Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T05:55:06.316Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Inward versus reward: white matter pathways in extraversion

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2019

R. Leshem*
Affiliation:
Department of Criminology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel
P. Paoletti
Affiliation:
Research Institute for Neuroscience, Education and Didactics, Patrizio Paoletti Foundation, Neuroscientific Research Unit, Assisi, Italy
C. Piervincenzi
Affiliation:
Departmental Faculty of Medicine and Surgery, Università “Campus Bio-Medico di Roma”, Rome, Italy
F. Carducci
Affiliation:
Departmental Faculty of Medicine and Surgery, Università “Campus Bio-Medico di Roma”, Rome, Italy
C. A. Mallio
Affiliation:
Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Neuroimaging Laboratory, Sapienza University, Rome, Italy
Y. Errante
Affiliation:
Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Neuroimaging Laboratory, Sapienza University, Rome, Italy
C. C. Quattrocchi
Affiliation:
Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Neuroimaging Laboratory, Sapienza University, Rome, Italy
T. Dotan Ben-Soussan
Affiliation:
Research Institute for Neuroscience, Education and Didactics, Patrizio Paoletti Foundation, Neuroscientific Research Unit, Assisi, Italy
*
Author for correspondence: R. Leshem, Email: rotem.leshem@biu.ac.il
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The trait of extraversion is one of the longest-standing domains that captures the social dimension of personality and can potentially explain the covariation of a wide variety of behaviors. To date, there is a growing recognition that human behavior should be specified not only through the psychological mechanisms underlying each trait but also through their underlying neurobehavioral systems. While imaging studies have revealed important initial insights into the structural and functional neural correlates of extraversion, current knowledge about the relationships between extraversion and brain structures is still rather limited, especially with regard to the relationship between extraversion and white matter (WM). In this study, we aimed to investigate WM microstructure in extraversion in greater depth. Thirty-five healthy volunteers (21 women; mean age 35) underwent magnetic resonance imaging, as a part of a larger project aimed at investigating the longitudinal effect of motor training. WM integrity was assessed using the diffusion tensor imaging technique combining multiple diffusion tensor measures. Extraversion was assessed by the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised. Voxelwise correlation analyses between fractional anisotropy, axial diffusivities, and radial diffusivities maps and extraversion score showed decreased connectivity in the right inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus and forceps major among individuals who had high extraversion ratings. In conclusion, individual differences in extraversion may reflect differential organization of the WM tracts connecting frontal cortex, temporal, and occipital areas, which are related to socioemotional and control functions.

Type
Empirical Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2019

Personality traits involve individual tendencies to express specific patterns of cognition, emotion, and behavior (Xu & Potenza, Reference Xu and Potenza2012). The attitudes people have toward their environments, as well as their dispositions, personal preferences, and dislikes, all help determine their everyday actions, subjective well-being, mental and physical health, quality of relationships with peers and family members, and occupational performance and satisfaction (Mitchell & Kumari, Reference Mitchell and Kumari2016; Xu & Potenza, Reference Xu and Potenza2012). A large body of evidence describes human personality with respect to five core dimensions: extraversion, neuroticism, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness/intellect (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Williams, Feczko, Barrett, Dickerson, Schwartz and Wedig2006).

The extraversion dimension, which involves individual differences in social behavior and socioemotional functioning, is considered one of the fundamental dimensions of personality (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992; Wilt & Revelle, Reference Wilt, Revelle, Leary and Hoyle2009). As such, extraversion can potentially explain the covariation of a wide variety of behaviors and predict risk for and resilience to different forms of psychopathology, such as affective disorders (Wilt & Revelle, Reference Wilt, Revelle, Leary and Hoyle2009; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Williams, Feczko, Barrett, Dickerson, Schwartz and Wedig2006) and addictive behaviors (Munafo & Black, Reference Munafo and Black2007; Trémeau et al., Reference Trémeau, Darreye, Leroy, Renckly, Ertle, Weibel and Macher2003). Up to half of the variability in extraversion is believed to be heritable, suggesting that interindividual differences in this trait have biological bases in the brain (Kanai & Rees, Reference Kanai and Rees2011). As such, in personality neuroscience, there is a growing recognition that human behavior should be specified not only through the psychological mechanisms underlying each trait but also through the neurobehavioral systems that are linked to those psychological mechanisms (Depue & Collins, Reference Depue and Collins1999; DeYoung et al., Reference DeYoung, Hirsh, Shane, Papademetris, Rajeevan and Gray2010; Mitchell & Kumari, Reference Mitchell and Kumari2016).

Hans Eysenck (Reference Eysenck1967) was one of the first investigators to explore and develop a neurobiological model for personality. His explanation, which attempted to define neurophysiological sources of personality, uniquely addressed two major personality dimensions, extraversion and neuroticism, in terms of individual differences in central nervous system functions (Eysenck, Reference Eysenck1967). According to Eysenck’s model, individuals who are high in extraversion have lower cortical arousal and therefore require external stimulation. Those who score low on measures of extraversion (i.e., introverts) have lower response thresholds and are consequently more cortically aroused than those with high extraversion scores. The arousal theory generated a wide range of studies, some of which showed moderate support at most for the relationship between extraversion and regional arousal (Gao et al., Reference Gao, Xu, Long, Duan, Liao, Ding and Chen2013; Küssner, Reference Küssner2017; Wilt & Revelle, Reference Wilt, Revelle, Leary and Hoyle2009), suggesting that this relationship is not as simple as originally proposed by Eysenck (for further discussion, see Zuckerman & Glicksohn, Reference Zuckerman and Glicksohn2016). Gray (Reference Gray1970) further examined the physiology of extraversion using an animal model. He argued that extraversion–introversion reflect motivational systems that evolved to facilitate adaptation to classes of stimuli associated with positive and negative reinforcement. In accordance, differences in extraversion have been shown to be related to structural and functional differences in brain structures implicated in emotion and motivation. The brain structures most often studied in relation to extraversion are the prefrontal cortex (PFC), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) (Depue & Collins, Reference Depue and Collins1999; DeYoung et al., Reference DeYoung, Hirsh, Shane, Papademetris, Rajeevan and Gray2010; Xu & Potenza, Reference Xu and Potenza2012), anterior cingulated cortex (ACC), parietal cortex, striatum, middle temporal gyrus (MTG), and amygdala (Canli, Reference Canli2004; Canli et al., Reference Canli, Zhao, Desmond, Kang, Gross and Gabrieli2001; Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Satpute, Reference Eisenberger, Lieberman and Satpute2005; Haas, Omura, Amin, Constable, & Canli, Reference Haas, Omura, Amin, Constable and Canli2006; Koelsch, Skouras, & Jentschke, Reference Koelsch, Skouras and Jentschke2013). These structures are all involved in socioemotional processes and have been tied specifically to the regulation of behavior and emotion (Leshem, Reference Leshem2016).

Functional connectivity studies at rest (in the absence of presented stimuli or task instructions) as well as task-based studies show anatomic variations in different brain areas, with a variety of both positive and negative correlations identified between extraversion and neuroanatomic measures in several regions. Some studies reported a positive association between extraversion and gray matter (GM) in the OFC and sub-cortical regions related to emotional processing (Cremers et al., Reference Cremers, van Tol, Roelofs, Aleman, Zitman, van Buchem and van der Wee2011; DeYoung et al., Reference DeYoung, Hirsh, Shane, Papademetris, Rajeevan and Gray2010), whereas others have found no significant correlation between extraversion and GM in the OFC and amygdala (Wright et al., Reference Wright, Williams, Feczko, Barrett, Dickerson, Schwartz and Wedig2006). Additional studies have reported negative associations with GM in frontal-subcortical circuits, which are known to contribute to cognitive control and emotional functions (e.g., ventrolateral PFC, superior/middle frontal gyrus, MTG, and amygdala) (Blankstein, Chen, Mincic, McGrath, & Davis, Reference Blankstein, Chen, Mincic, McGrath and Davis2009; Coutinho, Sampaio, Ferreira, Soares, & Gonçalves, Reference Coutinho, Sampaio, Ferreira, Soares and Gonçalves2013; Gao et al., Reference Gao, Xu, Long, Duan, Liao, Ding and Chen2013; Lu et al., Reference Lu, Huo, Li, Chen, Liu, Wang and Chen2014; Omura, Constable, & Canli, Reference Omura, Constable and Canli2005).

The inconsistencies between studies reflect the instability of results in personality neuroscience, in general, perhaps because the relevant biological data depend on responses to different situations and to individual differences in extraversion-related traits (see Zuckerman & Glicksohn, Reference Zuckerman and Glicksohn2016). Analysis methods are another potential source of variance (Privado et al., Reference Privado, Román, Saénz-Urturi, Burgaleta and Colom2017; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Williams, Feczko, Barrett, Dickerson, Schwartz and Wedig2006).

While the studies presented above have revealed important initial insights into the structural and functional neural correlates of extraversion, current knowledge about the relationships between extraversion and brain function and structures is still rather limited (Mitchell & Kumari, Reference Mitchell and Kumari2016), especially with regard to the relationship between extraversion and white matter (WM). WM mediates communications in the brain and is critical for the integrity of the brain functions underlying emotional and cognitive processes (Privado et al., Reference Privado, Román, Saénz-Urturi, Burgaleta and Colom2017; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Kober, Carroll, Rounsaville, Pearlson and Potenza2012).

The current study aimed to investigate the relationship between extraversion and WM integrity using the diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) technique. DTI assesses WM integrity by measuring directional water diffusion (Basser, Mattiello, & LeBihan, Reference Basser, Mattiello and LeBihan1994; Pierpaoli, Jezzard, Basser, Barnett, & DiChiro, Reference Pierpaoli, Jezzard, Basser, Barnett and DiChiro1996). The DTI technique is sensitive to the magnitude and orientation of water diffusion throughout brain tissue and exploits this information to calculate several diffusion parameters through a tensor model. The most commonly used DTI parameter is the fractional anisotropy (FA). FA value designates the degree of anisotropy (a measure of how highly oriented substructures are within a volume) of a diffusion process. However, FA may not be sufficient for the investigation of specific axonal density and diameter and/or myelination (Jones, Knösche, & Turner, Reference Jones, Knösche and Turner2013). FA values are thought to reflect WM integrity, as a result of greater intravoxel coherence of fiber orientation, axon density and diameter, and/or myelination (Beaulieu, Reference Beaulieu2002; Caminiti et al., Reference Caminiti, Carducci, Piervincenzi, Battaglia-Mayer, Confalone, Visco-Comandini and Innocenti2013; Sen & Basser, Reference Sen and Basser2005). Yet, many factors may affect water anisotropy and consequently the FA value of a given fiber (e.g., myelin, cell membrane, axonal diameter, and density), and each of these factors can influence axonal conduction velocity (Assaf & Pasternak, Reference Assaf and Pasternak2008; Tavor et al., Reference Tavor, Yablonski, Mezer, Rom, Assaf and Yovel2014). Thus, for example, different combinations of axial diffusivities (AD) and radial diffusivities (RD) can give rise to identical FA values. The relationship between AD and RD (degree of anisotropy) can be expressed by FA (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Knösche and Turner2013). As such, increasing axonal density, reducing axonal caliber, and increasing the degree of myelination should all lead to reduced RD and therefore elevated FA (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Knösche and Turner2013). Since AD is thought to reflect axonal integrity and RD seems to be sensitive to myelination, joint use of the two indices makes it possible to capture different aspects of WM microstructure separately and to provide additional important information regarding WM organization (Jiang et al., Reference Jiang, Shi, Liu, Li, Ding, Shen and Shen2017). In the current study, multiple diffusion tensor measures (i.e., FA, AD, and RD) were employed to understand WM microstructure in extraversion in greater depth.

Recently, DTI has been used to assess correlations between the WM tracts interconnecting different brain regions (e.g., hippocampus, amygdala, striatum, and PFC) and behaviors related to selected personality traits, such as novelty-seeking, reward dependence, and creativity (Cohen, Schoene-Bake, Elger, & Weber, Reference Cohen, Schoene-Bake, Elger and Weber2009; Jung, Grazioplene, Caprihan, Chavez, & Haier, Reference Jung, Grazioplene, Caprihan, Chavez and Haier2010; Takeuchi et al., Reference Takeuchi, Taki, Sassa, Hashizume, Sekiguchi, Fukushima and Kawashima2010; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Kober, Carroll, Rounsaville, Pearlson and Potenza2012). A study by Xu and Potenza (Reference Xu and Potenza2012) assessed the relationships between WM integrity and personality traits among 51 healthy participants using FA and MD DTI measures and the revised NEO Personality Inventory. The researchers found no correlations between extraversion scores and the integrity of the WM tracts interconnecting the PFC, parietal cortex, and striatum. Also, functional connectivity (resting-state and task-based fMRI) studies have found positive correlations between extraversion and functional connectivity between the ACC, inferior parietal lobe, and PFC (DeYoung et al., Reference DeYoung, Hirsh, Shane, Papademetris, Rajeevan and Gray2010; Haas et al., Reference Haas, Omura, Amin, Constable and Canli2006; Mobbs, Hagan, Azim, Menon, & Reiss, Reference Mobbs, Hagan, Azim, Menon and Reiss2005). These positive correlations reflect high activity within the reward circuitry in response to rewarding stimuli in cases of high trait extraversion (Aghajani et al., Reference Aghajani, Veer, Van Tol, Aleman, Van Buchem, Veltman and van der Wee2014). In their study, Privado et al. (Reference Privado, Román, Saénz-Urturi, Burgaleta and Colom2017) analyzed relationships of both GM and WM to individual differences in the personality traits designated by the Five-Factor Model. Their results showed significant associations between extraversion and occipital cortical surface area variations. They did not find significant correlations between extraversion and selected WM tracts connecting anterior with posterior regions, in both hemispheres, or frontal-temporal regions. In addition, Privado et al. found that the variations in discrete GM clusters were associated with temperamental traits (extraversion), whereas long-distance structural connections were related to openness, the personality dimension that has been associated with high-level cognitive processes.

To date, most of the MRI studies examining personality traits, including extraversion, have focused on measures of functional and structural connectivity that indicate increased/decreased involvement of brain regions associated with cognitive and emotional processing. To the best of our knowledge, little work has examined WM pathways in extraversion (e.g., Privado et al., Reference Privado, Román, Saénz-Urturi, Burgaleta and Colom2017; Xu & Potenza, Reference Xu and Potenza2012). In the present study, we aimed to further explore possible correlations between extraversion FA, AD, and RD. Given the core features of extraversion (DeYoung et al., Reference DeYoung, Hirsh, Shane, Papademetris, Rajeevan and Gray2010), we predicted that low FA/AD and high RD values in WM pathways that interconnect regions are associated with extraversion-related tendencies (e.g., sociability, novelty and sensation seeking, reward sensitivity, and positive emotion).

1. Materials and methods

1.1 Participants and design

The participants belong to a larger study comprising three time points (T0, T1, and T2, Piervincenzi et al., Reference Piervincenzi, Ben-Soussan, Mauro, Mallio, Errante, Quattrocchi and Carducci2017). We recruited 50 healthy volunteers. Following the inclusion and exclusion criteria (see Piervincenzi et al., Reference Piervincenzi, Ben-Soussan, Mauro, Mallio, Errante, Quattrocchi and Carducci2017), four participants were excluded due to the presence of WM lesion, three participants because they were left-handed, six participants because of MRI incomplete protocol, and two participants because of lack in complying motor exercise. Thus, the analyses were conducted in a group of 35 healthy right-handed participants (21 women and 14 men, mean age ± SD: 35 ± 5 and 36 ± 5 years, respectively). The correlation between FA, AD and RD and the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire subscales scores were conducted only at T2, after 12 weeks of Quadrato Motor Training (QMT). QMT is a whole-body mindful movement practice created by Patrizio Paoletti, in which the participant is asked to move within a 50 by 50 cm2.

1.2 Ethics statement

All procedures were explained to participants, verifying sufficient understanding, and written informed consent was obtained in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the ethical committee of the Università Campus Bio-Medico di Roma, Rome, Italy.

The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised (EPQ-R, Eysenck & Eysenck, Reference Eysenck and Eysenck1991). This is a 106-item forced-choice personality questionnaire. Here, we focused on three subscales, namely: Extraversion (E) subscale = 23 items (item example: “Do you have many friends?”); Psychoticism (P) subscale = 23 items (item example: “Do you enjoy to insult people who you love?”); and Neuroticism (N) subscale = 24 items (item example: “Have you often felt guilty?”); on which participants answered choosing between YES and NO. Reliability coefficients for the Extraversion and Neuroticism scales ranged from 0.85 to 0.90 (Eysenck & Eysenck, Reference Eysenck and Eysenck1991). The scale was translated from English to Italian by Dazzi (Reference Dazzi2011) with good reliability for E, P, and N (0.78, 0.67, and 0.85, respectively). In the current study, the alpha coefficients were .68, .67, and .79, respectively.

1.3 MRI data acquisition

Imaging data were acquired using a Siemens 1.5-T MAGNETOM Avanto (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) whole body scanner equipped with a 12-element designed Head Matrix coil, as part of the standard system configuration. Diffusion-weighted images (DWIs) were acquired using an axial pulsed-gradient spin-echo echo-planar sequence (7600/103; 38 sections; section thickness, 3.0 mm with no intersection gap), with diffusion-encoding gradients applied in 12 noncollinear directions (b factor 0 and 1000 sec/mm2; number of acquired signals, four). A 2D fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) T2-weighted scan was also used to exclude the presence of small vessel ischemic disease and other supra- or infra-tentorial brain lesions (Repetition Time [TR] = 11 460 ms, Echo Time [TE] = 102 ms, Inversion Time [TI] = 2360 ms, Field of View [FOV] = 280 mm × 330 mm, Number of Excitations [NEX] = 2, matrix = 248 × 320, 1.00 × 1.00 mm2 in-plane resolution, horizontal slices with a slice thickness of 3.0 mm and no gap).

1.4 MRI data analysis

To avoid a type I error induced by the effect of WM hyperintensities on brain connectivity results, two expert radiologist (CCQ, YE) examined all MRIs. Participants were excluded when more than three lesions with a maximum diameter of 5 mm were detected in the subcortical or periventricular WM on axial FLAIR images (Quattrocchi et al., Reference Quattrocchi, De Pandis, Piervincenzi, Galli, Melgari, Salomone and Stocchi2015).

1.4.1 Preprocessing of diffusion data

All DWIs were visually inspected for artifacts and preprocessed using different tools from FDT [FMRIB Diffusion Toolbox, part of FSL (FMRIB’s Software Library v.5.0.8, https://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/); (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Jenkinson, Woolrich, Beckmann, Behrens, Johansen-Berg and Niazy2004)]. Images were corrected for eddy current distortion and head motion using a 12 parameter affine registration to the first no-diffusion-weighted volume of each participant, and the gradient directions were rotated accordingly (Leemans & Jones, Reference Leemans and Jones2009). Corrected images were skull-stripped using Brain Extraction Tool (Smith, Reference Smith2002). Diffusion tensor images were then generated for each participant and each time point using the Diffusion Tensor Imaging ToolKit software package (DTI-TK, https://dti-tk.sourceforge.net/pmwiki/pmwiki.php) (Zhang, Yushkevich, Alexander, & Gee, Reference Zhang, Yushkevich, Alexander and Gee2006).

An unbiased longitudinal analysis approach was chosen for the registration of DTI data (Keihaninejad et al., Reference Keihaninejad, Zhang, Ryan, Malone, Modat, Cardoso and Ourselin2013) using DTI-TK, which applies a registration algorithm that leverages the full diffusion tensor information to drive the registration, improving the alignment of WM structures (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Gupta, Liu, Zhang, Escolar, Gilmore and Styner2011). At the end of the registration procedure, each participant’s DTI data were normalized to the ICBM-152 template (Zhang, Peng, Dawe, & Arfanakis, Reference Zhang, Peng, Dawe and Arfanakis2011), and FA maps were generated for each participant using the normalized tensor images (for details on the DTI data analysis, see Piervincenzi et al., Reference Piervincenzi, Ben-Soussan, Mauro, Mallio, Errante, Quattrocchi and Carducci2017).

FA data from each participant were further analyzed using selected modules of the Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Jenkinson, Johansen-Berg, Rueckert, Nichols, Mackay and Behrens2006) toolbox, available in FSL. The mean FA image was created and thinned to create a mean FA skeleton, which represents the centers of all tracts common to the group. Each participant’s FA image was then projected onto this common skeleton to minimize any residual misalignment of tracts.

Randomize tool (5000 permutations) was used to examine the statistical correlation between FA maps at T2 and EPQ’s subscale (i.e., E, P, and N) scores. The statistical threshold was established with a family-wise error-corrected p value (pFWE) < .05 with multiple comparison corrections using threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) (Smith & Nichols, Reference Smith and Nichols2009). All the results were anatomically localized using the Johns Hopkins University (JHU) ICBM-DTI-81 White-Matter Labels and the JHU White-Matter Tractography atlases included in the FSL distribution (https://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/Atlases).

1.4.2 Statistical analysis

Voxelwise correlation analyses were conducted to assess the relationship between FA values and EPQ subscale scores.

2. Results

A voxelwise correlation analyses between FA, AD, and RD values and EPQ subscale scores showed that extraversion score was negatively correlated with FA/AD values and positively correlated with RD value in WM within the right inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF) and forceps major (posterior).

Results of voxelwise correlation analyses between FA values and EPQ subscale scores are shown in Table 1. A strong negative correlation (pFWE < .05 TFCE-corrected) was found between FA values and Extraversion (see Table 1, Figure 1). No significant correlation was found between FA values and Neuroticism or Psychoticism.

Table 1. Results of voxelwise correlation analyses between FA, RD, and AD values at T2 (after 12 weeks of QMT) and EPQ’s subscale scores. Negative correlations (pFWE < .05 TFCE-corrected) were found between FA/AD values and Extraversion, whereas a positive correlation (pFWE < .05 TFCE-corrected) was found between RD values and Extraversion

Figure 1. Significant negative correlation (pFWE < .05 TFCE-corrected) between FA values and Extraversion (red to yellow color). The study-specific FA skeleton, representing the centers of principal WM tracts, is displayed in green, overlaid on the mean FA map (for methodological details, see also Piervincenzi et al., Reference Piervincenzi, Ben-Soussan, Mauro, Mallio, Errante, Quattrocchi and Carducci2017).

Since a strong negative correlation (pFWE < .05 TFCE-corrected) was found between FA values and E subscale, we then examined possible correlation between Extraversion and RD (Table 1, Figure 2) as well as AD (Table 1, Figure 3) values.

Figure 2. Significant positive correlation (pFWE < .05 TFCE-corrected) between RD values and Extraversion (blue to light blue color). See Figure 1 for additional details.

Figure 3. Significant negative correlation (pFWE < .05 TFCE-corrected) between AD values and Extraversion (dark yellow to yellow color). See Figure 1 for additional details.

3. Discussion

This study examined the correlations between the integrity of WM tracts and extraversion, using FA, AD, and RD indices. In accordance with our prediction, we found a correlation between extraversion and specific fibers within WM tracts. More specifically, extraversion score was negatively correlated with FA/AD values and positively correlated with RD value, suggesting that high extraversion score was associated with decreased organization of WM tracts (FA) and demyelination (RD) in WM within the right IFOF and forceps major (posterior). These WM fibers interconnect brain regions that are implicated in emotional processing and cognitive control functions related to extraversion.

The IFOF is a large WM tract connecting the frontal (infero-lateral and dorso-lateral frontal), temporal, and occipital lobes and constitutes one of the major efferent and afferent neuronal projections to the frontal lobes. It runs from the lateral aspect of the frontal lobe, passing through the extreme and external capsules, as well as the temporal lobe stem, before radiating into the posterior temporal and occipital cortex (Catani, Howard, Pajevic, & Jones, Reference Catani, Howard, Pajevic and Jones2002). WM in these association fibers mediates communications in brain areas that are active in controlling emotion and behavior (Kim & Whalen, Reference Kim and Whalen2009). Furthermore, IFOF is involved in insula connectivity, which is believed to play an important role in emotional regulation and cognition (Catani et al., Reference Catani, Howard, Pajevic and Jones2002).

The fibers of the posterior portion of the corpus callosum (CC) or the splenium form the forceps major (Catani et al., Reference Catani, Howard, Pajevic and Jones2002), which interconnect the two hemispheres and include fibers with projection zones in temporal, parietal, and occipital lobes (Witelson, Reference Witelson1989). In general, the CC connects the two cerebral hemispheres and is believed to play an important role in supporting sensory-motor functional integration, attention, language, interhemispheric transfer of associative learning, and emotional regulation and in understanding social situations and adopting appropriate social behavior (Jiang et al., Reference Jiang, Shi, Liu, Li, Ding, Shen and Shen2017). Studies of callosotomies and research on animals indicate that disruption of commissural tracts can cause imbalances in interhemispheric communication (see Bloom & Hynd, Reference Bloom and Hynd2005).

The IFOF and forceps major WM tracts hold the most prominent fibers running between the cognitive control and emotional systems in the brain and are responsible for effective communication between them. As evidenced by neurodevelopmental studies, improved cognitive and emotional functions are thought to involve maturation of functional connections subserved by ongoing myelination, which allows more rapid and efficient communication between neurons (Spear, Reference Spear2000). Specifically, the increasing organization of WM from childhood to early adulthood, particularly in the prefrontal area but also in the parietal, occipital, and temporal lobes, corresponds to the development of cognitive control. Furthermore, the interaction of PFC with other functionally specialized brain regions through WM has been increasingly recognized as critical for these integrative brain functions (Clark, Thatcher, & Tapert, Reference Clark, Thatcher and Tapert2008; Luna, Paulsen, Padmanabhan, & Geier, Reference Luna, Paulsen, Padmanabhan and Geier2013; Olson et al., Reference Olson, Collins, Hooper, Muetzel, Lim and Luciana2009).

In the current study, we found decreased connectivity (low FA/AD together with high RD) values in the right IFOF and in the forceps major among individuals who had high extraversion ratings. Notably, the negative correlation between extraversion and WM integrity in right IFOF, which interconnects frontal-temporal-occipital regions, suggests that lateralization of these regions may play a role in expressions of extraversion. Right lateralization has been associated with an increased tendency toward negative emotions and withdrawal tendencies (Davidson, Reference Davidson1992; Davidson, Ekman, Saron, Senulis, & Friesen, Reference Davidson, Ekman, Saron, Senulis and Friesen1990), which comprise an introversion-associated dimension (Pang et al., Reference Pang, Cui, Duan, Chen, Zeng, Zhang and Chen2017). It may be, then, that the decreasing organization of WM tracts, as reflected in low FA/AD and high RD (demyelination), in these pathways results in disinhibition, approach tendencies, and emotionally oriented behaviors (especially positive ones), many of which mediate extraversion. Extraversion is considered a higher-order trait that encompasses a set of correlated lower-order traits (Depue & Collins, Reference Depue and Collins1999), which include behavioral and emotional characteristics such as social dominance, positive emotions, sociability, achievement, reward sensitivity, novelty seeking, and motor activity (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992; Depue & Collins, Reference Depue and Collins1999; DeYoung et al., Reference DeYoung, Hirsh, Shane, Papademetris, Rajeevan and Gray2010; Eysenck & Eysenck, Reference Eysenck and Eysenck1985). Furthermore, in their neurobehavioral model of extraversion, Depue and Collins (Reference Depue and Collins1999) defined extraversion as having two characteristics: (1) interpersonal engagement, which consists of affiliation (enjoying and valuing close interpersonal bonds, being warm and affectionate) and agency (being socially dominant, enjoying leadership roles, being assertive, being exhibitionistic, and having a sense of potency in accomplishing goals) and (2) impulsivity, which emerges from the interaction of extraversion and a second independent trait (constraint).

We speculated that the aforementioned individual differences in extraversion might reflect decreasing organization of the WM tracts connecting frontal cortex, temporal, and occipital areas, which are related to socioemotional and control functions (Aghajani et al., Reference Aghajani, Veer, Van Tol, Aleman, Van Buchem, Veltman and van der Wee2014; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Williams, Feczko, Barrett, Dickerson, Schwartz and Wedig2006). This possibility is supported by several DTI studies showing significant negative correlations between extraversion-related behaviors and the strength of fiber tracts, including IFOF, that interconnect frontal-temporal-occipital regions. A DTI study by Xu et al. (Reference Xu, Kober, Carroll, Rounsaville, Pearlson and Potenza2012), for example, reported that higher scores on the fun-seeking subscale of the behavioral activation system were correlated with lower WM integrity (i.e., greater MD values) in the left IFOF and inferior longitudinal fasciculus. In a systematic review, Waller, Dotterer, Murray, Maxwell, & Hyde (Reference Waller, Dotterer, Murray, Maxwell and Hyde2017) specify DTI studies that reported decreased FA in major WM fibers, including IFOF and forceps major, in adults with antisocial behaviors as compared to healthy controls. In another recent DTI study, Jiang et al. (Reference Jiang, Shi, Liu, Li, Ding, Shen and Shen2017) examined WM microstructure in antisocial personality disorder. They found decreased FA values in patients with antisocial disorder within the IFOF and other WM pathways, particularly those connecting the fronto-parietal control network and the fronto-temporal network. These pathways showed correlations with impulsivity and risky behaviors. The authors argued that damage to specific WM tracts might result in disinhibition, poor decision-making, and an inability to follow rules, due to impaired modulation of the frontal cortex.

Note that the findings reported by Waller et al. (Reference Waller, Dotterer, Murray, Maxwell and Hyde2017) and Jiang et al. (Reference Jiang, Shi, Liu, Li, Ding, Shen and Shen2017) were based on studies of individuals with antisocial disorder, which is linked to psychoticism rather than to the extraversion dimension. Here, we found a similar pattern of decreased FA values in WM pathways, but it was associated with the extraversion dimension. This may indicate that characteristics related to the psychoticism dimension, such as impulsivity, are also related to extraversion. As a multifaceted concept, impulsivity, when broken down into its subfactors, may well occupy overlapping areas within the psychoticism and extraversion dimensions (Zuckerman & Glicksohn, Reference Zuckerman and Glicksohn2016).

Overall, these studies suggest that decreased WM organization across regions and hemispheres is associated with behavioral characteristics that encompass both broad factors and narrower facets of extraversion. In accordance, extraverts typically show high functional connectivity within brain networks subserving reward and motivation in response to rewarding stimuli (Canli, Reference Canli2004; Cohen, Young, Baek, Kessler, & Ranganath, Reference Cohen, Young, Baek, Kessler and Ranganath2005; Deckersbach et al., Reference Deckersbach, Miller, Klibanski, Fischman, Dougherty, Blais and Rauch2006). Similar findings have been reported in resting-state connectivity studies (Adelstein et al., Reference Adelstein, Shehzad, Mennes, DeYoung, Zuo, Kelly and Milham2011; Aghajani et al., Reference Aghajani, Veer, Van Tol, Aleman, Van Buchem, Veltman and van der Wee2014; Pang et al., Reference Pang, Cui, Wang, Chen, Wang, Han and Chen2016). For example, Pang et al. (Reference Pang, Cui, Duan, Chen, Zeng, Zhang and Chen2017) employed the functional connectivity density (FCD) method for the first time to distinguish the energy-efficient hubs associated with extraversion. Their results were extracted from resting-state fMRI data from 71 healthy participants and reported that short-range FCD was positively correlated with extraversion in the left cuneus, revealing a link between the local functional activity of this region and extraversion as expressed in risk-taking. In contrast, long-range FCD was negatively correlated with extraversion in the right superior frontal gyrus and the inferior frontal gyrus, a region uniquely positioned between emotional limbic regions and cognitive executive processing networks in the dorsal and medial areas of the PFC. Subsequent seed-based resting-state functional connectivity analyses revealed that a decreased long-range FCD in individuals with high extraversion scores showed a low long-range functional connectivity pattern between the medial and dorsolateral PFC, MTG, and anterior cingulate cortex. These regions are involved in inhibitory control and emotion perception and likely have an important role in extraversion (for details, see Pang et al., Reference Pang, Cui, Duan, Chen, Zeng, Zhang and Chen2017).

It should be noted that characteristics and behaviors associated with extraversion, such as outgoing nature, novelty seeking, and reward sensitivity, can constitute both risk factors and protective factors (e.g., family and/or marital stressors, employment, environments, and genetics factors) and that the quality of these factors may depend on the strength of communication between them and the involved brain regions. However, we did not find increased FA in WM tracts interconnecting brain areas associated with regulatory control. Also, we did not find significant correlations between extraversion and WM tract integrity in other brain areas, for example, the uncinate fasciculus, longitudinal (arcuate) fasciculus, and insula, which were found in anatomical studies to be associated with extraversion (Aghajani et al., Reference Aghajani, Veer, Van Tol, Aleman, Van Buchem, Veltman and van der Wee2014; Gao et al., Reference Gao, Xu, Long, Duan, Liao, Ding and Chen2013; Pang et al., Reference Pang, Cui, Wang, Chen, Wang, Han and Chen2016). The most parsimonious interpretation is that these negative findings can be attributed to characteristics of the current sample. With no significant correlations between WM integrity and two personality traits (psychoticism and neuroticism), the present study supports Privado et al.’s (Reference Privado, Román, Saénz-Urturi, Burgaleta and Colom2017) assertion “that replicability is far from straightforward when structural brain features are related with the considered basic personality traits” (p.180).

Although DTI study in meditators is surprisingly rare, a few DTI studies have found a connection between meditation training and FA changes in tracts similar to those found in the current report, including increased FA of several fiber tracts, including the superior and anterior corona radiata, the genu and body of the CC, and the superior longitudinal fasciculus (Tang et al., Reference Tang, Ma, Fan, Feng, Wang, Feng and Zhang2009; Luders, Clark, Narr, & Toga, Reference Luders, Clark, Narr and Toga2011). Also, specifically structured mindful sensorimotor training was found to increase FA in these tracts (Piervincenzi et al., Reference Piervincenzi, Ben-Soussan, Mauro, Mallio, Errante, Quattrocchi and Carducci2017). Together, these results suggest that training, such as meditation and mindful sensorimotor training, might be a powerful tool to change the physical structure of the brain and thus of personality.

The current study focused on the association between trait extraversion and WM and did not examine the effect of training on the relationship between extraversion and WM. As such, EPQ traits were assessed after 12 weeks of training to avoid the effect of T1 measurement on trait values in T2 measurement. Although the prevailing assumption that personality traits are stable over time, personality traits may show mean level change across the life course (Caspi, Roberts, & Shiner, Reference Caspi, Roberts and Shiner2005; Harris, Brett, Johnson, & Deary, Reference Harris, Brett, Johnson and Deary2016). Since the correlation between FA, AD and RD and EPQ’s subscale scores was conducted only after 12 weeks of QMT training, and as training can affect the brain microstructure, the current results should be taken as suggestive and future studies should be conducted to examine this important connection between WM and personality.

Thus, an examination of possible effects of training on the level of trait extraversion in future research can expand the findings of the current work and also help to determine training-induced changes in personality and their underlying mechanism.

In conclusion, to better understand WM involvement in extraversion, we supplemented FA analysis with analyses of AD and RD, which helped provide more information about WM organization. All of the measures revealed associations between extraversion and the same WM tracts. We did not find correlations between AD and RD and other WM tracts, which were not detected by FA.

4. Limitations of the study

At present, we can report correlations, but not causal relationships, between WM organization (via FA, AD, and RD indices) and extraversion, such that the findings should be interpreted cautiously and future research should investigate the nature and implications of the current observations, by employing the effective connectivity method, which can provide information about causation (Sporns, Reference Sporns2013).

Furthermore, we used DTI images of 12 diffusion directions with four scan repetitions. Although more scan repetition seems to be related to a higher signal-to-noise ratio as well as to more reliable FA and tractography data (Jones, Reference Jones2004; Farrell et al., Reference Farrell, Landman, Jones, Smith, Prince, Van Zijl and Mori2007), it has been suggested that a larger number of diffusion gradient directions may improve the diffusion tensor estimation (Jones, Reference Jones2004; Ni, Kavcic, Zhu, Ekholm, & Zhong, Reference Ni, Kavcic, Zhu, Ekholm and Zhong2006). Therefore, future studies should include more directions of the diffusion gradient.

5. Conclusion

Our study revealed a link between extraversion and WM microstructure in a healthy sample, thereby providing insight into neurobiological mechanisms of extraversion. Evidence from functional and structural connectivity studies has shown that interindividual differences in extraversion and personality-related traits have biological bases in the brain. Whereas neuroscientists have begun to use DTI to examine the structural correlates of various psychiatric and neurological disorders (Kim & Whalen, Reference Kim and Whalen2009; Phan et al., Reference Phan, Orlichenko, Boyd, Angstadt, Coccaro, Liberzon and Arfanakis2009; Waller et al., Reference Waller, Dotterer, Murray, Maxwell and Hyde2017), few studies have examined the structural underpinnings of normal variations in personality characteristics using DTI and even fewer have examined extraversion as a trait (Xu & Potenza, Reference Xu and Potenza2012). To our knowledge, there was no previous investigation of the association between extraversion and FA, AD, and RD indices.

Given the role of extraversion in affect and in the development of both adaptive and risky behaviors, further knowledge about the relationship between WM integrity and extraversion may have broad applicability. In particular, it would be interesting to examine the effect of extraversion on risky behaviors (smoking, alcohol use, and violence) as compared to adaptive behaviors in different populations and to further research the neural underpinnings of behavioral differences as a function of extraversion.

Author Contributions

TDB-S, FC, and CCQ designed the research. CCQ, CAM, and YE performed the research. CP, TDB-S, FC, and RL analyzed the data. RL, CP, and TDB-S wrote the paper. FC, PP, and CCQ contributed to the writing process.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adelstein, J. S., Shehzad, Z., Mennes, M., DeYoung, C. G., Zuo, X. N., Kelly, C., … Milham, M. P. (2011). Personality is reflected in the brain’s intrinsic functional architecture. PloS One, 6, e27633. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aghajani, M., Veer, I. M., Van Tol, M. J., Aleman, A., Van Buchem, M. A., Veltman, D. J., … van der Wee, N. J. (2014). Neuroticism and extraversion are associated with amygdala resting-state functional connectivity. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 836848. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0224-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Assaf, Y., & Pasternak, O. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI)-based white matter mapping in brain research: A review. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience, 34, 5161. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12031-007-0029-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basser, P. J., Mattiello, J., & LeBihan, D. (1994). MR diffusion tensor spectroscopy and imaging. Biophysical Journal, 66, 259267. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3495(94)80775-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaulieu, C. (2002). The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system–A technical review. NMR in Biomedicine, 15, 435455. https://doi.org/10.1002/nbm.782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blankstein, U., Chen, J. Y., Mincic, A. M., McGrath, P. A., & Davis, K. D. (2009). The complex minds of teenagers: Neuroanatomy of personality differs between sexes. Neuropsychologia, 47, 599603. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, J. S., & Hynd, G. W. (2005). The role of the corpus callosum in interhemispheric transfer of information: Excitation or inhibition? Neuropsychology Review, 15, 5971. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-005-6252-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canli, T. (2004). Functional brain mapping of extraversion and neuroticism: Learning from individual differences in emotion processing. Journal of Personality, 72, 11051132. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00292.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001). An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 3342. https://doi.org/10.1037//0735-7044.115.1.33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caminiti, R., Carducci, F., Piervincenzi, C., Battaglia-Mayer, A., Confalone, G., Visco-Comandini, F., … Innocenti, G. M. (2013). Diameter, length, speed, and conduction delay of callosal axons in macaque monkeys and humans: Comparing data from histology and magnetic resonance imaging diffusion tractography. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1450114511. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0761-13.2013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., & Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: Stability and change. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 453484. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141913.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catani, M., Howard, R. J., Pajevic, S., & Jones, D. K. (2002). Virtual in vivo interactive dissection of white matter fasciculi in the human brain. NeuroImage, 17, 7794. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. B., Thatcher, D. L., & Tapert, S. F. (2008). Alcohol, psychological dysregulation, and adolescent brain development. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32, 375385. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00601.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M. X., Schoene-Bake, J. C., Elger, C. E., & Weber, B. (2009). Connectivity-based segregation of the human striatum predicts personality characteristics. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 32. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M. X., Young, J., Baek, J. M., Kessler, C., & Ranganath, C. (2005). Individual differences in extraversion and dopamine genetics predict neural reward responses. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 851861. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, P. T. Jr, & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Four ways five factors are basic. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 653665. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(92)90236-I.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coutinho, J. F., Sampaio, A., Ferreira, M., Soares, J. M., & Gonçalves, O. F. (2013). Brain correlates of pro-social personality traits: A voxel-based morphometry study. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 7, 293299. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-013-9227-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cremers, H., van Tol, M. J., Roelofs, K., Aleman, A., Zitman, F. G., van Buchem, M. A., … van der Wee, N. J. (2011). Extraversion is linked to volume of the orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala. PloS One, 6, e28421. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R. J. (1992). Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion. Brain and Cognition, 20, 125151. https://doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(92)90065-T.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C. D., Senulis, J. A., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain physiology: I. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 330341. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dazzi, C. (2011). The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire–Revised (EPQ-R): A confirmation of the factorial structure in the Italian context. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 790794. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.12.032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deckersbach, T., Miller, K. K., Klibanski, A., Fischman, A., Dougherty, D. D., Blais, M. A., … Rauch, S. L. (2006). Regional cerebral brain metabolism correlates of neuroticism and extraversion. Depression and Anxiety, 23, 133138. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491517. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002046.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Rajeevan, N., & Gray, J. R. (2010). Testing predictions from personality neuroscience brain structure and the big five. Psychological Science, 21, 820828. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610370159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Satpute, A. B. (2005). Personality from a controlled processing perspective: An fMRI study of neuroticism, extraversion, and self-consciousness. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 169181. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.5.2.169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Thomas.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985) Personality and individual differences: A natural science approach. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1991). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Scales. London: Hodder and Stoughton.Google Scholar
Farrell, J. A., Landman, B. A., Jones, C. K., Smith, S. A., Prince, J. L., Van Zijl, P. C., & Mori, S. (2007). Effects of signal-to-noise ratio on the accuracy and reproducibility of diffusion tensor imaging-derived fractional anisotropy, mean diffusivity, and principal eigenvector measurements at 1.5T. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 26, 756767. https://doi.org/10.1002/jmri.21053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gao, Q., Xu, Q., Long, Z., Duan, X., Liao, W., Ding, J., … Chen, H. (2013). Extraversion and neuroticism relate to topological properties of resting-state brain networks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 257. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00257.Google ScholarPubMed
Gray, J. A. (1970). The psychophysiological basis of introversion–extraversion. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 8, 249266. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(70)90069-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haas, B. W., Omura, K., Amin, Z., Constable, R. T., & Canli, T. (2006). Functional connectivity with the anterior cingulate is associated with extraversion during the emotional Stroop task. Social Neuroscience, 1, 1624. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910600650753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, M. A., Brett, C. E., Johnson, W., & Deary, I. J. (2016). Personality stability from age 14 to age 77 years. Psychology and Aging, 31, 862874. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jiang, W., Shi, F., Liu, H., Li, G., Ding, Z., Shen, H., … Shen, D. (2017). Reduced white matter integrity in antisocial personality disorder: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Scientific Reports, 7, 43002. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep43002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, D. K. (2004). The effect of gradient sampling schemes on measures derived from diffusion tensor MRI: A Monte Carlo study. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 51, 807815. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.20033.Google ScholarPubMed
Jones, D. K., Knösche, T. R., & Turner, R. (2013). White matter integrity, fiber count, and other fallacies: The do’s and don’ts of diffusion MRI. NeuroImage, 73, 239254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jung, R. E., Grazioplene, R., Caprihan, A., Chavez, R. S., & Haier, R. J. (2010). White matter integrity, creativity, and psychopathology: Disentangling constructs with diffusion tensor imaging. PloS One, 5, e9818. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanai, R., & Rees, G. (2011). The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 231242. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keihaninejad, S., Zhang, H., Ryan, N. S., Malone, I. B., Modat, M., Cardoso, M. J., … Ourselin, S. (2013). An unbiased longitudinal analysis framework for tracking white matter changes using diffusion tensor imaging with application to Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroImage, 72, 153163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, M. J., & Whalen, P. J. (2009). The structural integrity of an amygdala–prefrontal pathway predicts trait anxiety. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1161411618. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2335-09.2009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koelsch, S., Skouras, S., & Jentschke, S. (2013). Neural correlates of emotional personality: A structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. PLoS One, 8, e77196. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0077196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Küssner, M. B. (2017). Eysenck’s theory of personality and the role of background music in cognitive task performance: A mini-review of conflicting findings and a new perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1991. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leemans, A., & Jones, D.K. (2009). The B-matrix must be rotated when correcting for subject motion in DTI data. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 61, 13361349. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.21890.Google ScholarPubMed
Leshem, R. (2016). Using dual process models to examine impulsivity throughout neural maturation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 41, 125143. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2016.1178266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, F., Huo, Y., Li, M., Chen, H., Liu, F., Wang, Y., … Chen, H. (2014). Relationship between personality and gray matter volume in healthy young adults: A voxel-based morphometric study. PloS One, 9, e88763. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luders, E., Clark, K., Narr, K. L., & Toga, A. W. (2011). Enhanced brain connectivity in long-term meditation practitioners. NeuroImage, 57, 13081316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luna, B., Paulsen, D. J., Padmanabhan, A., & Geier, C. (2013). The teenage brain: Cognitive control and motivation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 94100. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413478416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, R. L., & Kumari, V. (2016). Hans Eysenck’s interface between the brain and personality: Modern evidence on the cognitive neuroscience of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 103, 7481. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mobbs, D., Hagan, C. C., Azim, E., Menon, V., & Reiss, A. L. (2005). Personality predicts activity in reward and emotional regions associated with humor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 1650216506. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0408457102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munafo, M. R., & Black, S. (2007). Personality and smoking status: A longitudinal analysis. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 9, 397404. https://doi.org/10.1080/14622200701188869.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ni, H., Kavcic, V., Zhu, T., Ekholm, S., & Zhong, J. (2006). Effects of number of diffusion gradient directions on derived diffusion tensor imaging indices in human brain. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 27, 17761781.Google ScholarPubMed
Olson, E. A., Collins, P. F., Hooper, C. J., Muetzel, R., Lim, K. O., & Luciana, M. (2009). White matter integrity predicts delay discounting behavior in 9-to 23-year-olds: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 14061421. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Omura, K., Constable, R. T., & Canli, T. (2005). Amygdala gray matter concentration is associated with extraversion and neuroticism. NeuroReport, 16, 19051908. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000186596.64458.76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pang, Y., Cui, Q., Duan, X., Chen, H., Zeng, L., Zhang, Z., … Chen, H. (2017). Extraversion modulates functional connectivity hubs of resting-state brain networks. Journal of Neuropsychology, 11, 347361. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pang, Y., Cui, Q., Wang, Y., Chen, Y., Wang, X., Han, S., … Chen, H. (2016). Extraversion and neuroticism related to the resting-state effective connectivity of amygdala. Scientific Reports, 6, 35484. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep35484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phan, K. L., Orlichenko, A., Boyd, E., Angstadt, M., Coccaro, E. F., Liberzon, I., & Arfanakis, K. (2009). Preliminary evidence of white matter abnormality in the uncinate fasciculus in generalized social anxiety disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 691694. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pierpaoli, C., Jezzard, P., Basser, P.J., Barnett, A., & DiChiro, G. (1996). Diffusion tensor MR imaging of the human brain. Radiology, 201, 637648. https://doi.org/10.1148/radiology.201.3.8939209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piervincenzi, C., Ben-Soussan, T. D., Mauro, F., Mallio, C. A., Errante, Y., Quattrocchi, C. C., & Carducci, F. (2017). White matter microstructural changes following Quadrato Motor Training: A longitudinal study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 590. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Privado, J., Román, F. J., Saénz-Urturi, C., Burgaleta, M., & Colom, R. (2017). Gray and white matter correlates of the Big Five personality traits. Neuroscience, 349, 174184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.02.039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quattrocchi, C. C., De Pandis, M. F., Piervincenzi, C., Galli, M., Melgari, J. M., Salomone, G., … Stocchi, F. (2015). Acute modulation of brain connectivity in Parkinson disease after automatic mechanical peripheral stimulation: A pilot study. PLoS One 10(10), e0137977. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sen, P. N., & Basser, P. J. (2005). A model for diffusion in white matter in the brain. Biophysical Journal, 89, 29272938. https://doi.org/10.1529/biophysj.105.063016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, S. M. (2002). Fast robust automated brain extraction. Human Brain Mapping, 17, 143155. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Rueckert, D., Nichols, T. E., Mackay, C. E., … Behrens, T. E. (2006). Tract-based spatial statistics: Voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. NeuroImage, 31, 14871505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E., Johansen-Berg, H., … Niazy, R. K. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage, 23, S208S219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., & Nichols, T. E. (2009). Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. NeuroImage, 44, 8398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 417463. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00014-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sporns, O. (2013). Structure and function of complex brain networks. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 15, 247262.Google ScholarPubMed
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., & Kawashima, R. (2010). White matter structures associated with creativity: Evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. NeuroImage, 51, 1118. https://doi.org/10..1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Fan, Y., Feng, H., Wang, J., Feng, S., … Zhang, Y. (2009). Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is altered by short-term meditation. Proceedings of the national Academy of Sciences, 106, 88658870. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0904031106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tavor, I., Yablonski, M., Mezer, A., Rom, S., Assaf, Y., & Yovel, G. (2014). Separate parts of occipito-temporal white matter fibers are associated with recognition of faces and places. NeuroImage, 86, 123130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.085.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trémeau, F., Darreye, A., Leroy, B., Renckly, V., Ertle, S., Weibel, H., … Macher, J. P. (2003). Personality changes in opioid-dependent subjects in a methadone maintenance treatment program. L’encéphale, 29, 285292.Google Scholar
Wang, Y., Gupta, A., Liu, Z., Zhang, H., Escolar, M. L., Gilmore, J. H., … Styner, M. (2011). DTI registration in atlas based fiber analysis of infantile Krabbe disease. NeuroImage, 55, 15771586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, R., Dotterer, H. L., Murray, L., Maxwell, A. M., & Hyde, L. W. (2017). White-matter tract abnormalities and antisocial behavior: A systematic review of diffusion tensor imaging studies across development. NeuroImage: Clinical, 14, 201215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2017.01.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilt, J., & Revelle, W. (2009). Extraversion. In Leary, M. & Hoyle, R. (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in social behavior (pp. 2745), New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Witelson, S. F. (1989). Hand and sex differences in the isthmus and genu of the human corpus callosum: A postmortem morphological study. Brain, 112, 799835. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/112.3.799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, C. I., Williams, D., Feczko, E., Barrett, L. F., Dickerson, B. C., Schwartz, C. E., & Wedig, M. M. (2006). Neuroanatomical correlates of extraversion and neuroticism. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 18091819. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, J., Kober, H., Carroll, K. M., Rounsaville, B. J., Pearlson, G. D., & Potenza, M. N. (2012). White matter integrity and behavioral activation in healthy subjects. Human Brain Mapping, 33, 9941002. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, J., & Potenza, M. N. (2012). White matter integrity and five-factor personality measures in healthy adults. NeuroImage, 59, 800807. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, H., Yushkevich, P.A., Alexander, D.C., & Gee, J.C. (2006). Deformable registration of diffusion tensor MR images with explicit orientation optimization. Medical Image Analysis, 10, 764785. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.media.2006.06.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, S., Peng, H., Dawe, R.J., & Arfanakis, K. (2011). Enhanced ICBM diffusion tensor template of the human brain. NeuroImage, 54, 974984. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuckerman, M., & Glicksohn, J. (2016). Hans Eysenck’s personality model and the constructs of sensation seeking and impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 103, 4852. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Results of voxelwise correlation analyses between FA, RD, and AD values at T2 (after 12 weeks of QMT) and EPQ’s subscale scores. Negative correlations (pFWE < .05 TFCE-corrected) were found between FA/AD values and Extraversion, whereas a positive correlation (pFWE < .05 TFCE-corrected) was found between RD values and Extraversion

Figure 1

Figure 1. Significant negative correlation (pFWE < .05 TFCE-corrected) between FA values and Extraversion (red to yellow color). The study-specific FA skeleton, representing the centers of principal WM tracts, is displayed in green, overlaid on the mean FA map (for methodological details, see also Piervincenzi et al., 2017).

Figure 2

Figure 2. Significant positive correlation (pFWE < .05 TFCE-corrected) between RD values and Extraversion (blue to light blue color). See Figure 1 for additional details.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Significant negative correlation (pFWE < .05 TFCE-corrected) between AD values and Extraversion (dark yellow to yellow color). See Figure 1 for additional details.