0.03), as attempted predation at four caged and four screen-covered nests was unsuccessful. Overall predation rate was higher for control nests than for protected nests (χ2 = 8.75, P = 0.003); however, no direct comparisons between caged and screen-protected nests were possible as each type of barrier deterred all predation attempts.

Table 1 Number of artificial marine turtle nests in Experiment 1 that red foxes Vulpes vulpes attended, attempted to predate, or successfully predated on Bald Head Island (Fig. 1). Each nest was assigned to one of 3 treatments: control (unprotected), protection with a wire cage surrounding the nest, or protection with a plastic mesh screen placed on top of the nest and buried under 1–2 cm of sand.

Experiment 2

In Experiment 2 predator motivation to breach nests was assumed to be high because of baiting and scenting with attractive lures. This assumption is supported by the more vigorous response of foxes in comparison to Experiment 1. Two of eight (25%) screen-protected nests were successfully predated whereas zero of eight caged nests were predated (Plate 1). Foxes predated screened nests by tearing through the plastic mesh and reaching the egg chamber from above. Foxes attended every nest in each trial, regardless of treatment type. Six predation attempts were made on caged nests, and predation was attempted on all eight screen-protected nests. Successful predation rates at caged vs screen-protected nests were not significantly different (χ2 = 2.50, P = 0.11).

Plate 1 (a) An unsuccessful predation attempt on a wire box cage-protected artificial nest (the fox, or foxes, attempted to dig at the nest from all sides of the cage but was/were unable to reach the egg chamber), and (b) a successful predation event by one or more red foxes on a screen-protected artificial nest.

Assessment of additional costs

Plastic screens could be deployed substantially faster than wire cages: on average, screens took 48% less time to install than cages under the same conditions. The cost per unit difference was less dramatic, but a screen still cost slightly less than a cage. A summary of the advantages and weaknesses of each nest protection measure is presented in Table 2.

Table 2 Relative strengths and weaknesses of two mechanical barriers (galvanized wire cage and plastic mesh screen) to red fox predation of artificial loggerhead marine turtle nests (++ and – – indicate qualitative assessments).

Discussion

Both wire box cages and plastic mesh screens proved effective at preventing fox predation of artificial loggerhead nests. Although we did not compare mechanical barriers using genuine loggerhead turtle nests, our findings seem to parallel high levels of fox and raccoon predation documented on real, unprotected loggerhead turtle nests (Stancyk et al., Reference Stancyk, Talbert and Dean1980; Davis & Whiting, Reference Davis and Whiting1997; Yerli et al., Reference Yerli, Canbolat, Brown and Macdonald1997) and protection rates of 96–100% of nests guarded by metal cages or screens (Addison & Henricy, Reference Addison and Henricy1994; Jordan, Reference Jordan, Bjorndal, Bolten, Johnson and Eliazar1994; Yerli et al., Reference Yerli, Canbolat, Brown and Macdonald1997). However, our study aimed to prevent fox predation specifically and may not be applicable to beaches with other mammalian predators. On some beaches nest caging may condition raccoons to the presence of a nest, making mechanical barriers more likely to attract predators than an uncaged nest (Mroziak et al., Reference Mroziak, Salmon and Rusenko2000).

Results from our second experiment may imply that wire box cages are more effective than plastic mesh screens at protecting artificial marine turtle nests under conditions of high predator motivation, although the difference was not statistically significant. For this experiment we used bait assumed to be attractive to foxes to test the strength of each type of mechanical barrier. This comparison was important, as stochasticity in predator population, prey availability and environmental conditions inevitably create conditions of food scarcity and thus high-effort predation attempts (BAD, pers. obs.). In addition to the quantitative differences in protection success (100% for cages, 75% for screens) our qualitative observations indicated that foxes may expend less energy penetrating screens than tunnelling under cages. Further research into nest protection materials could support use of a stronger mesh screen than the type we used.

Because our results show that mechanical barriers are effective at reducing predation of artificial loggerhead turtle nests, we recommend that efforts be made to protect (using an effective screen or cage) loggerhead turtle nests on beaches where fox predation is a threat. For example, foxes were by far the most destructive mammalian predator of marine turtle eggs in North Carolina in 2010 (M. Godfrey, pers. comm.) and are the dominant predator on some Mediterranean beaches (Erk’akan, Reference Erk’akan1993; Yerli et al., Reference Yerli, Canbolat, Brown and Macdonald1997). The scope of our study does not allow us to suggest how other mammalian predators would react to wire cages or plastic screens but it is possible that the implementation of mechanical barriers would also be efficacious against other predators. Despite the demonstrated effectiveness of mechanical barriers in protecting loggerhead nests from foxes and raccoons (Jordan, Reference Jordan, Bjorndal, Bolten, Johnson and Eliazar1994; Ratnaswamy et al., Reference Ratnaswamy, Warren, Kramer and Adam1997; Yerli et al., Reference Yerli, Canbolat, Brown and Macdonald1997), in some parts of the south-east USA < 50% of loggerhead turtle nests are protected with cages or screens (NMFS & USFWS, 2008). Documented mammalian depredation rates of up to 97% of unprotected nests suggest that nest protection is an essential component of long-term loggerhead conservation (Davis & Whiting, Reference Davis and Whiting1977; Talbert et al., Reference Talbert, Stancyk, Dean and Will1980; Schroeder, Reference Schroeder1981).

Although wire cages are the most established option for nest protection (Addison & Henricy, Reference Addison and Henricy1994; Ratnaswamy et al., Reference Ratnaswamy, Warren, Kramer and Adam1997; Kinsella et al., Reference Kinsella, Godley, Broderick, Furness, Epperly and Braun1998), many programmes may lack the resources necessary to purchase and deploy hundreds or thousands of cages. Our mesh screens took 48% less time to deploy and cost c. 15% less than cages. Furthermore, because no foxes attempted to predate the screen-protected nests by tunnelling from the side, we think that reducing the size of screens to 1.8 × 1.8 m could be an equally effective and substantially cheaper way to prevent red fox predation. Using the same materials this version would only cost c. USD 5.80, which is c. 47% less than the cost of our wire box cages. That plastic mesh screens are cheaper to construct and faster to deploy suggests that mesh screens may be particularly practical for beaches that support large numbers of turtle nests each year and suffer from fox predation. However, management measures must also be beach-specific, taking into account predation threats, resources for protection, and nesting turtle densities. Use of mechanical barriers may not be appropriate on beaches with high-density nesting because the presence of cages or screens could deter nesting females.

Marine turtle hatchlings use the earth’s magnetic field to orient and are thought to begin using this ability as they swim offshore following hatching (Lohmann, Reference Lohmann1991; Lohmann & Lohmann, Reference Lohmann and Lohmann1998; Irwin & Lohmann, Reference Irwin and Lohmann2003). As a result, galvanized wire poses a possible risk to the navigational abilities of hatchlings. Irwin et al. Reference Irwin, Horner and Lohmann(2004) found that galvanized wire cages alter magnetic intensity in the area of the egg chamber by 5–26% and field inclination by 4–20% (however, there is no experimental evidence demonstrating whether an altered magnetic field is detrimental to turtle hatchlings). While the extent to which marine turtles may be harmed remains unknown, an altered magnetic environment during development could negatively affect hatchlings: (1) as they migrate post-hatching; (2) as they respond to regional magnetic markers throughout their lives; and (3) as they attempt to relocate their natal beaches as adults (Irwin et al., Reference Irwin, Horner and Lohmann2004). Although these possible effects have not yet been studied, alternatives to galvanized wire should be sought until wire cages and screens can be exonerated from potentially disorienting effects on developing hatchlings.

Little in the literature points to a nest protection measure that is effective, affordable and free of the potential dangers associated with galvanized wire. Although other studies have independently assessed the performance of wire cages (Addison & Henricy, Reference Addison and Henricy1994; Jordan, Reference Jordan, Bjorndal, Bolten, Johnson and Eliazar1994) and wire screens (Yerli et al., Reference Yerli, Canbolat, Brown and Macdonald1997), to the best of our knowledge our study is the first to use artificial nests to compare the efficacy of two mechanical barriers to marine turtle nest predation. There is probably no single solution appropriate for reducing mammalian predation of marine turtle nests on every beach. Rather, our study suggests that continued research will help identify multiple potent mechanical barriers to nest predation, each suited to distinct conservation needs.

Acknowledgements

We thank the Bald Head Island Conservancy and its staff for providing materials and support, and Turtle Central and the community of Bald Head Island for encouragement and generous donations. Mr and Mrs Roth in particular were kind donors. We also appreciate the donation of project materials from Wal-Mart Stores, Inc., and the Maritime Market. Matthew Godfrey, Tom Hancock, Amanda Southwood, Nathan Gregory, Peter and Corriene Kurz, Chris Kelly, and an anonymous reviewer provided valuable advice and guidance. Thanks to William Cochran, Mark Accomando Jr, Lauren Adams and Lauren Marks for help in the field.

Biographical sketches

David J. Kurz hopes to become involved with captive breeding and release efforts to save threatened animals on the brink of extinction. Katherine M. Straley is interested in avian conservation and avian wildlife rehabilitation, especially of shorebirds and waterfowl. Brett A. DeGregorio’s research interests focus on the conservation of reptiles and their habitats.

References

Adamany, S., Salmon, M. & Witherington, B.E. (1997) Behavior of sea turtles at an urban beach III. Costs and benefits of nest caging as a management strategy. Florida Scientist, 60, 239254.Google Scholar
Addison, D.S. & Henricy, S. (1994) A comparison of galvanized wire mesh cages vs. flat chain-link screen in preventing Procyon lotor depredation of Caretta caretta nests. NOAA Technical Memorandum, 351, 174.Google Scholar
Crouse, D.T., Crowder, L.B. & Caswell, H. (1987) A stage-based population model for loggerhead sea turtles and implications for conservation. Ecology, 68, 14121423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowder, L.B., Crouse, D.T., Heppell, S.S. & Martin, T.H. (1994) Predicting the impact of turtle excluder devices on loggerhead sea turtle populations. Ecological Applications, 4, 437445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, G.E. & Whiting, M.C. (1977) Loggerhead sea turtle nesting in Everglades National Park, Florida, U.S.A.. Herpetologica, 33, 1828.Google Scholar
Erk’akan, F. (1993) Nesting biology of loggerhead turtles Caretta caretta on Dalyan Beach, Mugla-Turkey. Biological Conservation, 66, 14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkes, L.A., Broderick, A.C., Godfrey, M.H. & Godley, B.J. (2005) Status of nesting loggerhead turtles Caretta caretta at Bald Head Island (North Carolina, USA) after 24 years of intensive monitoring and conservation. Oryx, 39, 6572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, S.R. & Murphy, T.M. (1980) Reproductive Ecology of Caretta caretta in South Carolina. South Carolina Wildlife and Marine Resources Department, South Carolina, USA.Google Scholar
Irwin, W.P. & Lohmann, K.J. (2003) Magnet-induced disorientation in hatchling loggerhead sea turtles. Journal of Experimental Biology, 206, 497501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irwin, W.P., Horner, A.J. & Lohmann, K.J. (2004) Magnetic field distortions produced by protective cages around sea turtle nests: unintended consequences for orientation and navigation? Biological Conservation, 118, 117120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordan, E.R. (1994) Effects of a nest screening program on raccoon predation of sea turtle eggs at Canaveral national seashore. In Proceedings of the 14th Annual Symposium on Sea Turtle Biology and Conservation (eds Bjorndal, K.A., Bolten, A.B., Johnson, D.A. & Eliazar, P.J.), pp. 6667. NOAA Technical Memorandum NMFS-SEFC-351, Miami, USA.Google Scholar
Kinsella, L.E., Godley, B.J., Broderick, A.C. & Furness, R.W. (1998) Use of chemical deterrents and wire screening to reduce the incidence of predation of marine turtle (Chelonia mydas and Caretta caretta) nests in northern Cyprus, eastern Mediterranean. In Proceedings of the 17th Annual Symposium on Sea Turtle Biology and Conservation (eds Epperly, S.P. & Braun, J.), pp. 213215. NOAA Technical Memorandum NMFS-SEFC-415, Miami, USA.Google Scholar
Lohmann, K.J. (1991) Magnetic orientation by hatchling loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta). Journal of Experimental Biology, 155, 3749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lohmann, K.J. & Lohmann, C.M.F. (1998) Migratory guidance mechanisms in marine turtles. Journal of Avian Biology, 29, 585596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Longo, G.O., Pazeto, F.D., de Abreu, J.A.G. & Floeter, S.R. (2009) Flags reduce sea turtle nest predation by foxes in NE Brazil. Marine Turtle Newsletter, 125, 13.Google Scholar
Marchand, M.N. & Litvaitis, J.A. (2004) Effects of landscape composition, habitat features, and nest distribution on predation rates of simulated turtle nests. Biological Conservation, 117, 243251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mroziak, M.L., Salmon, M. & Rusenko, K. (2000) Do wire cages protect sea turtles from foot traffic and mammalian predators? Chelonian Conservation and Biology, 3, 693698.Google Scholar
NMFS (National Marine Fisheries Service) & USFWS (U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service) (2008) Recovery Plan for the Northwest Atlantic Population of the Loggerhead Sea Turtle (Caretta caretta), 2nd Revision. National Marine Fisheries Service, Silver Spring, USA.Google Scholar
Ratnaswamy, M.J., Warren, R.J., Kramer, M.T. & Adam, M.D. (1997) Comparisons of lethal and non-lethal techniques to reduce raccoon depredation of sea turtle nests. Journal of Wildlife Management, 61, 368376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schroeder, B.A. (1981) Predation and nest success in two species of marine turtles (Caretta caretta and Chelonia mydas) at Merritt Island, Florida. Florida Scientist, 44, 35.Google Scholar
Shamblin, B.M. (2007) Tetranucleotide microsatellites from the loggerhead sea turtle (Caretta caretta). Molecular Ecology, 7, 784787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stancyk, S.E. (1982) Non-human predators of sea turtles and their control. In Biology and Conservation of Sea Turtles (ed. Bjorndal, K.A.), pp. 139152. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC, USA.Google Scholar
Stancyk, S.E., Talbert, O.R. & Dean, J.M. (1980) Nesting activity of the loggerhead turtle Caretta caretta in South Carolina II. Protection of nests from raccoon predation by transplantation. Biological Conservation, 18, 289298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talbert, O.R. Jr, Stancyk, S.E., Dean, J.M. & Will, J.M. (1980) Nesting activity of the loggerhead turtle (Caretta caretta) in South Carolina I: a rookery in transition. Copeia, 4, 709719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiwari, M. & Bjorndal, K.A. (2000) Variation in morphology and reproduction in loggerheads, Caretta caretta, nesting in the United States, Brazil, and Greece. Herpetologica, 56, 343356.Google Scholar
Yerli, S., Canbolat, A.F., Brown, L.J. & Macdonald, D.W. (1997) Mesh grids protect loggerhead turtle Caretta caretta nests from red fox Vulpes vulpes predation. Biological Conservation, 82, 109111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Bald Head Island, North Carolina, USA (map modified with permission from Hawkes et al., 2005). Approximately 15 km of beach on the west, south and east of Bald Head Island provide nesting habitat for loggerhead Caretta caretta marine turtles annually. Broken lines represent nesting beaches on Bald Head Island and nearby Oak Island (not included in our study). Bald Head Island is located at the confluence of the Cape Fear River and Atlantic Ocean. The rectangle on the inset indicates the location of the main map on the east coast of the USA.

Figure 1

Table 1 Number of artificial marine turtle nests in Experiment 1 that red foxes Vulpes vulpes attended, attempted to predate, or successfully predated on Bald Head Island (Fig. 1). Each nest was assigned to one of 3 treatments: control (unprotected), protection with a wire cage surrounding the nest, or protection with a plastic mesh screen placed on top of the nest and buried under 1–2 cm of sand.

Figure 2

Plate 1 (a) An unsuccessful predation attempt on a wire box cage-protected artificial nest (the fox, or foxes, attempted to dig at the nest from all sides of the cage but was/were unable to reach the egg chamber), and (b) a successful predation event by one or more red foxes on a screen-protected artificial nest.

Figure 3

Table 2 Relative strengths and weaknesses of two mechanical barriers (galvanized wire cage and plastic mesh screen) to red fox predation of artificial loggerhead marine turtle nests (++ and – – indicate qualitative assessments).