Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T07:14:51.406Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Domain generalisation in artificial language learning*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 February 2015

Scott Myers*
Affiliation:
University of Texas at Austin
Jaye Padgett*
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Cruz

Abstract

Many languages have restrictions on word-final segments, such as a requirementthat any word-final obstruent be voiceless. There is a phonetic basis for suchrestrictions at the ends of utterances, but not the ends of words. Historicallinguists have long noted this mismatch, and have attributed it to an analogicalgeneralisation of such restrictions from utterance-final to word-final position.To test whether language learners actually generalise in this way, twoartificial language learning experiments were conducted. Participants heardnonsense utterances in which there was a restriction on utterance-finalobstruents, but in which no information was available about word-finalutterance-medial obstruents. They were then tested on utterances that includedobstruents in both positions. They learned the pattern and generalised it toword-final utterance-medial position, confirming that learners are biased towardword-based distributional patterns.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We would like to thank audiences at the annual meeting of the LinguisticSociety of America (Pittsburgh, 2011), UC Berkeley Phorum, StanfordP-interest and UCSC Phlunch, as well as Megan Crowhurst (including forher help in creating stimuli), Maria Gouskova, Junko Ito, Danny Law,Grant McGuire, Richard Meier, Armin Mester, Deniz Rudin, Kristine Yu andthe anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions and stimulatingdiscussion of this paper. We are also grateful to Angela Aiello,Kimberly Cooper, Tommy Denby, Saralynn Emery, Scott Gaudinier, ElannaGrossman, Sean Hayes, Jules Lacour, Jessica Magallan, Melissa Ottele,Tatiana Puente, Rémy Ullman, Aileen Villapudua, BlakeWatkins, Stephen Welch, Nicholas Whittier and Akari Yamamura for theirwork in support of this project.

References

Andrzejewski, B. W. (1955). The problem of vowel representation in the Isaaq dialect of Somali. Bulletin of the School of Oriental and African Studies 17. 567580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald, Davidson, D. J. & Bates, Douglas M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59. 390412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, Dale J., Levy, Roger, Scheepers, Christoph & Tily, Harry J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68. 255278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, Douglas M., Maechler, Martin & Bolker, Ben (2012). Package ‘lme4’ (Version 0.999999-0): linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. Available (August 2014) at http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/lme4.pdf.Google Scholar
Becker, Lee A. (1977). Perceptually motivated phonetic change. CLS 13. 4557.Google Scholar
Becker, Lee A. (1979). A contribution to an explanation of the Neo-Stokavian accent retraction. Zbornik za Filologiju i Lingvistiku 22. 8794.Google Scholar
Beckman, Jill, Dickey, Laura Walsh & Urbanczyk, Suzanne (eds.) (1995). Papers in Optimality Theory. Amherst: GLSA.Google Scholar
Benua, Laura (1995). Identity effects in morphological truncation. In Beckman et al. (1995). 77–136.Google Scholar
Blevins, Juliette (2006). A theoretical synopsis of Evolutionary Phonology. Theoretical Linguistics 32. 117166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloomfield, Leonard (1933). Language. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul (1997). How we learn variation, optionality, and probability. Proceedings of the Institute of Phonetic Sciences of the University of Amsterdam 21. 4358.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul & Hayes, Bruce (2001). Empirical tests of the Gradual Learning Algorithm. LI 32. 4586.Google Scholar
Borowsky, Toni, Kawahara, Shigeto, Shinya, Takahito & Sugahara, Mariko (eds.) (2012). Prosody matters: essays in honor of Elisabeth Selkirk. London: Equinox.Google Scholar
Brent, Michael R. & Siskind, Jeffrey Mark (2001). The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development. Cognition 81. B33B34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, Angela C. (2010). A naturalness bias in learning stress. Phonology 27. 345392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, Wallace L. (1959). Internal reconstruction in Seneca. Lg 35. 477495.Google Scholar
Cutler, Anne & Norris, Dennis (1988). The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14. 113121.Google Scholar
Docherty, Gerard J. (1992). The timing of voicing in British English obstruents. Berlin & New York: Foris.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, Jan, Beckman, Mary E. & Munson, Benjamin (2004). The interaction between vocabulary size and phonotactic probability effects on children's production accuracy and fluency in nonword repetition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 47. 421436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ernestus, Mirjam & Baayen, R. Harald (2007). Intraparadigmatic effects on the perception of voice. In van de Weijer, Jeroen & van der Torre, Erik Jan (eds.) Voicing in Dutch: (de)voicing – phonology, phonetics, and psycholinguistics. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: Benjamins. 153174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ewert, Alfred (1943). The French language. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Finley, Sara & Badecker, William (2009). Artificial language learning and feature-based generalization. Journal of Memory and Language 61. 423437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flack, Kathryn (2009). Constraints on onsets and codas of words and phrases. Phonology 26. 269302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gafos, Adamantios I. (2006). Dynamics in grammar: comment on Ladd and Ernestus & Baayen. In Goldstein, Louis, Whalen, D. H. & Best, Catherine T. (eds.) Laboratory phonology 8. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 5179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haggard, M. (1978). The devoicing of voiced fricatives. JPh 6. 95102.Google Scholar
Hansen, Kenneth C. & Hansen, Lesley E. (1969). Pintupi phonology. Oceanic Linguistics 8. 153170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Bruce (1995). Metrical stress theory: principles and case studies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Herman, Rebecca (1996). Final lowering in Kipare. Phonology 13. 171196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hock, Hans Henrich (1991). Principles of historical linguistics. 2nd edn. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hualde, José Ignacio (2013). Intervocalic lenition and word-boundary effects: evidence from Judeo-Spanish. Diachronica 30. 232266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (1976). Phonologization. In Juilland, Alphonse (ed.) Linguistic studies offered to Joseph Greenberg on the occasion of his sixtieth birthday. Vol. 2: Phonology . Saratoga: Anma Libri. 407418.Google Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (1978a). Historical tonology. In Fromkin, Victoria A. (ed.) Tone: a linguistic survey. New York: Academic Press. 257269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (1978b). Word demarcation. In Greenberg, Joseph H., Ferguson, Charles A. & Moravcsik, Edith A. (eds.) Universals of human language. Vol. 2: Phonology . Stanford: Stanford University Press. 443470.Google Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (2013). Enlarging the scope of phonologization. In Yu, Alan C. L. (ed.) Origins of sound change: approaches to phonologization. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingram, David (1989). Phonological disability in children. 2nd edn: London: Cole & Whurr.Google Scholar
Iverson, Gregory K. & Salmons, Joseph C. (2007). Domains and directionality in the evolution of German final fortition. Phonology 24. 121145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jassem, Wiktor & Richter, Lutosława (1989). Neutralization of voicing in Polish obstruents. JPh 17. 317325.Google Scholar
Jespersen, Otto (1904). Lehrbuch der Phonetik. Leipzig & Berlin: Teubner.Google Scholar
Jessen, Michael (1998). Phonetics and phonology of tense and lax obstruents in German. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Jessen, Michael & Ringen, Catherine (2002). Laryngeal features in German. Phonology 19. 189218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, Elizabeth K., Seidl, Amanda & Tyler, Michael D. (2014). The edge factor in early word segmentation: utterance-level prosody enables word form extraction by 6-month-olds. PLoS One 9:1. Available at http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0083546.Google Scholar
Kenstowicz, Michael & Kisseberth, Charles (1979). Generative phonology: description and theory. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kerkhoff, Annemarie (2004). Acquisition of voicing alternations. In van Kempen, Jacqueline & Baauw, Sergio (eds.) Proceedings of GALA 2003 (Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition). Vol. 2. Utrecht: LOT. 269280.Google Scholar
Klatt, Dennis H. & Klatt, Laura C. (1990). Analysis, synthesis, and perception of voice quality variations among female and male talkers. JASA 87. 820857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legendre, Géraldine, Miyata, Yoshiro & Smolensky, Paul (1990a). Harmonic Grammar: a formal multi-level connectionist theory of linguistic well-formedness: an application. In Proceedings of the 12th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale: Erlbaum. 884891.Google Scholar
Legendre, Géraldine, Miyata, Yoshiro & Smolensky, Paul (1990b). Harmonic Grammar: a formal multi-level connectionist theory of linguistic well-formedness: theoretical foundations. In Proceedings of the 12th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale: Erlbaum. 388395.Google Scholar
Lehtonen, Jaakko (1970). Aspects of quantity in Standard Finnish. Jyväskylä: Jyväskylä University Press.Google Scholar
Liberman, Mark & Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (1984). Intonational invariance under changes in pitch range and length. In Aronoff, Mark & Oehrle, Richard T. (eds.) Language sound structure. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. 157233.Google Scholar
Lightner, Theodore M. (1972). Problems in the theory of phonology. Vol. 1: Russian phonology and Turkish phonology. Edmonton: Linguistic Research.Google Scholar
Lindblom, Björn (1983). Economy of speech gestures. In MacNeilage, Peter F. (ed.) The production of speech. New York: Springer. 217245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lisker, Leigh, Abramson, Arthur S., Cooper, Franklin S. & Schvey, Malcolm H. (1969). Transillumination of the larynx in running speech. JASA 45. 15441546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lombardi, Linda (1995). Laryngeal neutralization and syllable wellformedness. NLLT 13. 3974.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. (2005). Optimal paradigms. In Downing, Laura J., Hall, T. Alan & Raffelsiefen, Renate (eds.) Paradigms in phonological theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 170210.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. & Prince, Alan (1995). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In Beckman et al. (1995). 249384.Google Scholar
Miller, George A. (1958). Free recall of redundant strings of letters. Journal of Experimental Psychology 56. 485491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreton, Elliott (2008). Analytic bias and phonological typology. Phonology 25. 83127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreton, Elliott (2009). Underphonologization and modularity bias. In Parker, Steve (ed.) Phonological argumentation: essays on evidence and motivation. London: Equinox. 79101.Google Scholar
Moreton, Elliott & Pater, Joe (2012a). Structure and substance in artificial-phonology learning. Part 1: Structure. Language and Linguistics Compass 6. 686701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreton, Elliott & Pater, Joe (2012b). Structure and substance in artificial-phonology learning. Part 2: Substance. Language and Linguistics Compass 6. 702718.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, James L., Meier, Richard P. & Newport, Elissa L. (1987). Structural packaging in the input to language learning: contributions of prosodic and morphological marking of phrases to the acquisition of language. Cognitive Psychology 19. 498550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, Scott (1999). Tone association and f0 timing in Chichewa. Studies in African Linguistics 28. 215239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, Scott (2005). Vowel duration and neutralization of vowel length contrasts in Kinyarwanda. JPh 33. 427446.Google Scholar
Myers, Scott (2012). Final devoicing: production and perception studies. In Borowsky et al. (2012). 148–180.Google Scholar
Myers, Scott & Hansen, Benjamin B. (2007). The origin of vowel length neutralization in final position: evidence from Finnish speakers. NLLT 25. 157193.Google Scholar
Ohala, John J. (1993). The phonetics of sound change. In Jones, Charles (ed.) Historical linguistics: problems and perspectives. London & New York: Longman. 237278.Google Scholar
Padgett, Jaye (2012). The role of prosody in Russian voicing. In Borowsky et al. (2012). 181–207.Google Scholar
Peperkamp, Sharon, Skoruppa, Katrin & Dupoux, Emmanuel (2006). The role of phonetic naturalness in phonological rule acquisition. In Bamman, David, Magnitskaia, Tatiana & Zaller, Colleen (eds.) Proceedings of the 30th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville: Cascadilla. 464475.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (2003). Probabilistic phonology: discrimination and robustness. In Bod, Rens, Hay, Jennifer & Jannedy, Stefanie (eds.) Probabilistic linguistics. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. 177228.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. & Beckman, Mary E. (1988). Japanese tone structure. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Port, Robert F. & O'Dell, Michael L. (1985). Neutralization of syllable-final voicing in German. JPh 13. 455471.Google Scholar
Prieto, Pilar, Santen, Jan van & Hirschberg, Julia (1995). Tonal alignment patterns in Spanish. JPh 23. 429451.Google Scholar
Prince, Alan & Smolensky, Paul (1993). Optimality Theory: constraint interaction in generative grammar. Ms, Rutgers University & University of Colorado, Boulder. Published 2004, Malden, Mass. & Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Pycha, Anne, Nowak, Pawel, Shin, Eurie & Shosted, Ryan (2003). Phonological rule-learning and its implications for a theory of vowel harmony. WCCFL 22. 423435.Google Scholar
Pycha, Anne, Shin, Eurie & Shosted, Ryan (2006). Directionality of assimilation in consonant clusters: an experimental approach. UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report. 152166.Google Scholar
Schane, Sanford A., Tranel, Bernard & Lane, Harlan (1974/75). On the psychological reality of a natural rule of syllable structure. Cognition 3. 351358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidl, Amanda & Buckley, Eugene (2005). On the learning of arbitrary phonological rules. Language Learning and Development 1. 289316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidl, Amanda & Johnson, Elizabeth K. (2006). Infant word segmentation revisited: edge alignment facilitates target extraction. Developmental Science 9. 565573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shadle, Christine H. (1997). The aerodynamics of speech. In Hardcastle, William J. & Laver, John (eds.) The handbook of phonetic sciences. Oxford & Cambridge, Mass.: Blackwell. 3364.Google Scholar
Sievers, Eduard (1901). Grundzüge der Phonetik, zur Einführung in das Studium der Lautlehre der indogermanischen Sprachen. 5th edn. Leipzig: Breitkopf & Härtel.Google Scholar
Silverman, Kim E. A. & Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (1990). The timing of prenuclear high accents in English. In Kingston, John & Beckman, Mary E. (eds.) Papers in laboratory phonology I: between the grammar and physics of speech. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 72106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slifka, Janet (2006). Some physiological correlates to regular and irregular phonation at the end of an utterance. Journal of Voice 20. 171186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, Caroline L. (1997). The devoicing of /z/ in American English: effects of local and prosodic context. JPh 25. 471500.Google Scholar
Smith, Caroline L. (1999). Marking the boundary: utterance-final prosody in French questions and statements. In Ohala, John J., Hasegawa, Yoko, Ohala, Manjari, Granville, Daniel & Bailey, Ashlee C. (eds.) Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Vol. 5. Berkeley: University of California. 11811184.Google Scholar
Smith, Caroline L. (2003). Vowel devoicing in contemporary French. Journal of French Language Studies 13. 177194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steriade, Donca (1997). Phonetics in phonology: the case of laryngeal neutralization. Ms, University of California, Los Angeles. Available (August 2014) at http://www.linguistics.ucla.edu/people/steriade/papers/phoneticsinphonology.pdf.Google Scholar
Steriade, Donca (2000). Paradigm uniformity and the phonetics–phonology boundary. In Broe, Michael B. & Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (eds.) Papers in laboratory phonology V: acquisition and the lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 313334.Google Scholar
Sundara, Megha, Demuth, Katherine & Kuhl, Patricia K. (2011). Sentence-position effects on children's perception and production of English third person singular s. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 54. 5571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sweet, Henry (1877). A handbook of phonetics. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Tessier, Anne-Michelle (2012). Testing for OO-faithfulness in the acquisition of consonant clusters. Language Acquisition 19. 144173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vennemann, Theo (1974). Words and syllables in Natural Generative Grammar. In Bruck, Anthony, Fox, Robert A. & Galy, Michael W. La (eds.) Papers from the parasession on natural phonology. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society. 346374.Google Scholar
Wackernagel, Jakob (1957). Altindische grammatik. Vol. 1: Lautlehre. Reprinted from the 1896 edition. Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht.Google Scholar
Warner, Natasha, Jongman, Allard, Sereno, Joan & Kemps, Rachèl (2004). Incomplete neutralization and other sub-phonemic durational differences in production and perception: evidence from Dutch. JPh 32. 251276.Google Scholar
Westbury, John R. & Keating, Patricia A. (1986). On the naturalness of stop consonant voicing. JL 22. 145166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, James (2013). Evidence for a learning bias against saltatory phonological alternations. Cognition 130. 96115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, Colin (2003). Experimental investigation of phonological naturalness. WCCFL 22. 533546.Google Scholar
Wilson, Colin (2006). Learning phonology with substantive bias: an experimental and computational study of velar palatalization. Cognitive Science 30. 945982.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, Alan C. L. (2004). Explaining final obstruent voicing in Lezgian: phonetics and history. Lg 80. 7397.Google Scholar