Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T04:02:08.016Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Explanation, Teleology, and Operant Behaviorism: A Study of the Experimental Analysis of Purposive Behavior

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2022

Jon D. Ringen*
Affiliation:
Indiana University at South Bend

Abstract

B. F. Skinner's claim that “operant behavior is essentially the field of purpose” is systematically explored. It is argued that Charles Taylor's illuminating analysis of the explanatory significance of common-sense goal-ascriptions (1) lends some (fairly restricted) support to Skinner's claim, (2) considerably clarifies the conceptual significance of differences between operant and respondent behavior and conditioning, and (3) undercuts influential assertions (e.g., Taylor's) that research programs for behavioristic psychology share a “mechanistic” orientation. A strategy is suggested for assessing the plausibility of Skinner's broader claims about the adequacy of the operant behaviorist program for the analysis of purposive behavior.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © 1976 by the Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

I would like to thank Theodore Mischel for inviting me to participate in his 1974 NEH summer seminar, and Kenneth MacCorquodale for allowing me to audit his excellent course on operant behaviorism during the 1973–74 academic year. These two opportunities greatly facilitated the writing of this paper. Support of preliminary research for the paper was provided by a 1973 Indiana University Summer Faculty Fellowship. Writing of the paper was partially supported by NSF grant SOC 75-13423. Support from these two sources and from NEH is gratefully acknowledged.

References

REFERENCES

Arnold, W. (ed.) (forthcoming) Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Vol. 23, 1975.Google Scholar
Boden, M. Purposive Explanation in Psychology. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borger, R.Comment.” In [4], pp. 8088.Google Scholar
Borger, R. and Cioffi, F. (eds.) Explanation in the Behavioural Sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1970.Google Scholar
Braithwaite, R.Causal and Teleological Explanation.” In Scientific Explanation. New York and London: Cambridge University Press, 1953. Pages 319341.Google Scholar
Care, N. and Landesman, C. (eds.) Readings in the Theory of Action. Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1968.Google Scholar
Catania, A. C. (ed.) Contemporary Research in Operant Behavior. Glenview, III.: Scott, Foresman and Company, 1968.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959) “A Review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior.” Language 35 (1959): 2658. Reprinted in L. A. Jakobovits, and M. S. Miron (eds.), Readings in the Psychology of Language. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1967. Pages 142–171.Google Scholar
Deutsch, J. A. The Structural Basis of Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1960.Google Scholar
Dixon, T. and Horton, D. (eds.) Verbal Behavior and General Behavior Theory. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1968.Google Scholar
Estes, W., et al. Modern Learning Theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1954.Google Scholar
Feigl, H. and Scriven, M. (eds.) The Foundations of Science and the Concepts of Psychology and Psychoanalysis, Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, Vol. 1. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1956.Google Scholar
Goldman, A.The Compatibility of Mechanism and Purpose.” Philosophical Review LXXVIII (1969): 468482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A. A Theory of Human Action. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1970.Google Scholar
Hamlyn, D.Conditioning and Behaviour.” in [4], pp. 139152.Google Scholar
Harré, R. and Secord, P. The Explanation of Social Behaviour. Totowa, N.J.: Littlefield, Adams, and Co., 1973.Google Scholar
Hempel, C. G.The Logic of Functional Analysis.” In Symposium on Sociological Theory. Edited by Gross, L. New York: Harper and Row, 1959. Pages 271287.Google Scholar
Honig, W. K. (ed.) Operant Behavior: Areas of Research and Application. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. Principles of Behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1943.Google Scholar
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior.Google Scholar
Koch, S. (1964) “Psychology and Conceptions of Knowledge as Unitary.” In [69], pp. 146.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1962.Google Scholar
Lacey, H. “The Scientific Study of Linguistic Behavior: a Perspective on the Skinner-Chomsky Controversy. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 4 (1974): 1751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacCorquodale, K. “On Chomsky's Review of Skinner's Verbal Behavior.” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 13 (1970): 8399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malcolm, N.The Conceivability of Mechanism.” Philosophical Review LXXVII (1968): 4572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeil, D. The Acquisition of Language. New York: Harper and Row, 1970.Google Scholar
Mischel, T. (ed.) Human Action. New York: Academic Press, 1969.Google Scholar
Mischel, T. (ed.) Cognitive Development and Epistemology. New York: Academic Press, 1971.Google Scholar
Mischel, T. (ed.) Understanding Other Persons. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 1974.Google Scholar
Mischel, T. (1975) “Psychological Explanations and Their Vicissitudes.” In [1].Google Scholar
Nevin, J. A. and Reynolds, G. (eds.) The Study of Behavior. Glenview, Ill.: Scott, Foresman and Company, 1973.Google Scholar
Noble, D.Charles Taylor on Teleological Explanation.” Analysis XXVII (1967): 96103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osgood, C.Toward a Wedding of Insufficiencies.” In [10], pp. 495519.Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. London: Oxford University Press, 1927.Google Scholar
Peters, R. (ed.) The Concept of Education. New York: The Humanities Press, 1967.Google Scholar
Popper, K. R. The Logic of Scientific Discovery. New York: Basic Books, 1959.Google Scholar
Reynolds, G. S. A Primer of Operant Conditioning. Glenview, Ill.: Scott, Foresman and Company, 1968.Google Scholar
Ringen, J. Behavior Theoretic Explanation. Indiana University: Unpublished Dissertation, 1971.Google Scholar
Ryle, G. The Concept of Mind. London: Hutchinson and Company, 1949.Google Scholar
Scheffler, I.Thoughts on Teleology.” The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 9 (1959): 265284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheffler, I. The Anatomy of Inquiry. New York: Alfred Knopf, 1963.Google Scholar
Scriven, M.A Study of Radical Behaviorism.” In [12], pp. 88130.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. The Behavior of Organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1938.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F.The Operational Analysis of Psychological Terms.” Psychological Review 52 (1945): 270277. Reprinted with some editorial changes in [53], pp. 370–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. Science and Human Behavior. New York: The Macmillan Company, 1953.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. Verbal Behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F.Behaviorism at Fifty.” In [69], pp. 79108.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F.Operant Behavior.” In [18], pp. 1232.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F.Preface to the Seventh Printing.” In [43], pp. ix-xiv.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. The Technology of Teaching. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1968.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. Contingencies of Reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1969.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. Beyond Freedom and Dignity. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1971.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. Cumulative Record, 3rd ed. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1972.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. About Behaviorism. New York: Alfred Knopf, 1974.Google Scholar
Taylor, C. The Explanation of Behaviour. New York: The Humanities Press, 1964.Google Scholar
Taylor, C.Teleological Explanation: A Reply to Denis Noble.” Analysis XXVII (1967): 141143.Google Scholar
Taylor, C.The Explanation of Purposive Behaviour.” and “Reply.” In [4], 49–79; 8996.Google Scholar
Terrace, H.Classical Conditioning.” In [31], 71114.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. Animal Intelligence. New York: The Macmillan Company, 1911.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C.Behaviorism and purpose.” The Journal of Philosophy (1925). Reprinted in [64], pp. 3237.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C.Purpose and Cognition: The Determiners of Animal Learning.” Psychological Review (1925). Reprinted in [64], pp. 3847.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C.A Behavioristic Theory of Ideas.” Psychological Review (1926). Reprinted in [64], pp. 4862.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press, 1932.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. Behavior and Psychological Man. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press, 1958.Google Scholar
Toulmin, S.Concepts and the Explanation of Human Behavior.” In [27], pp. 4170.Google Scholar
Verplanck, W.Burrhus F. Skinner.” In [11], pp. 267316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vesey, G.Conditioning and Learning.” In [35], pp. 6172.Google Scholar
von Wright, G. Explanation and Understanding. Ithaca: Cornel! University Press, 1971.Google Scholar
Wann, T. W. Behaviorism and Phenomenology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1964.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B.Psychology as the Behaviorist Views It.” Psychological Review 20 (1913): 158–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. Behaviorism. revised Edition. New York: W. W. Norton and Company, 1930.Google Scholar
Wright, L.The Case Against Teleological Reductionism.” The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 19 (1968): 211223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, L.Explanation and Teleology.” Philosophy of Science 39 (1972): 204218.10.1086/288434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, L. (1973) “Functions.” Philosophical Review LXXXII (1973): 139168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, L.Mechanisms and Purposive Behavior III.” Philosophy of Science 41 (1974): 345360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar