Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T08:41:27.321Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Learning the identity effect as an artificial language: bias and generalisation*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 August 2013

Gillian Gallagher*
Affiliation:
New York University

Abstract

The results of two artificial grammar experiments show that individuals learn a distinction between identical and non-identical consonant pairs better than an arbitrary distinction, and that they generalise the distinction to novel segmental pairs. These results have implications for inductive models of learning, because they necessitate an explicit representation of identity. While identity has previously been represented as root-node sharing in autosegmental representations (Goldsmith 1976, McCarthy 1986), or implicitly assumed to be a property that constraints can reference (MacEachern 1999, Coetzee & Pater 2008), the model of inductive learning proposed by Hayes & Wilson (2008) assumes strictly feature-based representations, and is unable to reference identity directly. This paper explores the predictions of the Hayes & Wilson model and compares it to a modification of the model where identity is represented (Colavin et al.2010). The results of both experiments support a model incorporating direct reference to identity.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

I am particularly grateful to Peter Graff and three anonymous reviewers for detailed comments on this work, and also to Tal Linzen for helpful discussion. I also thank participants in the Fall 2012 phonology seminar and the Phonetics and Experiment Phonology reading group at New York University.

References

REFERENCES

Abramson, Arthur S. & Lisker, Leigh (1970). Discriminability along the voicing continuum: cross-language tests. Proceedings of the 6th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Prague: Publishing House of the Czechoslovak Academy of Sciences. 569573.Google Scholar
Albright, Adam (2009). Feature-based generalisation as a source of gradient acceptability. Phonology 26. 941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aulie, H. Wilbur & Aulie, Evelyn W (1978). Diccionario ch'ol–español, español–ch'ol. México, D. F.: Institutó Lingüístico de Verano.Google Scholar
Bates, Douglas M. & Maechler, Martin (2010). Package ‘lme4’ (Version 0.999375-34): linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. Available (April 2013) at http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/lme4.pdf.Google Scholar
Becker, Michael & Levine, Jonathan (2010). Experigen: an online experiment platform. Available (April 2013) at https://github.com/tlozoot/experigen.Google Scholar
Berent, Iris, Marcus, Gary F., Shimron, Joseph & Gafos, Adamantios I. (2002). The scope of linguistic generalizations: evidence from Hebrew word formation. Cognition 83. 113139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berent, Iris & Shimron, Joseph (1997). The representation of Hebrew words: evidence from the Obligatory Contour Principle. Cognition 64. 3972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berent, Iris, Steriade, Donca, Lennertz, Tracy & Vaknin, Vered (2007). What we know about what we have never heard: evidence from perceptual illusions. Cognition 104. 591630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berent, Iris, Wilson, Colin, Marcus, Gary F. & Bemis, Douglas K. (2012). On the role of variables in phonology: remarks on Hayes and Wilson 2008. LI 43. 97119.Google Scholar
Berg, René van den & Sidu, La Ode (1996). Muna–English dictionary. Leiden: KITLV Press.Google Scholar
Broselow, Ellen & Finer, Daniel (1991). Parameter setting in second language phonology and syntax. Second Language Research 7. 3559.Google Scholar
Broselow, Ellen, Chen, Su-I & Wang, Chilin (1998). The emergence of the unmarked in second language phonology. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 20. 261280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckley, Eugene (1990). Edge-in association and OCP ‘violations’ in Tigrinya. WCCFL 9. 7590.Google Scholar
Carpenter, Angela C. (2010). A naturalness bias in learning stress. Phonology 27. 345392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam & Halle, Morris (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Coetzee, Andries W. (2009a). Grammar is both categorical and gradient. In Parker, Steve (ed.) Phonological argumentation: essays on evidence and motivation. London: Equinox. 942.Google Scholar
Coetzee, Andries W. (2009b). Learning lexical indexation. Phonology 26. 109145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coetzee, Andries W. & Pater, Joe (2008). Weighted constraints and gradient restrictions on place co-occurrence in Muna and Arabic. NLLT 26. 289337.Google Scholar
Colavin, Rebecca S., Levy, Roger & Rose, Sharon (2010). Modeling OCP-Place in Amharic with the Maximum Entropy phonotactic learner. Ms, University of California, San Diego. Available (April 2013) at http://idiom.ucsd.edu/∼rose/Modeling%20OCP.pdf. To appear in CLS 46.Google Scholar
Davidson, Lisa (2006). Phonology, phonetics, or frequency: influences on the production of non-native sequences. JPh 34. 104137.Google Scholar
Endress, Ansgar D., Dehaene-Lambertz, Ghislaine & Mehler, Jacques (2007). Perceptual constraints and the learnability of simple grammars. Cognition 105. 577614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finley, Sara (2011). The privileged status of locality in consonant harmony. Journal of Memory and Language 65. 7483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frisch, Stefan A., Pierrehumbert, Janet B. & Broe, Michael B. (2004). Similarity avoidance and the OCP. NLLT 22. 179228.Google Scholar
Frisch, Stefan A. & Zawaydeh, Bushra (2001). The psychological reality of OCP-place in Arabic. Lg 77. 91106.Google Scholar
Gafos, Adamantios I. (1998). Eliminating long-distance consonantal spreading. NLLT 16. 223278.Google Scholar
Gallagher, Gillian (2010a). The perceptual basis of long-distance laryngeal restrictions. PhD dissertation, MIT.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, Gillian (2010b). Perceptual distinctness and long-distance laryngeal restrictions. Phonology 27. 435480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, Gillian & Coon, Jessica (2009). Distinguishing total and partial identity: evidence from Chol. NLLT 27. 545582.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, John A. (1976). Autosegmental phonology. PhD dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Goldwater, Sharon & Johnson, Mark (2003). Learning OT constraint rankings using a Maximum Entropy model. In Spenador, Jennifer, Eriksson, Anders & Dahl, Östen (eds.) Proceedings of the Stockholm Workshop on Variation within Optimality Theory. Stockholm: Stockholm University. 111120.Google Scholar
Graff, Peter & Jäger, Florian (2009). Locality and feature specificity in OCP effects: evidence from Ayamar, Dutch and Javanese. CLS 45. 127141.Google Scholar
Halle, Morris (1962). Phonology in a generative grammar. Word 18. 5472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, Jette G. (2004). Developmental sequences in the acquisition of English L2 syllable codas: a preliminary study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 26. 85124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Bruce & Wilson, Colin (2008). A maximum entropy model of phonotactics and phonotactic learning. LI 39. 379440.Google Scholar
Hudson, Grover (1995). Phonology of Ethiopian languages. In Goldsmith, John A. (ed.) The handbook of phonological theory. Cambridge, Mass. & Oxford: Blackwell. 782797.Google Scholar
Jaynes, E. T. (1983). Papers on probability, statistics, and statistical physics. Edited by Rosenkrantz, R. D.. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Jensen, John T. (1977). Yapese reference grammar. Honolulu: University Press of Hawaii.Google Scholar
Klein, Dan & Manning, Christopher (2003). Maxent models, conditional estimation, and optimization, without the magic. Tutorial presented at NAACL-03 and ACL-03.Google Scholar
Leben, William (1973). Suprasegmental phonology. PhD dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Lisker, Leigh & Abramson, Arthur S. (1967). Some effects of context on voice onset time in English stops. Language and Speech 10. 128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucca, Manuel F. de (1987). Diccionario practico aymara–castellano castellano–aymara. La Paz: Editorial Los Amigos del Libro.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. (1986). OCP effects: gemination and antigemination. LI 17. 207263.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. (1988). Feature geometry and dependency: a review. Phonetica 45. 84108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, John J. & Pater, Joe (eds.) (to appear). Harmonic Grammar and harmonic serialism. London: Equinox.Google Scholar
MacEachern, Margaret R. (1999). Laryngeal cooccurrence restrictions. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Manning, Christopher D. & Schütze, Hinrich (1999). Foundations of statistical natural language processing. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marcus, Gary F., Vijayan, S., Bandi Rao, S. & Vishton, P. M. (1999). Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science 283. 7780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mester, R. Armin (1986). Studies in tier structure. PhD dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Moreton, Elliott (2002). Structural constraints in the perception of English stop-sonorant clusters. Cognition 84. 5571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreton, Elliott (2008). Analytic bias and phonological typology. Phonology 25. 83127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreton, Elliott (2012). Inter- and intra-dimensional dependencies in implicit phonotactic learning. Journal of Memory and Language 67. 165183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pater, Joe (2009). Weighted constraints in generative linguistics. Cognitive Science 33. 9991035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pater, Joe, Bhatt, Rajesh & Potts, Chris (2007). Linguistic optimization. Ms, University of Massachusetts, Amherst. Available (April 2013) at http://people.umass.edu/pater/pater-bhatt-potts-hg07.pdf.Google Scholar
Pater, Joe & Moreton, Elliott (2012). Structure and substance in artificial-phonology learning. 2 parts. Language and Linguistics Compass 6. 686–701, 702718.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (1993). Dissimilarity in Arabic verbal roots. NELS 23. 367381.Google Scholar
Prince, Alan & Smolensky, Paul (1993). Optimality Theory: constraint interaction in generative grammar. Ms, Rutgers University & University of Colorado, Boulder. Published 2004, Malden, Mass. & Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rose, Sharon (1997). Theoretical issues in comparative Ethio-Semitic phonology and morphology. PhD dissertation, McGill University.Google Scholar
Rose, Sharon & King, Lisa (2007). Speech error elicitation and co-occurrence restrictions in two Ethiopian Semitic languages. Language and Speech 50. 451504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sagey, Elizabeth (1986). The representation of features and relations in nonlinear phonology. PhD dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Scholes, Robert J. (1966). Phonotactic grammaticality. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Straight, H. Stephen (1976) The acquisition of Maya phonology: variation in Yucatec child language. New York & London: Garland.Google Scholar
Thomas, Jacqueline M. C. (1963). Le parler Ngbaka de Bokanga: phonologie, morphologie, syntaxe. Paris: Paillart.Google Scholar
Treiman, Rebecca, Kessler, Brett, Knewasser, Stephanie, Tincoff, Ruth & Bowman, Margo (2000). English speakers' sensitivity to phonotactic patterns. In Broe, Michael B. & Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (eds.) Papers in laboratory phonology V: acquisition and the lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 269282.Google Scholar
Warkentin, Viola M. & Brend, Ruth M. (1974). Chol phonology. Linguistics 132. 87101.Google Scholar
Wilson, Colin (2006). Learning phonology with substantive bias: an experimental and computational study of velar palatalization. Cognitive Science 30. 945982.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed