Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T20:48:56.830Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Born this way? A review of neurobiological and environmental evidence for the etiology of psychopathy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 October 2019

Annabelle Frazier*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Lowell, Lowell, MA, USA
Patricia A. Ferreira
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Lowell, Lowell, MA, USA
Joseph E. Gonzales
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Lowell, Lowell, MA, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Annabelle Frazier, Email: Annabelle_frazier@uml.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Across a significant body of research, psychopathy has often been conceptualized as a biologically based malady. In this research, genetic and neurobiological differences have been conceptualized to underlie psychopathy, while affected individuals’ life experiences only influence expressed psychopathic features and their severity. Psychopathy research has largely ignored developmental evidence demonstrating significant influences of environment on both biological and behavioral processes, resulting in several prominent criticisms (Edens & Vincent, 2008; Loeber, Byrd, & Farrington, 2015). The current review was conducted with two main aims: (a) to collect and consider etiological evidence from the extant body of research on genetic and neurobiological factors in psychopathy; and (b) to evaluate findings from genetic, neurotransmitter, brain structure, and brain function studies in the context of relevant evidence from developmental research. Examples from research on adversity and traumatic stress, a common correlate of psychopathy, were used to highlight current research gaps and future directions to aid in the integration of developmental and neurobiological research agendas. While some promising evidence exists regarding possible underlying neurobiological processes of psychopathic traits, this evidence is insufficient to suggest a largely biological etiology for the disorder. Further, information from developmental and epigenetic research may suggest complex, multidimensional trajectories for individuals experiencing psychopathy. Based on these observations, the authors make several recommendations for future research, as well as for current clinical application and practice.

Type
Review Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2019

Psychopathy is a clinical term originating in early 19th-century psychiatry, first comprehensively described in Cleckley’s (Reference Cleckley1941) seminal book, The Mask of Sanity. In recent years psychopathy has been used extensively in clinical, legal, and forensic settings (Edens & Vincent, Reference Edens and Vincent2008; Hare, Reference Hare1996, Reference Hare1999). Psychopathy is often described as an untreatable personality disorder consisting of an apparent absence of empathy and remorse, along with superficial charm, shallow relationships, and rational, cold-blooded self-gratification, which often occurs at the expense of others (Hare, Reference Hare1996, Reference Hare1999, Reference Hare2003; Moss & Prins, Reference Moss and Prins2006). Today, the label can produce significant consequences within the legal realm, such as increased sentence severity and likelihood of execution (Cox, Edens, Rulseh, & Clark, Reference Cox, Edens, Rulseh and Clark2016; Edens, Davis, Fernandez Smith, & Guy, Reference Edens, Davis, Fernandez Smith and Guy2013; Hare, Reference Hare1996) and harsher, less engaged treatment in community settings (Edens & Vincent, Reference Edens and Vincent2008; Kirkman, Reference Kirkman2008; Salekin, Worley, & Grimes, Reference Salekin, Worley and Grimes2010; Vidal & Skeem, Reference Vidal and Skeem2007).

In his 1999 book Without Conscience, developer of the Psychopathy Checklist (PCL), Robert Hare, explained that while the terms psychopath and sociopath are used interchangeably, the distinction lies in how one interprets the origins and determinants of each label. Generally, the former is used by those who ascribe the label a perdominantly biological etiology, whereas environmental factors are highlighted by those using the latter. A similar distinction is often made in considering primary and secondary psychopathy, where the former is thought to be a result of biological deficits, while the latter is attributed to various forms of social disadvantage (Newman, MacCoon, Vaughn, & Sadeh, Reference Newman, MacCoon, Vaughn and Sadeh2005; Vaughn, Edens, Howard, & Smith, Reference Vaughn, Edens, Howard and Smith2009).

In the following decades, Hare’s implied position – that psychopathy was much more a biological or genetic malady – has gained largely uncritical acceptance in both research and practice (Blair, Reference Blair2003, Reference Blair2006; Hare & Neumann, Reference Hare and Neumann2008). Researchers have invested heavily in exploring the various neurological and genetic factors suspected to cause or underlie psychopathic traits. The premise of such research has been that genes influence brain structures that are associated with psychopathic tendencies (Blair, Reference Blair2003; Hare & Neumann, Reference Hare and Neumann2008). Environmental influences merely act to shape psychopathic characteristics, which manifest in childhood as callous unemotional (CU) traits, and are then stable throughout the lifespan (Blair, Reference Blair2003; Hare & Neumann, Reference Hare and Neumann2008; Viding, Blair, Moffitt, & Plomin, Reference Viding, Blair, Moffitt and Plomin2005).

1. Developmental parallels

Prior to the advent of medicalized theories of its etiology, psychopathy was often regarded as environmentally derived (Blair, Reference Blair2003, Reference Blair2006; Hare & Neumann, Reference Hare and Neumann2008). In a parallel to Cleckley’s (Reference Cleckley1941) conceptualization of psychopathy, Bowlby (Reference Bowlby1944) described 14 “affectionless psychopaths,” who would meet today’s standard for diagnosis of reactive attachment disorder, given their documented behavior, affect, and histories of early and prolonged deprivation of care (Follan & Minnis, Reference Follan and Minnis2010). Bowlby hypothesized that deficits in early bonding with a consistent caregiver had caused these young people to perceive others as unworthy of trust, empathy, and concern (Saltaris, Reference Saltaris2002). Subsequently, insecure attachment patterns (Frodi, Dernevik, Sepa, Philipson, & Bragesjö, Reference Frodi, Dernevik, Sepa, Philipson and Bragesjö2001) and neglectful and maltreating family environments (Krischer & Sevecke, Reference Krischer and Sevecke2008; Marshall & Cooke, Reference Marshall and Cooke1999; Piquero et al., Reference Piquero, Farrington, Fontaine, Vincent, Coid and Ullrich2012) have been reported for individuals high in psychopathic traits.

Developmental research points toward the critical influence of early caregiving in healthy emotional development. Specifically, caregiver attunement and responsiveness to an infant’s emotional and physical needs plays a critical role in many psychological and physiological development processes. For instance, Hane and Fox (Reference Hane and Fox2006) reported that even typical variations in maternal sensitivity and intrusiveness affected infants’ social interaction, with infants whose mothers were less sensitive and/or more intrusive showing significantly less interest in social interaction and significantly more negative affect than those whose mothers were more sensitive and less intrusive. More importantly, even in their sample of low-risk, non-maltreated children, parenting differences were associated with neurological differences among the children (Hane & Fox, Reference Hane and Fox2006).

These neurological differences (Hane & Fox, Reference Hane and Fox2006) appear to be the product of neurobiological changes during a critical developmental period, caused by the infant’s interaction with his or her social environment (Gerhardt, Reference Gerhardt2006; Schore, Reference Schore2005). For example, Strathearn (Reference Strathearn2011) pointed out numerous biological influences of neglect. Observations of reductions in dopamine transporter binding in the ventral striatum, which cause elevated baseline dopamine levels, increased dopamine release in response to acute stress in adulthood, and increased sensitivity to psychostimulants were noted (Strathearn, Reference Strathearn2011). In a retrospective analysis, Pruessner, Champagne, Meaney, and Dagher (Reference Pruessner, Champagne, Meaney and Dagher2004) reported increased dopamine and cortisol release during stressful situations in individuals who reported low-quality relationships with caregivers in childhood.

Noteworthy is a timeline for the aforementioned neurobiological changes. Infants’ brain development begins with a growth spurt of the right hemisphere during the first 2 years of life (Schore, Reference Schore2005). The right hemisphere – involved in emotional and social processing – maintains dominance for the first 3 years after birth, until the left hemisphere’s development catches up (Schore, Reference Schore2005). Across longitudinal and cross-sectional brain imaging studies, chronic stress, deprivation, or maltreatment in the first 3 years of life has been shown to cause brain volume reductions and significant brain development abnormalities in affected 3-year-olds (Gerhardt, Reference Gerhardt2006; Perry & Pollard, Reference Perry and Pollard1997; Teicher & Samson, Reference Teicher and Samson2016). Yet children are highly unlikely to remember or have the ability to report on these early environments due to infantile amnesia (Peterson, Warren, & Short, Reference Peterson, Warren and Short2011). Consequently, the impact of adversity in infancy and early childhood cannot be captured by common traumatic exposure measures.

2. Exploring current research on psychopathy

Currently, despite the absence of conclusive evidence of a specific innate cause for psychopathy and despite indications of significant environmental influences on its development, research continues to focus principally on the search for its biological determinants, often to the exclusion of social, developmental, and environmental factors. Such efforts have also been extended to children, with both a modified version of the Psychopathy Checklist (PCL-Youth Version) and research targeting children with CU traits, which are argued to precede psychopathy in adulthood (Barry et al., Reference Barry, Frick, DeShazo, McCoy, Ellis and Loney2000; Frick, Reference Frick2004; Viding et al., Reference Viding, Blair, Moffitt and Plomin2005). In these research lines, researchers hope to identify psychopathic traits when they are more malleable, or find environmental triggers that activate the expression of genetically based psychopathic traits (Barry et al., Reference Barry, Frick, DeShazo, McCoy, Ellis and Loney2000; Frick, Reference Frick2004; Viding et al., Reference Viding, Blair, Moffitt and Plomin2005).

The application of psychopathy to children’s underdeveloped personalities has drawn the criticism of developmental psychologists and life-course criminologists alike (Edens et al., Reference Edens, Davis, Fernandez Smith and Guy2013; Edens & Vincent, Reference Edens and Vincent2008). In their criticism of the practice, Edens and Vincent (Reference Edens and Vincent2008) note that along with the increase in research evaluating psychopathic traits in youth, the applied (clinical and forensic) use of these measures has been on the rise. Yet, in adult populations, such applications lead to inherently negative, life-altering outcomes (Edens et al., Reference Edens, Davis, Fernandez Smith and Guy2013). Given limited evidence of temporal stability in personality characteristics, such applications can pose serious consequences on children whose current behavior and personality may be nothing beyond a mere state (Edens & Vincent, Reference Edens and Vincent2008). Notably, heterotypic continuity, the differential manifestation of symptoms over time (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996; Vitacco & Vincent, Reference Vitacco and Vincent2006), and typical developmental shifts in personality have been observed repeatedly in research (Edens & Vincent, Reference Edens and Vincent2008). For instance, when assessing the stability of psychopathic characteristics during the transition to adulthood, Hawes, Mulvey, Schubert, and Pardini (Reference Hawes, Mulvey, Schubert and Pardini2014) noted a decrease in these characteristics over time. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual 5 (DSM-5, 2013), and previous editions, specifically excludes children from its diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder (APD), based on similar concerns, while allowing diagnosis with oppositional-defiant disorder and conduct disorder in youth.

These research trends have been more broadly criticized for failing to adequately integrate forensic and developmental research. For instance, Loeber, Byrd, and Farrington (Reference Loeber, Byrd, Farrington, Morizot and Kazemian2015) argued that criminological and clinical research is largely based on male inmate or patient samples, and is rarely longitudinal. Consequently, neurological differences identified in this research cannot be shown to represent life-course stable abnormalities rather than normal or temporary variants. These critiques implicate the need to integrate clinical and developmental approaches to further develop the knowledge base on the etiology of antisociality (Edens & Vincent, Reference Edens and Vincent2008; Loeber et al., Reference Loeber, Byrd, Farrington, Morizot and Kazemian2015). However, few comprehensive efforts for such an integration have been made. The purpose of this article is to offer one such effort, by (a) evaluating the degree of integration of environmental and developmental correlates in biological research and (b) reviewing neurobiological research findings in the context of their developmental correlates. Conclusions regarding both the current state of knowledge on the etiology, treatment, and prognosis of psychopathy are also discussed.

3. Method

3.1. Inclusion criteria

Since 1999, when Hare’s book was published, a multitude of studies have investigated psychopathic traits in biological terms. However, much of this literature has been affected by two areas of debate among scholars and practitioners: (a) whether the psychopathy construct is driven by violent or rule-breaking behavior (Blair, Reference Blair2001; Porter & Woodworth, Reference Porter, Woodworth and Patrick2006; Skeem & Cooke, Reference Skeem and Cooke2010) and (b) whether APD and psychopathy represent two distinct constructs (Ogloff, Reference Ogloff2006; Patrick & Brislin, Reference Patrick, Brislin, Cautin and Lilienfeld2014). Within this body of literature, violence and aggression, CU traits, and antisocial personality and behavior are often discussed as proxies to (or precursers of) psychopathy (Barry et al., Reference Barry, Frick, DeShazo, McCoy, Ellis and Loney2000; Beaver et al., Reference Beaver, Wright, Boutwell, Barnes, DeLisi and Vaughn2013; Blair, Reference Blair2001; Gunter, Vaughn, & Philibert, Reference Gunter, Vaughn and Philibert2010; Hoenicka et al., Reference Hoenicka, Ponce, Jiménez-Arriero, Ampuero, Rodríguez-Jiménez, Rubio and Palomo2007; Hyde et al., Reference Hyde, Byrd, Votruba-Drzal, Hariri and Manuck2014; Raine et al., Reference Raine, Lencz, Taylor, Hellige, Bihrle, Lacasse and Colletti2003; Rautiainen et al., Reference Rautiainen, Paunio, Repo-Tiihonen, Virkkunen, Ollila, Sulkava and Tiihonen2016). The authors therefore elected to consider studies that measured these psychopathy traits using established measures of the construct (e.g., PCL and PCL-R), but also theoretically related traits and behaviors used in lieu of psychopathy measures due to the age of the subjects (e.g., CU traits) or their role as a proto-psychopathy indicator (e.g., antisocial behavior).

Within the current review, authors sought to identify studies that (a) were published between 1999 and 2017; (b) described quantitative analyses that included some measure of psychopathic tendency (broadly construed to include antisocial personality and behavior); and (c) included some neurobiological (i.e., genetic, neurotransmitter, brain imaging) measures with a human sample.

3.2. Search procedure

Based on the aforementioned criteria, the authors conducted a literature search on Google Scholar, using keyword combinations to account for the largest possible body of research in psychopathic and antisocial traits. For example, keywords included “psychopathy+gene,” as well as “antisocial+personality+neurotransmitter.” Abstracts of studies identified by this search (>1500) were then reviewed for relevance to the current review. Studies that (a) did not include a neurobiological measure, (b) where no human sample was used, (c) in which only traits or behaviors indirectly related to psychopathy were assessed (e.g., criminal or delinquent behavior; sexual aggression), or (d) where no empirical data collection was reported (i.e., theoretical papers and literature reviews) were excluded from this review. This process resulted in the inclusion of 54 studies in this review. Upon detailed review, four additional studies were excluded because they used the same dataset for multiple similar analyses (National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health; sample from employment temp agencies) or because characteristics of the sample were not described, ultimately resulting in a review of 50 studies.

3.3. Initial evaluation of studies

Each study was then assessed to determine whether at least one environmental factor (e.g., socioeconomic status, maltreatment, or trauma) was analyzed. The frequency with which environmental/developmental factors were reported was counted. Of the 50 studies, 17 (34%) included at least one environmental covariate or control. The list of studies included, and factors evaluated in them, is presented in Table 1. Similarly, studies were evaluated for their use of PCL type instruments and use of forensic and clinical samples, adults and children, and male and female participants.

Table 1. Characteristics of the reviewed studies

4. Sampling and measurement trends

Several general trends were observed in reviewed studies. First and foremost, environmental and developmental factors were not adequately integrated with biological research. Sixty-six percent of the studies reviewed did not consider any influences of environment or relevant developmental processes. In the remaining studies, the most common environmental variables included socioeconomic status (10% of studies), family environment (8% of studies), and maltreatment (8% of studies). In these studies, divergent measures and definitions made comparisons across findings difficult to make.

Second, the use of PCL-type measures – e.g., PCL-Revised (PCL-R, Hare, Reference Hare1991, Reference Hare2003) and PCL-Screening Version (PCL-SV, Hart, Cox, & Hare, Reference Hart, Cox and Hare1995) – decreased over time, corresponding with an increase in the use of child samples (16% of studies), although adults remained the primary subjects of this research. Overall, 46% of studies used PCL instruments, while 54% gauged psychopathic tendencies in other ways. The latter studies predominantly used the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM (SCID) diagnosis (12% of studies) along with some measure(s) of criminality or aggression – though others used versions of the Antisocial Process Screening Device (8% of studies), the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (6% of studies), and the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (4% of studies). While the majority of these instruments correlate with one another, they measure psychopathy differently (e.g., informal interview compared to self-report), and many consider criminality, which may be related to – but not essential to – the psychopathy construct. Studies that utilized PCL-type measures were similar to studies using other instruments across demographic characteristics (i.e., inclusion of female participants), except that 83% used adult forensic or prisoner samples. Given that the PCL-R requires a record review as part of accurate administration (Hare, Reference Hare2003), reliance on forensic samples in which these records exist for research using the PCL-R is unsurprising.

Further, neurobiological studies consistently favored male participants over females, and both forensic and clinical samples over community samples (36% of studies). Most frequently, adult participants were recruited from prisons and forensic institutional settings (26% of studies). Yet the influence of residing in the institution (Boxer, Middlemass, & Delorenzo, Reference Boxer, Middlemass and Delorenzo2009; Bukstel & Kilmann, Reference Bukstel and Kilmann1980; Haney, Reference Haney2003; Wooldredge, Reference Wooldredge1999) was not itself considered in the research. Female participants were included in 44% of studies, but in these studies, 50 male participants were recruited for every 27 female participants, on average. None of the studies used a female-only sample, despite concerns regarding the capacity of psychopathy measures to adequately capture the construct in females (Vitale, Smith, Brinkley, & Newman, Reference Vitale, Smith, Brinkley and Newman2002). Relatedly, only 30% of studies considered nonwhite racial and ethnic groups. While all humans share basic biological characteristics, genetic and biomedical differences (and thus risk factors) within different ethnic groups have been noted (Burchard et al., Reference Burchard, Ziv, Coyle, Gomez, Tang, Karter and Risch2003), including different genetic correlates of antisociality (Lu, Lin, Lee, Ko, & Shih, Reference Lu, Lin, Lee, Ko and Shih2003). Importantly, these shortages in participant diversity pose significant limitations to the generalizability of psychopathy research findings to the population.

5. Genetic etiology

Several authors have argued for a genetic or hereditary pathway toward psychopathy (Auty, Farrington, & Coid, Reference Auty, Farrington and Coid2015; Blair, Reference Blair2003, Reference Blair2006; Viding et al., Reference Viding, Blair, Moffitt and Plomin2005). The notion is that psychopathic individuals inherit a genetic makeup that manifests in altered brain functioning and physiological reactivity, and along with some environmental triggers or influences, shapes their behavior in childhood (Blair, Reference Blair2003, Reference Blair2006; Viding et al., Reference Viding, Blair, Moffitt and Plomin2005). For instance, Viding et al. (Reference Viding, Blair, Moffitt and Plomin2005) reported finding strong evidence of heredity and no evidence of shared environmental influences in their study of 7-year-old twins with antisocial behavior and CU traits. Similarly, Auty et al. (Reference Auty, Farrington and Coid2015) found strong evidence of transmission of psychopathy from fathers to their children, though this was mediated by environmental factors. Given the strength of genetic influence reported in these and similar findings, examining the evidence for specific genetic factors is important.

5.1. MAO-A

The strongest cumulative evidence base for a genetic pathway toward psychopathy is associated with the low-expression variant of the Monoamine Oxidase-A (MAO-A) gene, which encodes an enzyme that degrades mono-amine neurotransmitters – that is, dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin, Craig and Poulton2002; Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Thibodeau, Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Thibodeau2012). The low-expression variant of the MAO-A gene is linked to the X chromosome. Possessing only a single X chromosome, males are more likely to be influenced by a low-expression variant (Hunter, Reference Hunter2010).

In all eight studies investigating MAO-A polymorphisms (Beaver et al., Reference Beaver, Wright, Boutwell, Barnes, DeLisi and Vaughn2013; Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin, Craig and Poulton2002; Fowler et al., Reference Fowler, Langley, Rice, van den Bree, Ross, Wilkinson and Thapar2009; Kolla et al., Reference Kolla, Matthews, Wilson, Houle, Bagby, Links and Meyer2015; Longato-Stadler et al., Reference Longato-Stadler, af Klinteberg, Garpenstrand, Oreland and Hallman2002; Sadeh et al., Reference Sadeh, Javdani and Verona2013; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Gatt, Kuan, Dobson-Stone, Palmer, Paul and Gordon2009; Young et al., Reference Young, Smolen, Hewitt, Haberstick, Stallings, Corley and Crowley2006), statistically significant correlations were identified between the short allele and psychopathic and/or antisocial traits. The use of differing measures of psychopathy and antisocial behavior, along with inadequate delineation of participants’ histories of criminality and delinquency from the psychopathy construct, posed a significant limitation for many studies. Several authors found associations between subjects’ criminality and a lower-expression variant of the MAO-A gene (Beaver et al., Reference Beaver, Wright, Boutwell, Barnes, DeLisi and Vaughn2013; Kolla et al., Reference Kolla, Matthews, Wilson, Houle, Bagby, Links and Meyer2015; Sadeh et al., Reference Sadeh, Javdani and Verona2013), or specifically investigated APD diagnosis (Longato-Stadler et al., Reference Longato-Stadler, af Klinteberg, Garpenstrand, Oreland and Hallman2002; Young et al., Reference Young, Smolen, Hewitt, Haberstick, Stallings, Corley and Crowley2006), which placed significantly more weight on rule-breaking behavior (Hare, Reference Hare1996). Focusing specifically on core psychopathic traits, only two studies (Kolla et al., Reference Kolla, Matthews, Wilson, Houle, Bagby, Links and Meyer2015; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Gatt, Kuan, Dobson-Stone, Palmer, Paul and Gordon2009) identified significant differences between carriers of lower- and higher-expression MAO-A variants (i.e., increased psychopathic traits in short allele carriers). Fowler et al. (Reference Fowler, Langley, Rice, van den Bree, Ross, Wilkinson and Thapar2009) found lower-expression MAO-A variants to be significantly related to emotional dysfunction scores in youth diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), but the cumulative effect size for the same relation with total psychopathy-related scores was medium-sized (f 2 = 0.16).

In 2002, Caspi et al. observed an interaction between childhood adversity and the MAO-A risk allele, demonstrating that for those who carried this allele, adversity was positively related with psychopathy. Building on findings from Caspi et al. (Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin, Craig and Poulton2002), several subsequent MAO-A studies (Sadeh et al., Reference Sadeh, Javdani and Verona2013; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Gatt, Kuan, Dobson-Stone, Palmer, Paul and Gordon2009; Young et al., Reference Young, Smolen, Hewitt, Haberstick, Stallings, Corley and Crowley2006) included measures of childhood adversity or maltreatment. However, whereas some studies found associations between maltreatment, adversity, and rule-breaking behavior (Sadeh et al., Reference Sadeh, Javdani and Verona2013; Young et al., Reference Young, Smolen, Hewitt, Haberstick, Stallings, Corley and Crowley2006) or between the risk allele and antisocial behavior in participants who experienced severe family adversity (Fowler et al., Reference Fowler, Langley, Rice, van den Bree, Ross, Wilkinson and Thapar2009), others did not (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Gatt, Kuan, Dobson-Stone, Palmer, Paul and Gordon2009). Notably, these studies varied not only in their measures of psychopathic traits but also in their measurement of adversity and maltreatment. For instance, Williams et al.’s (Reference Williams, Gatt, Kuan, Dobson-Stone, Palmer, Paul and Gordon2009) analysis used only a dichotomous adversity score, comparing participants who experienced more than three adverse events to those who experienced three or fewer. Nonetheless, the interaction between MAO-A and maltreatment observed by Caspi et al. (Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin, Craig and Poulton2002) was not replicated in these subsequent studies.

5.2. 5-HTT

Based on observations of reduced serotonin in aggressive and impulsive individuals (Ferguson & Beaver, Reference Ferguson and Beaver2009; Goodman & New, Reference Goodman and New2000; Lesch & Merschdorf, Reference Lesch and Merschdorf2000), multiple studies have sought to associate 5-HTT with psychopathy. Findings in the seven studies that examined this relationship (Fowler et al., Reference Fowler, Langley, Rice, van den Bree, Ross, Wilkinson and Thapar2009; Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Aluja, Fibla, Cuevas and García2010; Hallikainen et al., Reference Hallikainen, Saito, Lachman, Volavka, Pohjalainen, Ryynanen and Tiihonen1999; Sadeh et al., Reference Sadeh, Javdani, Jackson, Reynolds, Potenza, Gelernter and Verona2010, experiments 1 and 2; Sadeh et al., Reference Sadeh, Javdani and Verona2013; Van de Giessen et al., Reference van de Giessen, Rosell, Thompson, Xu, Girgis, Ehrlich and Siever2014) were inconsistent. Sadeh et al. (Reference Sadeh, Javdani and Verona2013), for instance, found that PCL:SV Factor 2 scores were significantly related to carrying the long allele of 5-HTT and to Childhood Trauma Questionnaire scores, though no interaction was identified. Similarly, in Sadeh et al. (Reference Sadeh, Javdani, Jackson, Reynolds, Potenza, Gelernter and Verona2010, study II), CU traits increased in long/long allele carriers, but only as socioeconomic resources decreased. In contrast, aggression, impulsivity, and antisocial behavior was found to be related to carrying the short allele of 5-HTT (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Aluja, Fibla, Cuevas and García2010; Sadeh et al., Reference Sadeh, Javdani, Jackson, Reynolds, Potenza, Gelernter and Verona2010, study I). No relation between aggression and 5-HTT availability was identified in Van de Giessen et al. (Reference van de Giessen, Rosell, Thompson, Xu, Girgis, Ehrlich and Siever2014), but the authors observed a positive relation with callousness traits.

5.3. Dopamine receptor genes

Because of their role in the pleasure/reward system in humans, dopaminergic system genes have been a source of considerable research relating to violence, aggression, and antisocial behavior (Ferguson & Beaver, Reference Ferguson and Beaver2009; Ferguson, Reference Ferguson2010). Nonetheless, only three of the studies investigated dopaminergic gene systems (Hoenicka et al., Reference Hoenicka, Ponce, Jiménez-Arriero, Ampuero, Rodríguez-Jiménez, Rubio and Palomo2007; Ponce et al., Reference Ponce, Hoenicka, Jimenez-Arriero, Rodriguez-Jimenez, Aragüés, Martin-Sune and Palomo2008; Wu & Barnes, Reference Wu and Barnes2013). An additional study (Fowler et al., Reference Fowler, Langley, Rice, van den Bree, Ross, Wilkinson and Thapar2009) investigated the COMT gene, which regulates the production of the dopamine degrading enzyme, and is therefore discussed here as well.

All four studies reported relatively small impact of dopaminergic system genes. For instance, Fowler et al. (Reference Fowler, Langley, Rice, van den Bree, Ross, Wilkinson and Thapar2009) reported the high-activity COMT genotype was statistically associated with significantly higher emotional dysfunction scores, with a small effect size (f 2 = 0.08), and without significant impact on total psychopathy scores. Wu and Barnes (Reference Wu and Barnes2013) found that only DRD4 (and not DAT1 or DRD2) was significantly related to psychopathic personality traits. Yet carrying two DRD4 risk alleles (as compared to zero) increased participants’ mean psychopathy scores by only two points (from 55.97 to 57.99, on a scale of 23–115). Hoenicka et al. (Reference Hoenicka, Ponce, Jiménez-Arriero, Ampuero, Rodríguez-Jiménez, Rubio and Palomo2007) and Ponce et al. (Reference Ponce, Hoenicka, Jimenez-Arriero, Rodriguez-Jimenez, Aragüés, Martin-Sune and Palomo2008) found DRD2 gene polymorphisms to be associated with higher scores on the International Personality Disorder Examination (IPDE). Hoenicka et al. (Reference Hoenicka, Ponce, Jiménez-Arriero, Ampuero, Rodríguez-Jiménez, Rubio and Palomo2007) reported these genetic markers (when combined) may be responsible for 11.4% of the variance in psychopathy scores.

5.4. Other targets

Several other genetic polymorphisms have been analyzed for associations with psychopathy-related tendencies. These included SNAP25 t-snare protein gene (Basoglu et al., Reference Basoglu, Oner, Ates, Algul, Bez, Cetin and Munir2011; Hoenicka et al., Reference Hoenicka, Ponce, Jiménez-Arriero, Ampuero, Rodríguez-Jiménez, Rubio and Palomo2007), OXT, the gene responsible for oxytocin production (Dadds et al., Reference Dadds, Moul, Cauchi, Dobson-Stone, Hawes, Brennan and Ebstein2014), and the CNR1 and FAAH cannabinoid receptor gene polymorphisms (Hoenicka et al., Reference Hoenicka, Ponce, Jiménez-Arriero, Ampuero, Rodríguez-Jiménez, Rubio and Palomo2007). Hoenicka et al. (Reference Hoenicka, Ponce, Jiménez-Arriero, Ampuero, Rodríguez-Jiménez, Rubio and Palomo2007) found that variants of FAAH and CNR1 were related to an increase in PCL-R scores for their subjects, but found no impact of SNAP25 genes. In contrast, Basoglu et al. (Reference Basoglu, Oner, Ates, Algul, Bez, Cetin and Munir2011) reported that a variant of the SNAP25 gene was much more common in antisocial personality subjects, and that these antisocial subjects had significantly higher novelty-seeking scores. Dadds et al. (Reference Dadds, Moul, Cauchi, Dobson-Stone, Hawes, Brennan and Ebstein2014) observed maturation-related differences in genetic activity, with the low-CU group showing lower methylation of the OXT gene in older children, while the opposite trend was observed in the high-CU group. In contrast, Rautiainen et al.’s (Reference Rautiainen, Paunio, Repo-Tiihonen, Virkkunen, Ollila, Sulkava and Tiihonen2016) genome-wide association study found none of these associations in their subjects, instead reporting an observed association between APD diagnosis (based on SCID-II interview) and Human Leukocyte Antigen (HLA) system genes, which impact the immune system and express in the brain.

5.5. Reconsidering gene-environment interaction

Overall, it is well established that experiences and environmental influences can moderate the effects of individual polymorphisms on behavior and impact biological pathways that intersect with genetic influences, ultimately affecting gene expression (Manuck & McCaffery, Reference Manuck and McCaffery2014). The latter has been repeatedly shown to occur in maltreated children (Manuck & McCaffery, Reference Manuck and McCaffery2014; McDade et al., Reference McDade, Ryan, Jones, MacIsaac, Morin, Meyer and Kuzawa2017), with children who experienced early stress showing different gene methylation, and thus expression, in adolescence than those whose environments were relatively low in stress (McDade et al., Reference McDade, Ryan, Jones, MacIsaac, Morin, Meyer and Kuzawa2017; Essex et al., Reference Essex, Thomas Boyce, Hertzman, Lam, Armstrong, Neumann and Kobor2013). Despite this, fewer than 50% of genetic studies included any environmental covariates, and none accounted for epigenetic change. Epigenetic mechanisms are rarely considered in relation to psychopathic trait development but may provide important insights regarding its etiology (Gillett & Tamatea, Reference Gillett and Tamatea2012).

While the aforementioned findings regarding genetic factors in psychopathy are variable, their interpretation is further complicated by gene-environment interaction research in antisocial personality traits (Cicchetti et al., Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Thibodeau2012; Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Viding and Plomin2008). Particularly, maltreatment and childhood adversity have been linked to increases in CU or APD traits (Beach, Brody, Todorov, Gunter, & Philibert, Reference Beach, Brody, Todorov, Gunter and Philibert2010; Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin, Craig and Poulton2002; Chapman, Dube, & Anda, Reference Chapman, Dube and Anda2007; Cicchetti et al., Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Thibodeau2012; Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Viding and Plomin2008). Similarly, Kolla et al. (Reference Kolla, Matthews, Wilson, Houle, Bagby, Links and Meyer2015) reported a higher prevalence of childhood physical abuse in offenders exhibiting psychopathic traits, and that these early experiences were associated with increases in reactive aggression. Finally, several investigations (Hoenicka et al., Reference Hoenicka, Ponce, Jiménez-Arriero, Ampuero, Rodríguez-Jiménez, Rubio and Palomo2007; Ponce et al., Reference Ponce, Hoenicka, Jimenez-Arriero, Rodriguez-Jimenez, Aragüés, Martin-Sune and Palomo2008) have focused on psychopathy in those with substance use histories, and these have offered further support for the impact of childhood adversity on later substance use (Anda et al., Reference Anda, Whitfield, Felitti, Chapman, Edwards, Dube and Williamson2002; Dube et al., Reference Dube, Felitti, Dong, Chapman, Giles and Anda2003).

Other behavioral and environmental factors may influence gene expression in ways that genetic research has yet to predict. For instance, several of the same genetic polymorphisms have been investigated in both psychopathy and ADHD research. Indeed, subjects with ADHD were the focus of Fowler et al (Reference Fowler, Langley, Rice, van den Bree, Ross, Wilkinson and Thapar2009). However, childhood ADHD is associated with significant problems in social, school, and emotional functioning (DuPaul, McGoey, Eckert, & VanBrakle, Reference DuPaul, McGoey, Eckert and VanBrakle2001). These impairments have been linked with problematic family functioning, including greater family stress, higher rates of parental psychopathology, and conflicted parent–child relationships (Deault, Reference Deault2010), which may produce a developmental cascade unaccounted for by currently available research.

6. The role of neurotransmitters

Along with genetic polymorphism examinations, some researchers have sought to identify neurotransmitter involvement in psychopathy (Glenn & Raine, Reference Glenn and Raine2008; Wu & Barnes, Reference Wu and Barnes2013). This focus is not dissimilar to the interest in neurotransmitter activity underlying other mental disorders, based on the neurochemical imbalance hypothesis, which suggests that an imbalance in the brain chemistry of affected individuals leads to a variety of mental health problems (Deacon, Reference Deacon2013; France, Lysaker, & Robinson, Reference France, Lysaker and Robinson2007).

6.1. Norepinephrine

Often known as noradrenaline, norepinephrine functions as an activation neurotransmitter in the threat-response system. It has been long hypothesized to interact with dopamine in a system of neurochemicals linked to reward-oriented behavior and response to stress, ultimately leading to undesirable or dysfunctional behavior such as aggression or absent empathy (Antelman & Caggiula, Reference Antelman and Caggiula1977; Cases et al., Reference Cases, Seif, Grimsby, Gaspar, Chen, Pournin and De Maeyer1995; Thomas & Palmiter, Reference Thomas and Palmiter1997). Of the studies included in this review, the only one to investigate norepinephrine (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Moi, Bussandri, Delsignore, Caccavari and Brambilla2003) did not find a significant relationship with psychopathy.

6.2. Serotonin

Researchers have long speculated about an association between core psychopathic traits and nonoptimal stability and efficiency in serotonin functioning, whereby healthy levels of baseline arousal dampen psychophysiological responses to threat, punishment, and social stimuli (Yildirim & Derksen, Reference Yildirim and Derksen2013). These hypotheses have been supported by the finding that serotonin inhibits aggressive behavior (Carrillo, Ricci, Coppersmith, & Melloni, Reference Carrillo, Ricci, Coppersmith and Melloni2009; Cases et al., Reference Cases, Seif, Grimsby, Gaspar, Chen, Pournin and De Maeyer1995; de Boer & Koolhaas, Reference de Boer and Koolhaas2005). Serotonin was, therefore, the focus of a significant number of recent studies on antisocial and psychopathic samples (da Cunha-Bang et al., Reference da Cunha-Bang, Hjordt, Perfalk, Beliveau, Bock, Lehel and Knudsen2016; Dolan & Anderson, Reference Dolan and Anderson2003; Fanning et al., Reference Fanning, Berman, Guillot, Marsic and McCloskey2014; Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Moi, Bussandri, Delsignore, Caccavari and Brambilla2003; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Scarpa and Raine2002; Moul et al., Reference Moul, Dobson-Stone, Brennan, Hawes and Dadds2013; Rosell et al., Reference Rosell, Thompson, Slifstein, Xu, Frankle, New and Siever2010; Soderstrom et al., Reference Soderstrom, Blennow, Sjodin and Forsman2003; Stanley et al., Reference Stanley, Molcho, Stanley, Winchel, Gameroff, Parsons and Mann2000). In the eight reviewed studies, lower serotonin levels were linked more often to increased impulsivity traits (da Cunha-Bang et al., Reference da Cunha-Bang, Hjordt, Perfalk, Beliveau, Bock, Lehel and Knudsen2016; Dolan & Anderson, Reference Dolan and Anderson2003; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Scarpa and Raine2002) than to callous-unemotional traits (Moul et al., Reference Moul, Dobson-Stone, Brennan, Hawes and Dadds2013) or aggression (Stanley et al., Reference Stanley, Molcho, Stanley, Winchel, Gameroff, Parsons and Mann2000). A deficiency in serotonin production may, therefore, not actually underlie aggressive, antisocial tendencies but rather impulse control problems that may lead to antisocial behaviors. Notably, Moore et al. (Reference Moore, Scarpa and Raine2002) reported that this effect was moderated by age, thus supporting the notion that differences in serotonin production are not life-course stable.

6.3. Dopamine

Due to its association with hyperactivity, aggressive, and reward-motivated behavior, dopamine has been investigated in three of the reviewed studies (Buckholtz et al., Reference Buckholtz, Treadway, Cowan, Woodward, Benning, Li and Smith2010; Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Moi, Bussandri, Delsignore, Caccavari and Brambilla2003; Soderstrom et al., Reference Soderstrom, Blennow, Sjodin and Forsman2003). It is often assumed that dopaminergic genes cause an overabundance of the neurotransmitter, which produce some of the behavioral and emotional manifestations common in psychopathy. However, results of these studies have contradicted this idea. For instance, Gerra et al. (Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Moi, Bussandri, Delsignore, Caccavari and Brambilla2003) found that subjects with antisocial personalities showed reduced dopaminergic receptor sensitivity, suggesting that their subjects’s brains were less responsive to dopamine and thus required more of it. In contrast, Buckholtz et al. (Reference Buckholtz, Treadway, Cowan, Woodward, Benning, Li and Smith2010) reported dopamine system hyperactivity in their subjects, which they associated with antisocial-related impulsivity. Soderstrom et al. (Reference Soderstrom, Blennow, Sjodin and Forsman2003) found that aggression was related to a high dopamine turnover together with serotonergic dysregulation. Since not all psychopathic individuals are both aggressive and impulsive (i.e., some may only possess one of the two traits), these latter findings suggest that researchers may be observing correlates of aggression and impulsivity rather than psychopathy.

7. Neurological differences

Observations of the emotional and empathic deficits in psychopathic offenders have led to hypotheses of functional and/or structural neurological abnormalities in antisocial individuals (Shamay-Tsoory, Harari, Aharon-Peretz, & Levkovitz, Reference Shamay-Tsoory, Harari, Aharon-Peretz and Levkovitz2010; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Raine, Narr, Colletti and Toga2009). Specifically, it has been suggested that, as a result of inherited genetic differences affecting neurotransmission and neurodevelopment, psychopathic individuals have decreased activation and volume in key areas of the brain, including the orbitofrontal cortex (Mitchell, Colledge, Leonard, & Blair, Reference Mitchell, Colledge, Leonard and Blair2002) and amygdalae, as well as broader gray matter volume reductions (Blair, Reference Blair2006; Blair, Peschardt, Budhani, Mitchell, & Pine, Reference Blair2006).

7.1. Whole-brain studies

Multiple whole-brain volume (Barkataki et al., Reference Barkataki, Kumari, Das, Taylor and Sharma2006; Raine et al., Reference Raine, Lencz, Taylor, Hellige, Bihrle, Lacasse and Colletti2003), blood flow (Soderstrom et al., Reference Soderstrom, Hultin, Tullberg, Wikkelso, Ekholm and Forsman2002), and neural activation (Marsh et al., Reference Marsh, Finger, Fowler, Adalio, Jurkowitz, Schechter and Blair2013; Müller et al., Reference Müller, Sommer, Wagner, Lange, Taschler, Röder and Hajak2003; Rilling et al., Reference Rilling, Glenn, Jairam, Pagnoni, Goldsmith, Elfenbein and Lilienfeld2007) studies assessed differences between typical and psychopathic individuals. Of the studies investigating whole-brain volume, one observed increased volume (Raine et al., Reference Raine, Lencz, Taylor, Hellige, Bihrle, Lacasse and Colletti2003), while the other observed decreased volume (Barkataki et al., Reference Barkataki, Kumari, Das, Taylor and Sharma2006). Oddly, both studies included participants with schizophrenia symptoms. Barkataki et al. (Reference Barkataki, Kumari, Das, Taylor and Sharma2006), whose sample comprised 50% schizophrenia-affected participants, reported significant differences in intelligence between them and antisocial participants (who did not exhibit similar psychotic symptoms), along with reduced brain volume in comparison to nonviolent individuals and healthy controls. In contrast, Raine et al. (Reference Raine, Lencz, Taylor, Hellige, Bihrle, Lacasse and Colletti2003) reported that though the difference between participants and controls in psychotic symptoms was statistically significant (i.e., 40% met schizophrenia spectrum diagnosis criteria), they believed these symptoms did not significantly affect their participants’ outcomes on the measures of interest, reporting increased volume as compared to controls. Yet such a conclusion – that the presence of psychosis would not be meaningful for a brain volume analysis of antisocial participants – is at odds with strong evidence for brain volume and function differences in psychotic individuals (Radua et al., Reference Radua, Borgwardt, Crescini, Mataix-Cols, Meyer-Lindenberg, McGuire and Fusar-Poli2012). Further, the presence of psychotic symptoms is not typical or expected for antisocial or psychopathic individuals.

Regionally, Laakso et al. (Reference Laakso, Vaurio, Koivisto, Savolainen, Eronen, Aronen and Tiihonen2001) observed a positive correlation between the hippocampal volumes and age of the subjects, while also noting negative correlations between regional volume reductions and PCL-R Factor 1 scores. In analyzing blood circulation in the brain, Soderstrom et al. (Reference Soderstrom, Hultin, Tullberg, Wikkelso, Ekholm and Forsman2002) reported no significant correlation between bloodflow and total PCL-R scores, though strong negative correlations were identified between the temporal blood flow and Factor 1 (emotional/affective) PCL-R scores, with a Spearman’s rho of .34 for the right, and .49 for the left side. The highest-scoring subjects exhibited significantly lower bloodflow in the head of the caudate nucleus, the hippocampus, the left thalamus, the amygdala, and the medial and lateral frontal areas (Soderstrom et al., Reference Soderstrom, Hultin, Tullberg, Wikkelso, Ekholm and Forsman2002).

Several studies have reported different activation patterns across a variety of tasks in the brains of individuals with psychopathic traits compared to those of controls. Specifically, reduced activation in areas influencing autonomic regulation (i.e., the rostral anterior cingulate cortex and right ventral striatum) in response to perceived personal distress or pain, and in the affective regulation areas (i.e., the amygdala and insula) in response to others’ fear, distress and pain, has been reported in psychopathic individuals (Marsh et al., Reference Marsh, Finger, Fowler, Adalio, Jurkowitz, Schechter and Blair2013). Similarly, reduced activation was found in areas responsible for reinforcement learning (i.e., the dorsal striatal circuits; Marsh et al., Reference Marsh, Finger, Fowler, Adalio, Jurkowitz, Schechter and Blair2013; Rilling et al., Reference Rilling, Glenn, Jairam, Pagnoni, Goldsmith, Elfenbein and Lilienfeld2007).

Like Birbaumer et al.’s (Reference Birbaumer, Veit, Lotze, Erb, Hermann, Grodd and Flor2005), Marsh et al. (Reference Marsh, Finger, Fowler, Adalio, Jurkowitz, Schechter and Blair2013) subjects showed significantly less activation in the left amygdala, left middle and right anterior insula, anterior cingulate, right secondary somatosensory cortex, and the right ventromedial orbitofrontal cortex in the second half of the acquisition phase of the stimulus learning task. In contrast, Kiehl et al. (Reference Kiehl, Smith, Mendrek, Forster, Hare and Liddle2004) reported that activation (in response to stimulus words) was overall similar between subjects and controls, except in the right anterior superior temporal gyrus for psychopathic individuals’ responses between abstract and baseline phases (Kiehl et al., Reference Kiehl, Smith, Mendrek, Forster, Hare and Liddle2004). Further, Kiehl et al.’s (Reference Kiehl, Smith, Mendrek, Forster, Hare and Liddle2004) subjects also exhibited reduced activation in these regions in response to concrete stimuli.

In a prisoners’ dilemma game, male (but not female) subjects higher in psychopathy showed reduced activation in the right amygdala after a partner defected, and reduced activation within the rostral anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex when choosing to defect (Rilling et al., Reference Rilling, Glenn, Jairam, Pagnoni, Goldsmith, Elfenbein and Lilienfeld2007). In response to the International Standardized Affective Picture System positive/negative emotion task, subjects with higher psychopathy scores showed increased right-sided activation in prefrontal regions, anterior cingulate, and the amygdala for negative emotions, and reduced activation in the right subgenual cingulate and right medial temporal gyrus, left lobulus paracentralis, left dorsal cingulate, and left parahippocampal gyrus (Müller et al., Reference Müller, Sommer, Wagner, Lange, Taschler, Röder and Hajak2003). In contrast, positive emotions induced increased activation in the left gyrus frontalis, and right medial frontal and right medial temporal gyrus, leading to the conclusion that psychopathic individuals’ emotional responsivity differed from that of other subjects (Müller et al., Reference Müller, Sommer, Wagner, Lange, Taschler, Röder and Hajak2003).

7.2. Gray matter

Multiple research papers (de Oliveira-Souza et al., Reference de Oliveira-Souza, Hare, Bramati, Garrido, Ignácio, Tovar-Moll and Moll2008; Ermer et al., Reference Ermer, Cope, Nyalakanti, Calhoun and Kiehl2012; Raine et al., Reference Raine, Lencz, Bihrle, LaCasse and Colletti2000; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Raine, Colletti, Toga and Narr2010) have reported observations of reduced gray matter volume in the orbitofrontal cortex of psychopathic individuals. Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Raine, Colletti, Toga and Narr2010), in comparing “successful” and “unsuccessful” psychopaths, reported that these (and other observed differences) applied only to the latter due to arrest histories. In addition, de Oliveira-Souza et al. (Reference de Oliveira-Souza, Hare, Bramati, Garrido, Ignácio, Tovar-Moll and Moll2008) observed gray matter volume reductions in the mid-anterior insula, and left anterior temporal cortex, while Ermer et al. (Reference Ermer, Cope, Nyalakanti, Calhoun and Kiehl2012) noted gray matter volume reductions in the parahippocampal cortex, though these were not statistically significant in whole-brain analyses. Ermer et al. (Reference Ermer, Cope, Nyalakanti, Calhoun and Kiehl2012) speculated that differences in gray matter in psychopathy are subtle and widespread, leading to minimal differentiation when whole-brain analyses are used. Raine et al. (Reference Raine, Lencz, Bihrle, LaCasse and Colletti2000) noted that observed reductions amounted to 11% compared to controls. These studies suggest that higher PCL-R scores may be associated with subtle reductions in gray matter volume across several paralimbic and limbic areas, implicated in emotion processing, theory-of-mind-related tasks, regulation, and behavioral control. However, Yang et al.’s (Reference Yang, Raine, Colletti, Toga and Narr2010) distinction between individuals who have been arrested is important in interpreting these results given that the majority of psychopathy studies used forensic and institutional samples (Birbaumer et al., Reference Birbaumer, Veit, Lotze, Erb, Hermann, Grodd and Flor2005; de Oliveira-Souza et al., Reference de Oliveira-Souza, Hare, Bramati, Garrido, Ignácio, Tovar-Moll and Moll2008; Ermer et al., Reference Ermer, Cope, Nyalakanti, Calhoun and Kiehl2012; Kiehl et al., Reference Kiehl, Smith, Mendrek, Forster, Hare and Liddle2004; Laakso et al., Reference Laakso, Vaurio, Koivisto, Savolainen, Eronen, Aronen and Tiihonen2001; Marsh et al., Reference Marsh, Finger, Fowler, Adalio, Jurkowitz, Schechter and Blair2013; Müller et al., Reference Müller, Sommer, Wagner, Lange, Taschler, Röder and Hajak2003; Soderstrom et al., Reference Soderstrom, Hultin, Tullberg, Wikkelso, Ekholm and Forsman2002) – that is, these differences may not be evident in individuals with higher PCL-R scores but who avoid incarceration.

7.3. The amygdala

Due to its association with emotional functioning, the amygdala has been a popular focus in neurobiological studies (Boccardi et al., Reference Boccardi, Frisoni, Hare, Cavedo, Najt, Pievani and Vaurio2011; Hyde et al., Reference Hyde, Byrd, Votruba-Drzal, Hariri and Manuck2014; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Laurens, Herba, Barker and Viding2009; Marsh et al., Reference Marsh, Finger, Fowler, Jurkowitz, Schechter, Henry and Blair2011; Pardini et al., Reference Pardini, Raine, Erickson and Loeber2014; White et al., Reference White, Marsh, Fowler, Schechter, Adalio, Pope and Blair2012b; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Raine, Narr, Colletti and Toga2009). In studies that evaluated amygdala volume, one reported volume reductions (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Raine, Narr, Colletti and Toga2009), one reported increases (Boccardi et al., Reference Boccardi, Frisoni, Hare, Cavedo, Najt, Pievani and Vaurio2011), and yet another reported no difference (Pardini et al., Reference Hawes, Mulvey, Schubert and Pardini2014).

In observing amygdala activation patterns, right amygdala activation has been noted to increase in response to fearful faces (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Laurens, Herba, Barker and Viding2009), but decrease while comparing words related to legal and illegal acts (Marsh et al., Reference Marsh, Finger, Fowler, Jurkowitz, Schechter, Henry and Blair2011). Yet White et al. (Reference White, Marsh, Fowler, Schechter, Adalio, Pope and Blair2012b) reported that during low attentional load trials subjects’ amygdalae activation increased less than that of controls, as compared to high attentional load activation. Intrestingly, Hyde et al. (Reference Hyde, Byrd, Votruba-Drzal, Hariri and Manuck2014) reported that when considered separately, APD and psychopathy traits did not have a significant relation to amygdala activation. Only when both were considered together did an association emerge. Hyde et al. (Reference Hyde, Byrd, Votruba-Drzal, Hariri and Manuck2014) attributed this to suppression effects, whereby APD and psychopathy measures served as a “cooperative suppressor” for each other (Paulhus, Robins, Trzesniewski, & Tracy, Reference Paulhus, Robins, Trzesniewski and Tracy2004), amplifying the effects of the other on amygdala activation. Thus, Hyde et al. (Reference Hyde, Byrd, Votruba-Drzal, Hariri and Manuck2014) argued that one of the two constructs (antisocial personality or psychopathy) was unrelated to amygdala activation, yet raised the impact of the other on the predictive ability of the model as a whole (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, Reference Cohen, Cohen, West and Aiken2013; Paulhus et al., Reference Paulhus, Robins, Trzesniewski and Tracy2004).

7.4. Integrating environmental influences into brain research

Environmental covariates were rarely considered in brain research. Only 4 of the aforementioned 20 studies included such variables. Yet, developmental research has consistently shown that neglected, maltreated, or stress-exposed children exhibit many of the same brain abnormalities observed in in these studies. For instance, smaller left amygdala volumes and significant whole-brain gray matter reductions have been reported in post-institutionalized children (Mehta et al., Reference Mehta, Golembo, Nosarti, Colvert, Mota, Williams and Sonuga-Barke2009), as well as localized hippocampal, insular, orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate gyrus, and caudate volume reductions (Dannlowski et al., Reference Dannlowski, Stuhrmann, Beutelmann, Zwanzger, Lenzen, Grotegerd and Lindner2012). Perry (Reference Perry2002) reported abnormal, atypical frontal-occipital circumference development of children who were chronically neglected. Though multiple studies have found increased amygdala responsivity to acute stress in people who have experienced significant early-life environmental stress or adversity (Maheu et al., Reference Maheu, Dozier, Guyer, Mandell, Peloso, Poeth and Ernst2010; Swartz, Williamson, & Hariri, Reference Swartz, Williamson and Hariri2015; White et al., Reference White, Bogdan, Fisher, Munoz, Williamson and Hariri2012a), others have reported decreased amygdala activation in individuals resilient to post-traumatic stress disorder (Phan, Britton, Taylor, Fig, & Liberzon, Reference Phan, Britton, Taylor, Fig and Liberzon2006), and reciprocal and opposing relations between activation in the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex (Shin et al., Reference Shin, Orr, Carson, Rauch, Macklin, Lasko and Alpert2004).

In a first-of-its-kind recent study, Sethi et al. (Reference Sethi, McCrory, Puetz, Hoffmann, Knodt, Radtke and Viding2018) distinguished among psychopathic individuals with high anxiety and maltreatment (who may be classified with “secondary” psychopathy), and those without adversity or anxiety (i.e., with “primary” psychopathy). Despite similar interpersonal/affective facet scores among the two groups, the researchers indeed found differences among them in both fear-related brain activation patterns, and in amygdala activation (which was reportedly typical in those with maltreatment histories). This observation led the Sethi et al. (Reference Sethi, McCrory, Puetz, Hoffmann, Knodt, Radtke and Viding2018) to speculate that abnormalities in fear conditioning may play a role in the development of Factor 1 traits in the presence of anxiety, possibly in response to environmental trauma.

8. Psychophysiological manifestations

Several psychophysiological differences have been noted in biological research. Yildirim and Derksen (Reference Yildirim and Derksen2013) proposed that these result from neurological and neurotransmitter activity differences that produce typical baseline arousal levels while dampening arousal during stressful, frightening, or adverse social experiences. Indeed, Raine et al. (Reference Raine, Lencz, Bihrle, LaCasse and Colletti2000) reported that during a social stressor task, antisocial individuals exhibited reduced autonomic activity, skin conductance, and heart rate. In a meta-analysis of 95 studies, Lorber (Reference Lorber2004) found that low resting electrodermal activity (EDA) and low task EDA were associated with psychopathy, and that psychopathy was negatively associated with EDA reactivity. However, Lorber’s (Reference Lorber2004) analysis did not identify a significant effect for resting heart-rate or heart-rate reactivity when aggression was considered separately from the psychopathy construct.

In studying antisocial behavior, Portnoy and Farrington (Reference Portnoy and Farrington2015) found that across 114 studies, antisocial behavior was related to low resting heart rate, with no moderating impact of age, sex, study design. However, Portnoy and Farrington’s (Reference Portnoy and Farrington2015) meta-analysis demonstrated that across measurements of antisociality (e.g., offending behavior, violence, aggression, psychopathy) studies differed significantly in their findings. Similarly, in a sample of Asian “primary” and “secondary” psychopathic women, the latter exhibited much higher heart rate variability across rest and stress tasks, despite significant difference in interpersonal/affective facet scores in both groups (Goulter, Kimonis, Denson, & Begg, Reference Goulter, Kimonis, Denson and Begg2019). In a sample of juveniles, a lower blink rate was observed among those labeled with “primary” (as compared to “secondary”) psychopathy (Kimonis, Fanti, Goulter, & Hall, Reference Kimonis, Fanti, Goulter and Hall2017). Lorber (Reference Lorber2004) suggested that contradictory findings may be attributable to heterogeneity in the behavioral construct (e.g., “successful” vs. “unsuccessful”; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Raine, Colletti, Toga and Narr2010). Indeed, some have suggested that the construct includes so many diverse presentations that a multitude of etiological pathways should be considered (Brinkley, Newman, Widiger, & Lynam, Reference Brinkley, Newman, Widiger and Lynam2004). A further consideration of this multiple-pathway hypothesis can benefit from integration of biological findings with developmental literature.

8.1. Physiology and stress

Related to the documented neurological and neurochemical changes in stress-exposed children, alterations in physiological manifestation have been documented as well. The physiological consequences of these neurological changes are similarly diverse, with individuals’ affected traumatic stress symptoms exhibiting increased ANS activation during acute stress, while others exhibit reduced activation along with fewer PTSD symptoms (Perry, Reference Perry and Murberg1994, Reference Perry1999; Scheeringa, Zeanah, Myers, & Putnam, Reference Scheeringa, Zeanah, Myers and Putnam2004). These findings have prompted speculation that adaptive dissociative states may stabilize into traits in certain instances (Perry, Pollard, Blakley, Baker, & Vigilante, Reference Perry, Pollard, Blakley, Baker and Vigilante1995).

9. Making sense of neurobiological findings

While overarching conclusions on the neuobiological mechanisms underlying psychopathic traits cannot be made due to contradictory findings, a theoretical trend emerges from the explored evidence. Suppressed activation has been noted across genetic, neurological, and psychophysiological findings. As for genetic evidence, the low-expression MAO-A variant is understood as having an inhibitory effect on the enzyme encoded by it (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin, Craig and Poulton2002; Cicchetti et al., Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Thibodeau2012). Although the exact nature of its influence is unclear (Sadeh et al., Reference Sadeh, Javdani, Jackson, Reynolds, Potenza, Gelernter and Verona2010, Reference Sadeh, Javdani and Verona2013), the low-activity serotonin transporter (5-HTT) variant has also been linked with antisociality (Fowler et al., Reference Fowler, Langley, Rice, van den Bree, Ross, Wilkinson and Thapar2009). As for neurological studies, many have reported a comparative suppression of neural activity and/or volume in designated brain structures like the orbitofrontal cortex, frontal lobes, and amygdalae (Blair, Reference Blair2006; Blair et al., Reference Blair2006; Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, Colledge, Leonard and Blair2002; Raine et al., Reference Raine, Park, Lencz, Bihrle, LaCasse, Widom, Al-Dayeh and Singh2001). Moreover, in considering psychophysiological evidence, a pattern of repressed activity and/or physical responses is also pertinent, as evidenced in studies revealing depressed skin conductance and heart rate in antisocial participants (Lorber, Reference Lorber2004; Portnoy & Farrington, Reference Portnoy and Farrington2015; Raine et al., Reference Raine, Lencz, Bihrle, LaCasse and Colletti2000).

Despite the noted trend, many findings challenge the proposed pattern of suppressed activation. For this reason, these observations allow for little in the way of definitive conclusions. For instance, a link between a high-activity COMT variant and higher “emotional dysfunction” has been found (Fowler et al., Reference Fowler, Langley, Rice, van den Bree, Ross, Wilkinson and Thapar2009). A higher-activity 5-HTT variant has also been shown to exert greater influence on affective-interpersonal traits when compared to its lower-activity counterpart (Sadeh et al., Reference Sadeh, Javdani and Verona2013). In addition, higher activation of certain brain structures (see Müller et al., Reference Müller, Sommer, Wagner, Lange, Taschler, Röder and Hajak2003) has been associated with emotional reactions to both negative and positive stimuli in psychopathic subjects.

When it comes to neurotransmitter evidence, applying the suppressed activation theory becomes an even greater challenge. If the low-expression MAO-A variant encodes a lower-activity enzyme that reduces the breakdown of monoamine neurotransmitters, it is reasonable to theorize that this may result in increased synaptic presence of the same neurotransmitters (Cases et al., Reference Cases, Seif, Grimsby, Gaspar, Chen, Pournin and De Maeyer1995; Fowler et al., Reference Fowler, Langley, Rice, van den Bree, Ross, Wilkinson and Thapar2009). However, while increased presence (or decreased degradation) of neurotransmitters appears to contradict the overarching pattern of suppression, the absolute or partial inhibitory effects associated with some of these neurotransmitters (e.g., absent empathy, or decreased psychophysiological responses; Cases et al., Reference Cases, Seif, Grimsby, Gaspar, Chen, Pournin and De Maeyer1995; Thomas & Palmiter, Reference Thomas and Palmiter1997; Yildirim & Derksen, Reference Yildirim and Derksen2013) are in line with a suppression pattern.

One of the greatest challenges in drawing any trend-like conclusions concerning neurotransmitters is the fact that there appears to be a higher degree of inconsistency in the examined literature than that observed in genetic and brain structure research. For example, studies into the relationship between serotonin and aggression have produced only mixed results, with some offering support for a negative correlation (as cited by Ferguson & Beaver, Reference Ferguson and Beaver2009), while others point in the direction of a positive one (Carrillo et al., Reference Carrillo, Ricci, Coppersmith and Melloni2009; Van de Giessen et al., Reference van de Giessen, Rosell, Thompson, Xu, Girgis, Ehrlich and Siever2014). It has been suggested that aggression type (reactive vs. proactive) is likely responsible for this inconsistency (Blair, Reference Blair2006; Carrillo et al., Reference Carrillo, Ricci, Coppersmith and Melloni2009; Van de Giessen et al., Reference van de Giessen, Rosell, Thompson, Xu, Girgis, Ehrlich and Siever2014). This proposed distinction underscores the intricacy of characteristics underlying psychopathy and its many risk factors. Similarly, the distinction between “primary” and “secondary” psychopathy, which has thus been little investigated in this context (Goulter et al., Reference Goulter, Kimonis, Denson and Begg2019; Sethi et al., Reference Sethi, McCrory, Puetz, Hoffmann, Knodt, Radtke and Viding2018), may explain contradictory findings.

In addition to specific distinctions, determining the role individual neurotransmitters play in psychopathy is convoluted by the notion that some appear to have a differing, synergistic-like effect when interacting with one another. This is evident in the reported interactive effect of dopamine and epinephrine in producing both increased aggression and reduced empathy (Antelman & Caggiula, Reference Antelman and Caggiula1977; Cases et al., Reference Cases, Seif, Grimsby, Gaspar, Chen, Pournin and De Maeyer1995; Thomas & Palmiter, Reference Thomas and Palmiter1997); these qualities were not observed when epinephrine was examined in isolation (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Moi, Bussandri, Delsignore, Caccavari and Brambilla2003). Furthermore, an interaction between cerebrospinal fluid dopaminergic and serotonergic metabolite levels has been speculated to underlie aggression (Soderstrom et al., Reference Soderstrom, Blennow, Sjodin and Forsman2003), but this interaction has been rarely studied, while studies frequently examined serotonin alone (see Carrillo et al., Reference Carrillo, Ricci, Coppersmith and Melloni2009).

Because many studies have such a narrow scope, bridging the gap between their results becomes a difficult task. Moving beyond general observations of suppression, in a direction that leads to a fuller understanding of the interplay between all of the explored evidence, would presently be unfeasible. The inconsistency in findings appears to be rooted in a number of factors including variability in conceptualizations and measurements used across studies (Kim-Cohen et al., Reference Kim-Cohen, Caspi, Taylor, Williams, Newcombe, Craig and Moffitt2006; Lorber, Reference Lorber2004); absent consideration of environmental experiences that interact with one’s genotype, and their epigenetic outcomes (Fowler et al., Reference Fowler, Langley, Rice, van den Bree, Ross, Wilkinson and Thapar2009); and the use of broad categories and classifications for traits and behaviors that may, in fact, have different underlying neurobiological characteristics (e.g., measuring aggression without accounting for aggression types, as discussed earlier; Carrillo et al., Reference Carrillo, Ricci, Coppersmith and Melloni2009; Van de Gissen et al., 2014). Similar arguments may be made based on emergent research into primary and secondary variants of psychopathy (Goulter et al., Reference Goulter, Kimonis, Denson and Begg2019; Kimonis et al., Reference Kimonis, Fanti, Goulter and Hall2017; Sethi et al., Reference Sethi, McCrory, Puetz, Hoffmann, Knodt, Radtke and Viding2018). It is probable that other distinctions and interactions have yet to be uncovered; and until they are, understanding the relationship between genes, neurotransmission, brain activity, and psychophysiology in psychopathy remains obscured for the most part.

10. Neurobiology and development: directions for interdisciplinary collaboration

Considering the potential for early-life epigenetic change and early evolution of neurobiological differences produced by stress and adversity, it is critical that longitudinal perspectives be taken in psychopathy research utilizing neurobiological measures. In fact, cross-sectional designs using 7-year-olds may offer little added benefit to using adult participants, considering some changes are likely to occur in the first 3 years of life. In contrast, given reliance on incarcerated samples, much of the extant research excludes individuals who have entered and passed midlife, hindering predictions about stability of psychopathy traits over time. Furthermore, any observations made during adulthood are best interpreted as snapshots in time, which cannot inform our understanding of etiology nor prognosis, given later-in-life biological and behavioral changes (e.g., Hawes et al., Reference Hawes, Mulvey, Schubert and Pardini2014; Vitacco & Vincent, Reference Vitacco and Vincent2006). For instance, psychopathy reportedly increases in adolescence and diminishes as individuals reach young adulthood (Salekin, Rosenbaum, Lee, & Lester, Reference Salekin, Rosenbaum, Lee and Lester2009). Similarly, psychopathy scores (and offending behaviors) are generally significantly reduced as previously labeled ex-inmates enter into their 60s (Putkonen et al., Reference Putkonen, Weizmann-Henelius, Repo-Tiihonen, Lindberg, Saarela, Eronen and Häkkänen-Nyholm2010), suggesting an instability in psychopathy over the life course.

Moreover, the current understanding of genetic and neurobiological psychopathic mechanisms is based primarily on observations of incarcerated men of European descent. This limitation is particularly problematic to genetic research, since different ethnic groups vary in genetic manifestation of various conditions (Burchard et al., Reference Burchard, Ziv, Coyle, Gomez, Tang, Karter and Risch2003), including psychopathy (Lu et al., Reference Lu, Lin, Lee, Ko and Shih2003) and its socio-cultural interpretation (Mokros et al., Reference Mokros, Neumann, Stadtland, Osterheider, Nedopil and Hare2011). Multicultural investigations can be uniquely informative, since non-invariance in the psychopathy construct between samples from similar cultural backgrounds (i.e., North Americans and Germans) has been noted (Mokros et al., Reference Mokros, Neumann, Stadtland, Osterheider, Nedopil and Hare2011). Research using community samples, women, and people of non-European heritage is necessary before any sweeping etiological conclusions can be drawn.

Also critical to consider are the ways constructs are measured across current research. Psychopathy is measured in a wide variety of ways, with some measures relying heavily on aggression and criminal justice involvement. Moreover, reliance on criminal history in the measurement of psychopathy is particularly problematic given Yang et al.’s (Reference Yang, Raine, Colletti, Toga and Narr2010) findings that some physiological differences in individuals with arrest histories did not appear in those without such histories. Similarly, adversity-exposure measurement is often problematic in that different instruments capture different adverse events and all exclude adversity that occurs during the first 3 years of life. Furthermore, many measures do not assess the most invisible form of maltreatment: deprivation and neglect (Perry, Reference Perry2002). Though an inherently challenging endeavor, researchers should go beyond self-report questionnaires onto developing and utilizing measures that assess early-life experience holistically.

10.1. Limitations

In the current review, authors sought to summarize and evaluate the state of knowledge on the etiology of psychopathic traits, by focusing attention on research into genetic, neuroanatomical, and neurotransmission activity correlates of psychopathic and antisocial traits. Inevitably, such a pursuit is fraught with challenges related to the differing conceptualizations and measurements of the psychopathy construct. While a broad view of psychopathic personality is useful for a first effort, it should likely be preceded with more narrow reviews and meta-analytic efforts. Yet it is uncertain that these, too, would yield a clearer answer for the question at hand. For instance, a review of a subset of studies using PCL instruments (46% of the studies included in this review) did not produce increased consistency of findings regarding genetic or neurobiological mechanisms. That is, utilizing Hare’s (Reference Hare2003) definition of psychopathy (to the exclusion of others) did not appear to lead to a better overall understanding of its etiology. It is possible that additional research exists that has not been included in this review (e.g., because it is yet to be published, or because it was unavailable at the time), and which may add support for some of the more pronounced findings. Furthermore, no statistical analysis was performed for the purpose of this review, and thus, it is possible that some findings, while inconsistent across studies, are nonetheless meaningful in the samples in which they were observed, or across several of these samples. Meta-analytic strategies to evaluate subsets of these findings (e.g., regarding genetic influences) can shed further light on these questions.

10.2. Implications and conclusions

While it is important to consider the current status of knowledge and ways to advance research into psychopathic traits, it is equally important that an integrated perspective inform current practices in management and treatment of psychopathic individuals. Specifically, clinicians and legal professionals should consider how yet-unsubstantiated theories about its etiology have affected both prognosis and treatment of psychopathy. It is likely that, in the absence of a conceptualization of psychopathy as a biological malady, clinicians would perceive both psychopathy treatment and its desired outcomes differently. This is particularly important since the belief in its innate, immutable nature has affected the use of the psychopathy construct in high-stakes legal situations (e.g., death penalty sentencing hearings; Cox et al., Reference Cox, Edens, Rulseh and Clark2016), with overwhelmingly negative outcomes.

Prognosis. Based on anecdotal evidence from case studies such as Cleckley’s (Reference Cleckley1941), along with hereditary research, psychopathy has largely been perceived as immutable and untreatable (Looman, Abracen, Serin, & Marquis, Reference Looman, Abracen, Serin and Marquis2005; Skeem, Monahan, & Mulvey, Reference Skeem, Monahan and Mulvey2002). Yet research demonstrates that, even without treatment, psychopathic traits and temperamental qualities are not stable over time (Hawes et al., Reference Hawes, Mulvey, Schubert and Pardini2014; Hopwood et al., Reference Hopwood, Morey, Donnellan, Samuel, Grilo, McGlashan and Skodol2013; Josefsson et al., Reference Josefsson, Jokela, Cloninger, Hintsanen, Salo, Hintsa and Keltikangas-Järvinen2013; Kandler, Riemann, & Angleitner, Reference Kandler, Riemann and Angleitner2013; Laceulle, Ormel, Vollebergh, Aken, & Nederhof, Reference Laceulle, Ormel, Vollebergh, Aken and Nederhof2014). Even in actuarial recidivism risk assessment, considerations for the passage of time and aging have been recommended (Harris & Rice, Reference Harris and Rice2007). Given associations between psychopathy and early-life stress, it is notable that the idea of change has been even more pronounced across traumatic stress literature. Perry (Reference Perry2002) found that in following chronically neglected children over time, recovery of brain structure and function over a year period was inversely proportional to age of removal from the neglectful environment, noting that even the oldest children made significant gains over time. Is it then possible that psychopathy is not as immutable as many believe? To assess this, it is worthwhile to consider common treatment strategies.

Treatment. Many of the treatment paradigms targeting psychopathy are based in correctional settings, with the expressed goal of increasing remorse and reducing criminal or violent behavior (Harris & Rice, Reference Harris, Rice and Patrick2006; Looman et al., Reference Looman, Abracen, Serin and Marquis2005). Yet few clinicians would recommend a similar treatment for trauma-affected populations; aggressive or violent behavior in this context is typically seen as one of many traumatic stress symptoms to be addressed in treatment, and self-reproachfulness is often explicitly discouraged (Ford, Chapman, Connor, & Cruise, Reference Ford, Chapman, Connor and Cruise2012). Further, trauma treatment commonly involves characteristics that are diametrically opposed to those seen in offender treatment. If clinicians treating individuals with psychopathic traits were to borrow from the complex trauma literature (e.g., Perry, Reference Perry2009), they would recognize the possibility that many attempts at psychopathy treatment have not worked because of their failure to include the necessary elements for neurodevelopmental change: willing participation in consistent, patterned interaction with a caring, trusted person.

11. Conclusion

For several decades, research has focused intensely on identifying the biological underpinnings of psychopathy, under the assumption that this construct represented a unification between life-course stable, immutable traits. Yet despite the passage of much time, a significant investment, and substantial technological advances in measurements of biological differences, few consistent conclusions can be made from this research. Indeed, some differences in neurological structure and activation patterns have been observed, but these leave the research community no closer to identifying an etiology for the construct. Practitioners, nonetheless, have eagerly adopted a biologically deterministic perspective on psychopathy, utilizing research measures to determine the fates of individuals accused of crimes (including children; for an example see Rockett, Murrie, & Boccaccini, Reference Rockett, Murrie and Boccaccini2007), and to make decisions regarding treatment procedures and outcomes (Archer, Buffington-Vollum, Stredny, & Handel, Reference Archer, Buffington-Vollum, Stredny and Handel2006; Odgers, Reppucci, & Moretti, Reference Odgers, Reppucci and Moretti2005; Salekin, Reference Salekin2002). This, in turn, could lead more individuals to experience the kind of treatment that may only serve to perpetuate the lack of remorse, empathy, and human connection presumed to underlie psychopathy. Ultimately, even the staunchest supporters of the biological hypothesis can agree that environmental influences cannot be excluded from a complete understanding of psychopathy. It is critical that in both research and practice, the influence of environment be evaluated alongside other factors affecting the development of psychopathic traits. Hence, future research should provide potential to evaluate neurodevelopmental change (whether through epigenetic mechanisms or neural plasticity) and predictors thereof, since these will likely serve to inform both prevention and intervention efforts targeting psychopathic individuals.

Conflict of interest

The author and coauthors have no conflicts of interest to disclose in relation to this manuscript.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. http://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.893619.Google Scholar
Anda, R. F., Whitfield, C. L., Felitti, V. J., Chapman, D., Edwards, V. J., Dube, S. R., & Williamson, D. F. (2002). Adverse childhood experiences, alcoholic parents, and later risk of alcoholism and depression. Psychiatric Services, 53, 10011009. http://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.53.8.1001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antelman, S. M., & Caggiula, A. R. (1977). Norepinephrine-dopamine interactions and behavior. Science, 195, 646653. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.841304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Archer, R. P., Buffington-Vollum, J. K., Stredny, R. V., & Handel, R. W. (2006). A survey of psychological test use patterns among forensic psychologists. Journal of Personality Assessment, 8, 8494. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_07.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auty, K. M., Farrington, D. P., & Coid, J. W. (2015). Intergenerational transmission of psychopathy and mediation via psychosocial risk factors. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 206, 2631. http://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.114.151050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkataki, I., Kumari, V., Das, M., Taylor, P., & Sharma, T. (2006). Volumetric structural brain abnormalities in men with schizophrenia or antisocial personality disorder. Behavioural Brain Research, 169, 239247. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2006.01.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barry, C. T., Frick, P. J., DeShazo, T. M., McCoy, M., Ellis, M., & Loney, B. R. (2000). The importance of callous–unemotional traits for extending the concept of psychopathy to children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 335340. http://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.109.2.335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basoglu, C., Oner, O., Ates, A., Algul, A., Bez, Y., Cetin, M., … Munir, K. M. (2011). Synaptosomal-associated protein 25 gene polymorphisms and antisocial personality disorder: Association with temperament and psychopathy. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 341347. http://doi.org/10.1177/070674371105600605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beach, S. R., Brody, G. H., Todorov, A. A., Gunter, T. D., & Philibert, R. A. (2010). Methylation at SLC6A4 is linked to family history of child abuse: An examination of the Iowa Adoptee sample. American Journal of Medical Genetics: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 153, 710713. http://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.31028.Google Scholar
Beaver, K. M., Wright, J. P., Boutwell, B. B., Barnes, J. C., DeLisi, M., & Vaughn, M. G. (2013). Exploring the association between the 2-repeat allele of the MAOA gene promoter polymorphism and psychopathic personality traits, arrests, incarceration, and lifetime antisocial behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 164168. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.08.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birbaumer, N., Veit, R., Lotze, M., Erb, M., Hermann, C., Grodd, W., & Flor, H. (2005). Deficient fear conditioning in psychopathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 799805. http://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.7.799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2001). Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders, and psychopathy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 71, 727731. http://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.71.6.727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2003). Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 57. http://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.182.1.5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2006). The emergence of psychopathy: Implications for the neuropsychological approach to developmental disorders. Cognition, 101, 414442. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boccardi, M., Frisoni, G. B., Hare, R. D., Cavedo, E., Najt, P., Pievani, M., … Vaurio, O. (2011). Cortex and amygdala morphology in psychopathy. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 193, 8592. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.12.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J. (1944). Forty-four juvenile thieves: Their character and home-life. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 25, 1953.Google Scholar
Boxer, P., Middlemass, K., & Delorenzo, T. (2009). Exposure to violent crime during incarceration: Effects on psychological adjustment following release. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36, 793807. http://doi.org/10.1177/0093854809336453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brinkley, C. A., Newman, J. P., Widiger, T. A., & Lynam, D. R. (2004). Two approaches to parsing the heterogeneity of psychopathy. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 6994. http://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bph054.Google Scholar
Buckholtz, J. W., Treadway, M. T., Cowan, R. L., Woodward, N. D., Benning, S. D., Li, R., … Smith, C. E. (2010). Mesolimbic dopamine reward system hypersensitivity in individuals with psychopathic traits. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 419421. http://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bukstel, L. H., & Kilmann, P. R. (1980). Psychological effects of imprisonment on confined individuals. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 469493. http://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.88.2.469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burchard, E. G., Ziv, E., Coyle, N., Gomez, S. L., Tang, H., Karter, A. J., … Risch, N. (2003). The importance of race and ethnic background in biomedical research and clinical practice. Heart Failure, 347, 11961199. http://doi.org/10.1056/nejmsb025007.Google Scholar
Carrillo, M., Ricci, L. A., Coppersmith, G. A., & Melloni, R. H. (2009). The effect of increased serotonergic neurotransmission on aggression: A critical meta-analytical review of preclinical studies. Psychopharmacology, 205, 349368. http://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-009-1543-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cases, O., Seif, I., Grimsby, J., Gaspar, P., Chen, K., Pournin, S., … De Maeyer, E. (1995). Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin and norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA. Science, 268, 17631766. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.7792602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., … Poulton, R. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297, 851854. http://doi.org/10.4324/9781315096278-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, D. P., Dube, S. R., & Anda, R. F. (2007). Adverse childhood events as risk factors for negative mental health outcomes. Psychiatric Annals, 37, 359364.Google Scholar
Chen, T. J., Blum, K., Mathews, D., Fisher, L., Schnautz, N., Braverman, E. R., … Comings, D. E. (2005). Are dopaminergic genes involved in a predisposition to pathological aggression?: Hypothesizing the importance of “super normal controls” in psychiatricgenetic research of complex behavioral disorders. Medical Hypotheses, 65, 703707. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2005.04.037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 597600. http://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579400007318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Thibodeau, E. L. (2012). The effects of child maltreatment on early signs of antisocial behavior: Genetic moderation by tryptophan hydroxylase, serotonin transporter, and monoamine oxidase A genes. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 907928. http://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579412000442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cleckley, H. (1941). The mask of sanity; an attempt to reinterpret the so-called psychopathic personality. Oxford, UK: Mosby.Google Scholar
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2013). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Routledge. http://doi.org/10.4324/9780203774441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, J., Edens, J. F., Rulseh, A., & Clark, J. W. (2016). Juror perceptions of the interpersonal-affective traits of psychopathy predict sentence severity in a white-collar criminal case. Psychology, Crime & Law, 22, 721740. http://doi.org/10.1080/1068316x.2016.1174864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
da Cunha-Bang, S., Hjordt, L. V., Perfalk, E., Beliveau, V., Bock, C., Lehel, S., … Knudsen, G. M. (2016). Serotonin 1B receptor binding is associated with trait anger and level of psychopathy in violent offenders. Biological Psychiatry, 82, 267274. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.02.030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dadds, M. R., Moul, C., Cauchi, A., Dobson-Stone, C., Hawes, D. J., Brennan, J., & Ebstein, R. E. (2014). Methylation of the oxytocin receptor gene and oxytocin blood levels in the development of psychopathy. Development and Psychopathology, 26, 3340. http://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579413000497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dannlowski, U., Stuhrmann, A., Beutelmann, V., Zwanzger, P., Lenzen, T., Grotegerd, D., … Lindner, C. (2012). Limbic scars: Long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 71, 286293. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Boer, S. F., & Koolhaas, J. M. (2005). 5-HT 1A and 5-HT 1B receptor agonists and aggression: A pharmacological challenge of the serotonin deficiency hypothesis. European Journal of Pharmacology, 526, 125139. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.09.065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Oliveira-Souza, R., Hare, R. D., Bramati, I. E., Garrido, G. J., Ignácio, F. A., Tovar-Moll, F., & Moll, J. (2008). Psychopathy as a disorder of the moral brain: Fronto-temporo-limbic grey matter reductions demonstrated by voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage, 40, 12021213. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deacon, B. J. (2013). The biomedical model of mental disorder: A critical analysis of its validity, utility, and effects on psychotherapy research. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 846861. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deault, L. C. (2010). A systematic review of parenting in relation to the development of comorbidities and functional impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 41, 168192. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-009-0159-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolan, M. C., & Anderson, I. M. (2003). The relationship between serotonergic function and the psychopathy checklist: Screening version. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 17, 216222. http://doi.org/10.1177/0269881103017002011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dube, S. R., Felitti, V. J., Dong, M., Chapman, D. P., Giles, W. H., & Anda, R. F. (2003). Childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction and the risk of illicit drug use: The adverse childhood experiences study. Pediatrics, 111, 564572. http://doi.org/10.1542/peds.111.3.564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DuPaul, G. J., McGoey, K. E., Eckert, T. L., & VanBrakle, J. (2001). Preschool children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Impairments in behavioral, social, and school functioning. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 508515. http://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200105000-00009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edens, J. F., Davis, K. M., Fernandez Smith, K., & Guy, L. S. (2013). No sympathy for the devil: Attributing psychopathic traits to capital murderers also predicts support for executing them. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 4, 175181. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0026442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edens, J. F., & Vincent, G. M. (2008). Juvenile psychopathy: A clinical construct in need of restraint?. Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 8, 186197. http://doi.org/10.1080/15228930801964042.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ermer, E., Cope, L. M., Nyalakanti, P. K., Calhoun, V. D., & Kiehl, K. A. (2012). Aberrant paralimbic gray matter in criminal psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 649658. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0026371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Essex, M. J., Thomas Boyce, W., Hertzman, C., Lam, L. L., Armstrong, J. M., Neumann, S., & Kobor, M. S. (2013). Epigenetic vestiges of early developmental adversity: Childhood stress exposure and DNA methylation in adolescence. Child Development, 84, 5875. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01641.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fanning, J. R., Berman, M. E., Guillot, C. R., Marsic, A., & McCloskey, M. S. (2014). Serotonin (5-HT) augmentation reduces provoked aggression associated with primary psychopathy traits. Journal of Personality Disorders, 28, 449461. http://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2012_26_065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, C. J. (2010). Genetic contributions to antisocial personality and behavior: A meta-analytic review from an evolutionary perspective. The Journal of Social Psychology, 150, 160180. http://doi.org/10.1080/00224540903366503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, C. J., & Beaver, K. M. (2009). Natural born killers: The genetic origins of extreme violence. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14, 286294. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2009.03.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Follan, M., & Minnis, H. (2010). Forty-four juvenile thieves revisited: From Bowlby to reactive attachment disorder. Child: Care, Health and Development, 36, 639645. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2214.2009.01048.x.Google ScholarPubMed
Ford, J. D., Chapman, J., Connor, D. F., & Cruise, K. R. (2012). Complex trauma and aggression in secure juvenile justice settings. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 39, 694724. http://doi.org/10.1177/0093854812436957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, T., Langley, K., Rice, F., van den Bree, M. B., Ross, K., Wilkinson, L. S., … Thapar, A. (2009). Psychopathy trait scores in adolescents with childhood ADHD: The contribution of genotypes affecting MAOA, 5HTT and COMT activity. Psychiatric Genetics, 19, 312319. http://doi.org/10.1097/ypg.0b013e3283328df4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
France, C. M., Lysaker, P. H., & Robinson, R. P. (2007). The “chemical imbalance” explanation for depression: Origins, lay endorsement, and clinical implications. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 38, 411420. http://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.38.4.411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frick, P. J. (2004). Integrating research on temperament and childhood psychopathology: Its pitfalls and promise. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 27. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3301_1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frodi, A., Dernevik, M., Sepa, A., Philipson, J., & Bragesjö, M. (2001). Current attachment representations of incarcerated offenders varying in degree of psychopathy. Attachment & Human Development, 3, 269283. http://doi.org/10.1080/14616730110096889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia, L. F., Aluja, A., Fibla, J., Cuevas, L., & García, O. (2010). Incremental effect for antisocial personality disorder genetic risk combining 5-HTTLPR and 5-HTTVNTR polymorphisms. Psychiatry Research, 177, 161166. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.12.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerhardt, S. (2006). Why love matters: How affection shapes a baby’s brain. Infant Observation, 9, 305309. http://doi.org/10.1080/13698030601074476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerra, G., Zaimovic, A., Moi, G., Bussandri, M., Delsignore, R., Caccavari, R., & Brambilla, F. (2003). Neuroendocrine correlates of antisocial personality disorder in abstinent heroin-dependent subjects. Addiction Biology, 8, 2332. http://doi.org/10.1080/1355621031000069846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillett, G., & Tamatea, A. J. (2012). The warrior gene: Epigenetic considerations. New Genetics and Society, 31, 4153. http://doi.org/10.1080/14636778.2011.597982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenn, A. L., & Raine, A. (2008). The neurobiology of psychopathy. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 31, 463475. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2008.03.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, M., & New, A. (2000). Impulsive aggression in borderline personality disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 2, 5661. http://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-000-0043-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goulter, N., Kimonis, E. R., Denson, T. F., & Begg, D. P. (2019). Female primary and secondary psychopathic variants show distinct endocrine and psychophysiological profiles. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 104, 717. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.02.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunter, T. D., Vaughn, M. G., & Philibert, R. A. (2010). Behavioral genetics in antisocial spectrum disorders and psychopathy: A review of the recent literature. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 28, 148173. http://doi.org/10.1002/bsl.923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallikainen, T., Saito, T., Lachman, H. M., Volavka, J., Pohjalainen, T., Ryynanen, O. P., … Tiihonen, J. (1999). Association between low activity serotonin transporter promoter genotype and early onset alcoholism with habitual impulsive violent behavior. Molecular Psychiatry, 4, 385389. http://doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4000526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hane, A. A., & Fox, N. A. (2006). Ordinary variations in maternal caregiving influence human infants’ stress reactivity. Psychological Science, 17, 550556. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01742.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haney, C. (2003). The psychological impact of incarceration: Implications for post-prison adjustment. Paper prepared for the “From Prison to Home” Conference, January 30–31, 2002. Retrieved from http://webarchive.urban.org/UploadedPDF/410624_PyschologicalImpact.pdf Google Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-revised Manual. Multi-Health Systems, Incorporated.Google Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1996). Psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder: A case of diagnostic confusion. Psychiatric Times, 13, 3940.Google Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1999). Without conscience: The disturbing world of the psychopaths among us. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hare, R. D. (2003). Manual for the Revised Psychopathy Checklist (2nd ed.). Cheektowaga, NY: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Hare, R. D., & Neumann, C. S. (2008). Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct. Annual Review in Clinical Psychology, 4, 217246. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, G. T., & Rice, M. E. (2006). Treatment of psychopathy. In Patrick, C. J. (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 555572). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Harris, G. T., & Rice, M. E. (2007). Adjusting actuarial violence risk assessments based on aging or the passage of time. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 34, 297313. http://doi.org/10.1177/0093854806293486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, S. D., Cox, D. N., & Hare, R. D. (1995). Hare Psychopathy Checklist: Screening version (PCL: SV). Cheektowaga, NY: Multi-Heath Systems.Google Scholar
Hawes, S. W., Mulvey, E. P., Schubert, C. A., & Pardini, D. A. (2014). Structural coherence and temporal stability of psychopathic personality features during emerging adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123, 623633. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0037078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoenicka, J., Ponce, G., Jiménez-Arriero, M. A., Ampuero, I., Rodríguez-Jiménez, R., Rubio, G., … Palomo, T. (2007). Association in alcoholic patients between psychopathic traits and the additive effect of allelic forms of the CNR1 and FAAH endocannabinoid genes, and the 3′ Region of the DRD2 gene. Neurotoxicity Research, 11, 5159. http://doi.org/10.1007/bf03033482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopwood, C. J., Morey, L. C., Donnellan, M. B., Samuel, D. B., Grilo, C. M., McGlashan, T. H., … Skodol, A. E. (2013). Ten-year rank-order stability of personality traits and disorders in a clinical sample. Journal of Personality, 81, 335344. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00801.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunter, P. (2010). The psycho gene. EMBO Reports, 11, 667669. http://doi.org/10.1038/embor.2010.122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyde, L. W., Byrd, A. L., Votruba-Drzal, E., Hariri, A. R., & Manuck, S. B. (2014). Amygdala reactivity and negative emotionality: Divergent correlates of antisocial personality and psychopathy traits in a community sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123, 214224. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0035467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, A. P., Laurens, K. R., Herba, C. M., Barker, G. J., & Viding, E. (2009). Amygdala hypoactivity to fearful faces in boys with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 95102. http://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07071050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Josefsson, K., Jokela, M., Cloninger, C. R., Hintsanen, M., Salo, J., Hintsa, T., … Keltikangas-Järvinen, L. (2013). Maturity and change in personality: Developmental trends of temperament and character in adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 713727. http://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579413000126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kandler, C., Riemann, R., & Angleitner, A. (2013). Patterns and sources of continuity and change of energetic and temporal aspects of temperament in adulthood: A longitudinal twin study of self-and peer reports. Developmental Psychology, 49, 17391753. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0030744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., Hare, R. D., & Liddle, P. F. (2004). Temporal lobe abnormalities in semantic processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 130, 2742. http://doi.org/10.1016/s0925-4927(03)00106-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Williams, B., Newcombe, R., Craig, I. W., & Moffitt, T. E. (2006). MAOA, maltreatment, and gene-environment interaction predicting children’s mental health: New evidence and a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 903913. http://doi.org/10.1037/e552512012-071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimonis, E. R., Fanti, K. A., Goulter, N., & Hall, J. (2017). Affective startle potentiation differentiates primary and secondary variants of juvenile psychopathy. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 11491160. http://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579416001206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkman, C. A. (2008). Psychopathy: A confusing clinical construct. Journal of Forensic Nursing, 4, 2939. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-3938.2008.00004.x.Google ScholarPubMed
Kolla, N. J., Matthews, B., Wilson, A. A., Houle, S., Bagby, R. M., Links, P., … Meyer, J. H. (2015). Lower monoamine oxidase-a total distribution volume in impulsive and violent male offenders with antisocial personality disorder and high psychopathic traits: An [11C] harmine positron emission tomography study. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40, 25962603. http://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krischer, M. K., & Sevecke, K. (2008). Early traumatization and psychopathy in female and male juvenile offenders. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 31, 253262. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2008.04.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laakso, M. P., Vaurio, O., Koivisto, E., Savolainen, L., Eronen, M., Aronen, H. J., … Tiihonen, J. (2001). Psychopathy and the posterior hippocampus. Behavioural Brain Research, 118, 187193. http://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00324-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laceulle, O. M., Ormel, J., Vollebergh, W. A., Aken, M. A., & Nederhof, E. (2014). A test of the vulnerability model: Temperament and temperament change as predictors of future mental disorders – the TRAILS study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 227236. http://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, H., Viding, E., & Plomin, R. (2008). Callous—unemotional traits and antisocial behavior: Genetic, environmental, and early parenting characteristics. Criminal Justice and Behavior , 35, 197211. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854807310225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lesch, K. P., & Merschdorf, U. (2000). Impulsivity, aggression, and serotonin: A molecular psychobiological perspective. Behavioral Sciences & The Law, 18, 581604. http://doi.org/10.1002/1099-0798(200010)18:5<581:aid-bsl411>3.3.co;2-c.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loeber, R., Byrd, A. L., & Farrington, D. P. (2015). Why developmental criminology is still coming of age: The influence of biological factors on within-individual change. In Morizot, J. & Kazemian, L. (Eds.), The development of criminal and antisocial behavior (pp. 6573). Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-08720-7.Google Scholar
Longato-Stadler, E., af Klinteberg, B., Garpenstrand, H., Oreland, L., & Hallman, J. (2002). Personality traits and platelet monoamine oxidase activity in a Swedish male criminal population. Neuropsychobiology, 46, 202208. http://doi.org/10.1159/000067806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Looman, J., Abracen, J., Serin, R., & Marquis, P. (2005). Psychopathy, treatment change, and recidivism in high-risk, high-need sexual offenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20, 549568. http://doi.org/10.1177/0886260504271583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorber, M. F. (2004). Psychophysiology of aggression, psychopathy, and conduct problems: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 531552. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, R. B., Lin, W. W., Lee, J. F., Ko, H. C., & Shih, J. C. (2003). Neither antisocial personality disorder nor antisocial alcoholism is associated with the MAO-A gene in Han Chinese males. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 27, 889893. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2003.tb04412.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maheu, F. S., Dozier, M., Guyer, A. E., Mandell, D., Peloso, E., Poeth, K., … Ernst, M. (2010). A preliminary study of medial temporal lobe function in youths with a history of caregiver deprivation and emotional neglect. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 3449. http://doi.org/10.3758/cabn.10.1.34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manuck, S. B., & McCaffery, J. M. (2014). Gene-environment interaction. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 4170. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, A. A., Finger, E. C., Fowler, K. A., Adalio, C. J., Jurkowitz, I. T., Schechter, J. C., … Blair, R. J. R. (2013). Empathic responsiveness in amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex in youths with psychopathic traits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 900910. http://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, A. A., Finger, E. C., Fowler, K. A., Jurkowitz, I. T., Schechter, J. C., Henry, H. Y., … Blair, R. J. R. (2011). Reduced amygdala–orbitofrontal connectivity during moral judgments in youths with disruptive behavior disorders and psychopathic traits. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 194, 279286. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.07.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, L. A., & Cooke, D. J. (1999). The childhood experiences of psychopaths: A retrospective study of familial and societal factors. Journal of Personality Disorders, 13, 211225. http://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.1999.13.3.211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDade, T. W., Ryan, C., Jones, M. J., MacIsaac, J. L., Morin, A. M., Meyer, J. M., … Kuzawa, C. W. (2017). Social and physical environments early in development predict DNA methylation of inflammatory genes in young adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 76117616. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1620661114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehta, M. A., Golembo, N. I., Nosarti, C., Colvert, E., Mota, A., Williams, S. C., … Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2009). Amygdala, hippocampal and corpus callosum size following severe early institutional deprivation: The English and Romanian Adoptees study pilot. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 943951. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02084.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, D. G., Colledge, E., Leonard, A., & Blair, R. J. R. (2002). Risky decisions and response reversal: Is there evidence of orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in psychopathic individuals?. Neuropsychologia, 40, 20132022. http://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00056-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mokros, A., Neumann, C. S., Stadtland, C., Osterheider, M., Nedopil, N., & Hare, R. D. (2011). Assessing measurement invariance of PCL-R assessments from file reviews of North American and German offenders. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 34, 5663. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2010.11.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, T. M., Scarpa, A., & Raine, A. (2002). A meta-analysis of serotonin metabolite 5-HIAA and antisocial behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 28, 299316. http://doi.org/10.1002/ab.90027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moss, K., & Prins, H. (2006). Severe (psychopathic) personality disorder: A review. Medicine, Science and the Law, 46, 190207. http://doi.org/10.1258/rsmmsl.46.3.190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moul, C., Dobson-Stone, C., Brennan, J., Hawes, D., & Dadds, M. (2013). An exploration of the serotonin system in antisocial boys with high levels of callous-unemotional traits. PloS One, 8, e56619. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0056619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller, J. L., Sommer, M., Wagner, V., Lange, K., Taschler, H., Röder, C. H., … Hajak, G. (2003). Abnormalities in emotion processing within cortical and subcortical regions in criminal psychopaths: Evidence from a functional magnetic resonance imaging study using pictures with emotional content. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 152162. http://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01749-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, J. P., MacCoon, D. G., Vaughn, L. J., & Sadeh, N. (2005). Validating a distinction between primary and secondary psychopathy with measures of Gray’s BIS and BAS constructs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 319. http://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.114.2.319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Odgers, C. L., Reppucci, N. D., & Moretti, M. M. (2005). Nipping psychopathy in the bud: An examination of the convergent, predictive, and theoretical utility of the PCL-YV among adolescent girls. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 23, 743763. http://doi.org/10.1002/bsl.664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogloff, J. R. (2006). Psychopathy/antisocial personality disorder conundrum. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40, 519528. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1614.2006.01834.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pardini, D. A., Raine, A., Erickson, K., & Loeber, R. (2014). Lower amygdala volume in men is associated with childhood aggression, early psychopathic traits, and future violence. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 7380. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.04.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patrick, C. J., & Brislin, S. J. (2014). Antisocial personality disorder/psychopathy. In Cautin, R. L. & Lilienfeld, S. O. (Eds.), The encyclopedia of clinical psychology (pp. 174183). New York: Wiley-Blackwell. http://doi.org/10.1002/9781118625392.wbecp475.Google Scholar
Paulhus, D. L., Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Tracy, J. L. (2004). Two replicable suppressor situations in personality research. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 303328. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry, B. D. (1994). Neurobiological sequelae of childhood trauma: PTSD in children. In Murberg, M. (Ed.), Catecholamine function in posttraumatic stress disorder: Emerging concepts (pp. 233255). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Press.Google Scholar
Perry, B. D. (1999). The neuroarcheology of child maltreatment: Evidence for altered neurodevelopment following traumatic abuse. In Proceedings of the American Psychiatric Association: Volume 38E. Symposium on the biology of trauma (p. 88). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Perry, B. D. (2002). Childhood experience and the expression of genetic potential: What childhood neglect tells us about nature and nurture. Brain and Mind, 3, 79100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, B. D. (2009). Examining child maltreatment through a neurodevelopmental lens: Clinical applications of the neurosequential model of therapeutics. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 14, 240255. http://doi.org/10.1080/15325020903004350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, B. D., Pollard, R. A., Blakley, T. L., Baker, W. L., & Vigilante, D. (1995). Childhood trauma, the neurobiology of adaptation, and use dependent development of the brain: How states become traits. Infant Mental Health Journal, 16, 271291. http://doi.org/10.1002/1097-0355(199524)16:4<271:aid-imhj2280160404>3.0.co;2-b.3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, B. D., & Pollard, R. (1997). Altered brain development following global neglect in early childhood. Society for Neuroscience: Proceedings from Annual Meeting, New Orleans, LA, USA.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., Warren, K. L., & Short, M. M. (2011). Infantile amnesia across the years: A 2-year follow-up of children’s earliest memories. Child Development, 82, 10921105. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01597.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phan, K. L., Britton, J. C., Taylor, S. F., Fig, L. M., & Liberzon, I. (2006). Corticolimbic blood flow during nontraumatic emotional processing in posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 184192. http://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.63.2.184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piquero, A. R., Farrington, D. P., Fontaine, N. M., Vincent, G., Coid, J., & Ullrich, S. (2012). Childhood risk, offending trajectories, and psychopathy at age 48 years in the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 18, 577598. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0027061.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ponce, G., Hoenicka, J., Jimenez-Arriero, M. A., Rodriguez-Jimenez, R., Aragüés, M., Martin-Sune, N., … Palomo, T. (2008). DRD2 and ANKK1 genotype in alcohol-dependent patients with psychopathic traits: Association and interaction study. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 193, 121125. http://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.107.041582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porter, S., & Woodworth, M. (2006). Psychopathy and aggression. In Patrick, C. J. (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 481494). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Portnoy, J., & Farrington, D. P. (2015). Resting heart rate and antisocial behavior: An updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 22, 3345. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2015.02.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pruessner, J. C., Champagne, F., Meaney, M. J., & Dagher, A. (2004). Dopamine release in response to a psychological stress in humans and its relationship to early life maternal care: A positron emission tomography study using [11C] raclopride. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 28252831. http://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3422-03.2004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putkonen, H., Weizmann-Henelius, G., Repo-Tiihonen, E., Lindberg, N., Saarela, T., Eronen, M., & Häkkänen-Nyholm, H. (2010). Homicide, psychopathy, and agingA nationwide register-based case-comparison study of homicide offenders aged 60 years or older. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 55, 15521556. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1556-4029.2010.01488.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radua, J., Borgwardt, S., Crescini, A., Mataix-Cols, D., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., McGuire, P. K., & Fusar-Poli, P. (2012). Multimodal meta-analysis of structural and functional brain changes in first episode psychosis and the effects of antipsychotic medication. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 23252333. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.07.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., LaCasse, L., & Colletti, P. (2000). Reduced prefrontal gray matter volume and reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 119127. http://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.57.2.119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A., Lencz, T., Taylor, K., Hellige, J. B., Bihrle, S., Lacasse, L., … Colletti, P. (2003). Corpus callosum abnormalities in psychopathic antisocial individuals. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 11341142. http://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60.11.1134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A., Park, S., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., LaCasse, L., Widom, C. S., Al-Dayeh, L., … Singh, M., (2001). Reduced right hemisphere activation in severely abused violent offenders during a working memory task: An fMRI study. Aggressive Behavior, 27, 111129. http://doi.org/10.1002/ab.4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rautiainen, M. R., Paunio, T., Repo-Tiihonen, E., Virkkunen, M., Ollila, H. M., Sulkava, S., … Tiihonen, J. (2016). Genome-wide association study of antisocial personality disorder. Translational Psychiatry, 6, e883. http://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2016.155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rilling, J. K., Glenn, A. L., Jairam, M. R., Pagnoni, G., Goldsmith, D. R., Elfenbein, H. A., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Neural correlates of social cooperation and non-cooperation as a function of psychopathy. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 12601271. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rockett, J. L., Murrie, D. C., & Boccaccini, M. T. (2007). Diagnostic labeling in juvenile justice settings: Do psychopathy and conduct disorder findings influence clinicians? Psychological Services, 4, 107122. http://doi.org/10.1037/1541-1559.4.2.107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosell, D. R., Thompson, J. L., Slifstein, M., Xu, X., Frankle, W. G., New, A. S., … Siever, L. J. (2010). Increased serotonin 2A receptor availability in the orbitofrontal cortex of physically aggressive personality disordered patients. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 11541162. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.03.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadeh, N., Javdani, S., Jackson, J. J., Reynolds, E. K., Potenza, M. N., Gelernter, J., … Verona, E. (2010). Serotonin transporter gene associations with psychopathic traits in youth vary as a function of socioeconomic resources. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 604609. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0019709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadeh, N., Javdani, S., & Verona, E. (2013). Analysis of monoaminergic genes, childhood abuse, and dimensions of psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 167179. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0029866.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salekin, R. T. (2002). Psychopathy and therapeutic pessimism: Clinical lore or clinical reality? Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 79112. http://doi.org/10.4324/9781351161565-14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salekin, R. T., Rosenbaum, J., Lee, Z., & Lester, W. S. (2009). Child and adolescent psychopathy: Like a painting by Monet. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 7, 239255. http://doi.org/10.1177/1541204009333832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salekin, R. T., Worley, C., & Grimes, R. D. (2010). Treatment of psychopathy: A review and brief introduction to the mental model approach for psychopathy. Behavioral Sciences & The Law, 28, 235266. http://doi.org/10.1002/bsl.928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saltaris, C. (2002). Psychopathy in juvenile offenders: Can temperament and attachment be considered as robust developmental precursors? Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 729752. http://doi.org/10.1016/s0272-7358(01)00122-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheeringa, M. S., Zeanah, C. H., Myers, L., & Putnam, F. (2004). Heart period and variability findings in preschool children with posttraumatic stress symptoms. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 685691. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.01.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schore, A. N. (2005). Attachment, affect regulation, and the developing right brain: Linking developmental neuroscience to pediatrics. Pediatrics in Review, 26, 204217. http://doi.org/10.1542/pir.26-6-204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sethi, A., McCrory, E., Puetz, V., Hoffmann, F., Knodt, A. R., Radtke, S. R., … Viding, E. (2018). Primary and secondary variants of psychopathy in a volunteer sample are associated with different neurocognitive mechanisms. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3, 10131021. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.04.002.Google Scholar
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Harari, H., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Levkovitz, Y. (2010). The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in affective theory of mind deficits in criminal offenders with psychopathic tendencies. Cortex, 46, 668677. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.04.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shin, L. M., Orr, S. P., Carson, M. A., Rauch, S. L., Macklin, M. L., Lasko, N. B., … Alpert, N. M. (2004). Regional cerebral blood flow in the amygdala and medial prefrontalcortex during traumatic imagery in male and female Vietnam veterans with PTSD. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 168176. http://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.2.168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skeem, J. L., & Cooke, D. J. (2010). One measure does not a construct make: Directions toward reinvigorating psychopathy researchReply to Hare and Neumann (2010). Psychological Assessment, 22, 455459. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0014862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skeem, J. L., Monahan, J., & Mulvey, E. P. (2002). Psychopathy, treatment involvement, and subsequent violence among civil psychiatric patients. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 577. http://doi.org/10.1023/a:1020993916404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soderstrom, H., Blennow, K., Sjodin, A. K., & Forsman, A. (2003). New evidence for an association between the CSF HVA: 5-HIAA ratio and psychopathic traits. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 74, 918921. http://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.74.7.918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soderstrom, H., Hultin, L., Tullberg, M., Wikkelso, C., Ekholm, S., & Forsman, A. (2002). Reduced frontotemporal perfusion in psychopathic personality. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 114, 8194. http://doi.org/10.1016/s0925-4927(02)00006-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanley, B., Molcho, A., Stanley, M., Winchel, R., Gameroff, M. J., Parsons, B., & Mann, J. J. (2000). Association of aggressive behavior with altered serotonergic function in patients who are not suicidal. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 609614. http://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.4.609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strathearn, L. (2011). Maternal neglect: Oxytocin, dopamine and the neurobiology of attachment. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 23, 10541065. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2826.2011.02228.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swartz, J. R., Williamson, D. E., & Hariri, A. R. (2015). Developmental change in amygdala reactivity during adolescence: Effects of family history of depression and stressful life events. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 276283. http://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14020195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H., & Samson, J. A. (2016). Annual research review: Enduring neurobiological effects of childhood abuse and neglect. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 241266. http://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, S. A., & Palmiter, R. D. (1997). Impaired maternal behavior in mice lacking norepinephrine and epinephrine. Cell, 91, 583592. http://doi.org/10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80446-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van de Giessen, E., Rosell, D. R., Thompson, J. L., Xu, X., Girgis, R. R., Ehrlich, Y., … Siever, L. J. (2014). Serotonin transporter availability in impulsive aggressive personality disordered patients: A PET study with [11 C] DASB. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 58, 147154. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.07.025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaughn, M. G., Edens, J. F., Howard, M. O., & Smith, S. T. (2009). An investigation of primary and secondary psychopathy in a statewide sample of incarcerated youth. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 7, 172188. http://doi.org/10.1177/1541204009333792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vidal, S., & Skeem, J. L. (2007). Effect of psychopathy, abuse, and ethnicity on juvenile probation officers’ decision-making and supervision strategies. Law and Human Behavior, 31, 479498. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-006-9077-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viding, E., Blair, R. J. R., Moffitt, T. E., & Plomin, R. (2005). Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 592597. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00393.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vitacco, M. J., & Vincent, G. M. (2006). Understanding the downward extension of psychopathy to youth: Implications for risk assessment and juvenile justice. International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 5, 2938. http://doi.org/10.1080/14999013.2006.10471228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitale, J. E., Smith, S. S., Brinkley, C. A., & Newman, J. P. (2002). The reliability and validity of the Psychopathy Checklist–Revised in a sample of female offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 29, 202231. http://doi.org/10.1177/0093854802029002005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, M. G., Bogdan, R., Fisher, P. M., Munoz, K. E., Williamson, D. E., & Hariri, A. R. (2012a). FKBP5 and emotional neglect interact to predict individual differences in amygdala reactivity. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 11, 869878. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-183x.2012.00837.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, S. F., Marsh, A. A., Fowler, K. A., Schechter, J. C., Adalio, C., Pope, K., … Blair, R. J. R. (2012b). Reduced amygdala response in youths with disruptive behavior disorders and psychopathic traits: Decreased emotional response versus increased top-down attention to nonemotional features. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 750758. http://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11081270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, L. M., Gatt, J. M., Kuan, S. A., Dobson-Stone, C., Palmer, D. M., Paul, R. H., … Gordon, E. (2009). A polymorphism of the MAOA gene is associated with emotional brain markers and personality traits on an antisocial index. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34, 17971809. http://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wooldredge, J. D. (1999). Inmate experiences and psychological well-being. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 26, 235250. http://doi.org/10.1177/0093854899026002005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, T., & Barnes, J. C. (2013). Two dopamine receptor genes (DRD2 and DRD4) predict psychopathic personality traits in a sample of American adults. Journal of Criminal Justice, 41, 188195. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2013.02.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, Y., Raine, A., Colletti, P., Toga, A. W., & Narr, K. L. (2010). Morphological alterations in the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala in unsuccessful psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 546554. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0019611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, Y., Raine, A., Narr, K. L., Colletti, P., & Toga, A. W. (2009). Localization of deformations within the amygdala in individuals with psychopathy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 986994. http://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yildirim, B. O., & Derksen, J. J. (2013). Systematic review, structural analysis, and new theoretical perspectives on the role of serotonin and associated genes in the etiology of psychopathy and sociopathy. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 12541296. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.04.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, S. E., Smolen, A., Hewitt, J. K., Haberstick, B. C., Stallings, M. C., Corley, R. P., & Crowley, T. J. (2006). Interaction between MAO-A genotype and maltreatment in the risk for conduct disorder: Failure to confirm in adolescent patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 10191025. http://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.163.6.1019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Characteristics of the reviewed studies