Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T04:39:49.713Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Modelling incomplete neutralisation with weighted phonetic constraints

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 April 2019

Aaron Braver*
Affiliation:
Texas Tech University

Abstract

Incomplete neutralisation presents a problem for classical modular feed-forward grammars: it results in surface phonetic distinctions between phonologically neutralised segments. This paper argues for a model of incomplete neutralisation using two independently motivated theoretical devices: paradigm uniformity and weighted phonetic constraints. A case study is presented, showing that Japanese monomoraic lengthening results in incomplete neutralisation: when monomoraic nouns with short vowels are lengthened to fill a bimoraic minimality requirement, they reach a duration intermediate between that of unlengthened short vowels and underlyingly long vowels. The Japanese case has properties distinct from other classically cited examples of incomplete neutralisation such as final devoicing, which are not predicted by previous theories of neutralisation. The Weighted Paradigm Uniformity theory of incomplete neutralisation is shown to make four unique predictions, and is argued to better capture the typology of incomplete neutralisation.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

I thank Shigeto Kawahara and Will Bennett for their discussion of this project. I am also grateful to three anonymous reviewers and an associate editor for providing substantive comments and direction that greatly improved the paper. The Japanese data presented in §3 come from Braver & Kawahara (2014, 2016).

References

REFERENCES

Albright, Adam (2002a). Base selection in analogical change in Yiddish. BLS 28. 113.Google Scholar
Albright, Adam (2002b). The identification of bases in morphological paradigms. PhD dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Anderson, Stephen R. (1975). On the interaction of phonological rules of various types. JL 11. 3962.Google Scholar
Barnes, Jonathan (2006). Strength and weakness at the interface: positional neutralization in phonetics and phonology. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Behne, Dawn, Arai, Takayuki, Czigler, Peter & Sullivan, Kirk (1999). Vowel duration and spectra as perceptual cues to vowel quantity: a comparison of Japanese and Swedish. In Ohala, John J., Hasegawa, Yoko, Ohala, Manjari, Granville, Daniel & Bailey, Ashlee C. (eds.) Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Berkeley: University of California. 857860.Google Scholar
Benua, Laura (1997). Transderivational identity: phonological relations between words. PhD dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (2007). Diachronic phonology. In de Lacy, Paul (ed.) The Cambridge handbook of phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 497517.Google Scholar
Braver, Aaron (2013). Degrees of incompleteness in neutralization: paradigm uniformity in a phonetics with weighted constraints. PhD dissertation, Rutgers University.Google Scholar
Braver, Aaron (2014). Imperceptible incomplete neutralization: production, non-identifiability, and non-discriminability in American English flapping. Lingua 152. 2444.Google Scholar
Braver, Aaron & Kawahara, Shigeto (2014). Incomplete vowel lengthening in Japanese: a first study. WCCFL 31. 8695.Google Scholar
Braver, Aaron & Kawahara, Shigeto (2016). Incomplete neutralization in Japanese monomoraic lengthening. In Adam Albright & Michelle A. Fullwood (eds.) Proceedings of the 2014 Meeting on Phonology. http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/amp.v2i0.3757.Google Scholar
Chang, Steve S., Ohala, John J., Hansson, Gunnar, James, Benjamin, Lewis, Julie, Liaw, Lily, Urban, Margaret, Yu, Alan & Van Bik, Kenneth (1999). Vowel-dependent VOT variation: an experimental study. JASA 105. 1400.Google Scholar
Chen, Matthew (1970). Vowel length variation as a function of the voicing of the consonant environment. Phonetica 22. 129159.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam & Halle, Morris (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Dinnsen, Daniel A. & Charles-Luce, Jan (1984). Phonological neutralization, phonetic implementation and individual differences. JPh 12. 4960.Google Scholar
Dinnsen, Daniel A. & Garcia-Zamor, M. (1971). The three degrees of vowel length in German. Papers in Linguistics 4. 111126.Google Scholar
Dmitrieva, Olga (2005). Incomplete neutralization in Russian final devoicing: acoustic evidence from native speakers and second language learners. MA thesis, University of Kansas.Google Scholar
Fisher, William M. & Hirsh, Ira J. (1976). Intervocalic flapping in English. CLS 12. 183198.Google Scholar
Flemming, Edward (2001). Scalar and categorical phenomena in a unified model of phonetics and phonology. Phonology 18. 744.Google Scholar
Flemming, Edward (2002). Auditory representations in phonology. London & New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Flemming, Edward (2010). Modeling listeners: comments on Pluymaekers et al. and Scarborough. In Fougeron et al. (2010). 587605.Google Scholar
Fougeron, Cécile, Kühnert, Barbara, D'Imperio, Mariapaola & Vallée, Nathalie (eds.) (2010). Laboratory phonology 10. Berlin & New York: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Fougeron, Cécile & Steriade, Donca (1997). Does deletion of French schwa lead to neutralization of lexical distinctions? In Proceedings of the 5th European Conference on Speech Communication and Technology (Eurospeech 1997). Rhodes, Greece. 943946. http://www.isca-speech.org/archive/eurospeech_1997.Google Scholar
Fourakis, Marios & Iverson, Gregory K. (1984). On the ‘incomplete neutralization’ of German final obstruents. Phonetica 41. 140149.Google Scholar
Fox, Robert A. & Terbeek, Dale (1977). Dental flaps, vowel duration and rule ordering in American English. JPh 5. 2734.Google Scholar
Fujisaki, Hiroya, Nakamura, Kimie & Imoto, Toshiaki (1975). Auditory perception of duration of speech and non-speech stimuli. In Fant, G. & Tatham, M. A. A. (eds.) Auditory analysis and perception of speech. London: Academic Press. 197219.Google Scholar
Gerfen, Chip (2002). Andalusian codas. Probus 14. 247277.Google Scholar
Gianinnia, Antonella & Cinque, Umberto (1978). Phonetic status and phonemic function of the final devoiced stops in Polish. Naples: Laboratorio di Fonetica Sperimentale.Google Scholar
Goldinger, Stephan D. (1996). Words and voices: episodic traces in spoken word identification and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22. 11661183.Google Scholar
Goldrick, Matthew (2000). Turbid output representations and the unity of opacity. NELS 30. 231245.Google Scholar
Goldrick, Matthew & Blumstein, Sheila E. (2006). Cascading activation from phonological planning to articulatory processes: evidence from tongue twisters. Language and Cognitive Processes 21. 649683.Google Scholar
Gouskova, Maria & Hall, Nancy (2009). Acoustics of epenthetic vowels in Lebanese Arabic. In Parker, Steve (ed.) Phonological argumentation: essays on evidence and motivation. London: Equinox. 203225.Google Scholar
Haraguchi, Shosuke (1977). The tone pattern of Japanese: an autosegmental theory of tonology. Tokyo: Kaitakusha.Google Scholar
Herd, Wendy, Jongman, Allard & Sereno, Joan (2010). An acoustic and perceptual analysis of /t/ and /d/ flaps in American English. JPh 38. 504516.Google Scholar
Hirata, Yukari (2004). Effects of speaking rate on the vowel length distinction in Japanese. JPh 32. 565589.Google Scholar
Hirata, Yukari & Tsukada, Kimiko (2009). Effects of speaking rate and vowel length on formant frequency displacement in Japanese. Phonetica 66. 129149.Google Scholar
Hombert, Jean-Marie, Ohala, John J. & Ewan, William G. (1979). Phonetic explanations for the development of tones. Lg 55. 3758.Google Scholar
Itô, Junko (1990). Prosodic minimality in Japanese. CLS 26:2. 213239.Google Scholar
Ito, Junko & Mester, Armin (2003). Weak layering and word binarity. In Honma, Takeru, Okazaki, Masao, Tabata, Toshiyuki & Tanaka, Shin'ichi (eds.) A new century of phonology and phonological theory: a Festschrift for Professor Shosuke Haraguchi on the occasion of his sixtieth birthday. Tokyo: Kaitakusha. 2665.Google Scholar
Jassem, Wiktor & Richter, Lutosława (1989). Neutralization of voicing in Polish obstruents. JPh 17. 317325.Google Scholar
Johnson, Keith (1997). Speech perception without speaker normalization: an exemplar model. In Johnson, Keith & Mullenix, John W. (eds.) Talker variability in speech processing. San Diego: Academic Press. 145165.Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto (2015). The phonology of Japanese accent. In Kubozono, Haruo (ed.) The handbook of Japanese phonetics and phonology. Berlin: de Gruyter Mouton. 445492.Google Scholar
Keating, Patricia A. (1996). The phonology–phonetics interface. In Kleinhenz, Ursula (ed.) Interfaces in phonology. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag. 262278.Google Scholar
Kharlamov, Viktor (2012). Incomplete neutralization and task e ffects in experimentally-elicited speech: evidence from the production and perception of word-final devoicing in Russian. PhD thesis, University of Ottawa.Google Scholar
Kingston, John & Diehl, Randy L. (1994). Phonetic knowledge. Lg 70. 419454.Google Scholar
Kinoshita, Keisuke, Behne, Dawn & Arai, Takayuki (2002). Duration and F0 as perceptual cues to Japanese vowel quantity. Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Spoken Language Processing (Interspeech 2002). 757760.Google Scholar
Kirby, James P. (2010). Cue selection and category restructuring in sound change. PhD dissertation, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Klatt, Dennis H. & Cooper, William E. (1975). Perception of segment duration in sentence contexts. In Cohen, A. & Nooteboom, S. G. (eds.) Structure and process in speech perception. Berlin: Springer. 6989.Google Scholar
Kluender, Keith R., Diehl, Randy L. & Wright, Beverly A. (1988). Vowel-length differences before voiced and voiceless consonants: an auditory explanation. JPh 16. 153169.Google Scholar
Koenig, Laura L., Fuchs, Susanne & Lucero, Jorge C. (2011). Effects of consonant manner and vowel height on intraoral pressure and articulatory contact at voicing offset and onset for voiceless obstruents. JASA 129. 32333244.Google Scholar
Kubozono, Haruo (1993). The organization of Japanese prosody. Tokyo: Kurosio.Google Scholar
Lacerda, Francisco (1995). The perceptual-magnet effect: an emergent consequence of exemplar-based phonetic memory. In Elenius, Kjell & Branderud, Peter (eds.) Proceedings of the 13th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Vol. 2. Stockholm: KTH & Stockholm University. 140147.Google Scholar
Legendre, Géraldine, Miyata, Yoshiro & Smolensky, Paul (1990). Harmonic Grammar: a formal multi-level connectionist theory of linguistic well-formedness: an application. In Proceedings of the 12th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale: Erlbaum. 884891.Google Scholar
Lindblom, Björn (1990). Explaining phonetic variation: a sketch of the H&H theory. In Hardcastle, William J. & Marchal, Alain (eds.) Speech production and speech modelling. Dordrecht: Kluwer. 403439.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. (2007). Hidden generalizations: phonological opacity in Optimality Theory. Sheffield & Bristol, Conn.: Equinox.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. (2009). The P-Map in harmonic serialism. Ms, University of Massachusetts, Amherst. Available as ROA-1052 from the Rutgers Optimality Archive.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. & Prince, Alan (1986). Prosodic morphology. Ms, University of Massachusetts, Amherst & Brandeis University.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. & Prince, Alan (1993). Prosodic morphology: constraint interaction and satisfaction. Ms, University of Massachusetts, Amherst & Rutgers University. Available as ROA-482 from the Rutgers Optimality Archive.Google Scholar
McCawley, James D. (1968). The phonological component of a grammar of Japanese. The Hague & Paris: Mouton.Google Scholar
McCawley, James D. (1977). Accent in Japanese. In Hyman, Larry M. (ed.) Studies in stress and accent. Los Angeles: Department of Linguistics, University of Southern California. 261302.Google Scholar
Manaster Ramer, Alexis (1996a). Report on Alexis’ dreams – bad as well as good. JPh 24. 513519.Google Scholar
Manaster Ramer, Alexis (1996b). A letter from an incompletely neutral phonologist. JPh 24. 477489.Google Scholar
Mańczak, Witold (1958). Tendences générales des changements analogiques. Lingua 7. 298–325, 387420.Google Scholar
Matsui, Mayuki (2015). Roshia-go ni okeru yuusei-sei no tairitsu to tairitsu no jyakka: onkyo to chikaku. [Voicing contrast and contrast reduction in Russian: acoustics and perception.] PhD thesis, Hiroshima University.Google Scholar
Mester, Armin (1990). Patterns of truncation. LI 21. 478485Google Scholar
Mitleb, Fares M. (1981a). Segmental and non-segmental structure in phonetics: evidence from foreign accent. PhD thesis, Indiana University.Google Scholar
Mitleb, Fares M. (1981b). Temporal correlates of ‘voicing’ and its neutralization in German. Research in Phonetics 2. 173191.Google Scholar
Moreton, Elliott (2004). Realization of the English postvocalic [voice] contrast in F 1 and F 2. JPh 32. 133.Google Scholar
Mori, Yoko (2002). Lengthening of Japanese monomoraic nouns. JPh 30. 689708.Google Scholar
Nooteboom, S. G. & Doodeman, G. J. N. (1980). Production and perception of vowel length in spoken sentences. JASA 67. 276287.Google Scholar
Nosofsky, Robert M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identification–categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 115. 3957.Google Scholar
Oostendorp, Marc van (2008). Incomplete devoicing in formal phonology. Lingua 118. 13621374.Google Scholar
Pater, Joe (2009). Weighted constraints in generative linguistics. Cognitive Science 33. 9991035.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (2001a). Exemplar dynamics: word frequency, lenition and contrast. In Bybee, Joan & Hopper, Paul (eds.) Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: Benjamins. 137157.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (2001b). Stochastic phonology. Glot International 5. 195207.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (2002). Word-specific phonetics. In Gussenhoven, Carlos & Warner, Natasha (eds.) Laboratory Phonology 7. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 101139.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. & Beckman, Mary E. (1988). Japanese tone structure. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Piroth, Hans Georg & Janker, Peter M. (2004). Speaker-dependent differences in voicing and devoicing of German obstruents. JPh 32. 81109.Google Scholar
Port, Robert F. (1996). The discreteness of phonetic elements and formal linguistics: response to A. Manaster Ramer. JPh 24. 491511.Google Scholar
Port, Robert F. & Crawford, Penny (1989). Incomplete neutralization and pragmatics in German. JPh 17. 257282.Google Scholar
Port, Robert F., Mitleb, Fares & O'Dell, Michael L. (1981). Neutralization of obstruent voicing is incomplete. JASA 70. S13.Google Scholar
Port, Robert F. & O'Dell, Michael L. (1985). Neutralization of syllable-final voicing in German. JPh 13. 455471.Google Scholar
Poser, William J. (1984). The phonetics and phonology of tone and intonation in Japanese. PhD dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Poser, William J. (1990). Evidence for foot structure in Japanese. Lg 66. 78105.Google Scholar
Posner, Michael I. & Keele, Steven W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology 77. 353363.Google Scholar
Prince, Alan & Smolensky, Paul (1993). Optimality Theory: constraint interaction in generative grammar. Ms, Rutgers University & University of Colorado, Boulder. Published 2004, Malden, Mass. & Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Reed, Stephen K. (1972). Pattern recognition and categorization. Cognitive Psychology 3. 382407.Google Scholar
Rialland, Annie (1986). Schwa et syllabes en français. In Wetzels, Leo & Sezer, Engin (eds.) Studies in compensatory lengthening. Dordrecht: Foris. 187226.Google Scholar
Rosch, Eleanor (1973). Natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 4. 328350.Google Scholar
Röttger, T. B., Winter, B., Grawunder, S., Kirby, J. & Grice, M. (2014). Assessing incomplete neutralization of final devoicing in German. JPh 43. 1125.Google Scholar
Scarborough, Rebecca (2003). Lexical confusability and degree of coarticulation. BLS 29. 367378.Google Scholar
Scarborough, Rebecca (2010). Lexical and contextual predictability: confluent effects on the production of vowels. In Fougeron et al. 557586.Google Scholar
Slowiaczek, Louisa M. & Dinnsen, Daniel A. (1985). On the neutralizing status of Polish word-final devoicing. JPh 13. 325341.Google Scholar
Slowiaczek, Louisa M. & Szymanska, Helena J. (1989). Perception of word-final devoicing in Polish. JPh 17. 205212.Google Scholar
Smith, J. David & Minda, John Paul (2000). Thirty categorization results in search of a model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 26. 327.Google Scholar
Steriade, Donca (2000). Paradigm uniformity and the phonetics–phonology boundary. In Broe, Michael B. & Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (eds.) Papers in laboratory phonology V: acquisition and the lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 313334.Google Scholar
Steriade, Donca (2009). The phonology of perceptibility effects: the P-map and its consequences for constraint organization. In Hanson, Kristin & Inkelas, Sharon (eds.) The nature of the word: studies in honor of Paul Kiparsky. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. 151179.Google Scholar
Sugito, Miyoko (1982). Tōkyō akusento ni okeru ‘hana’ to ‘hana’ no sēsē to chikaku. [The production and perception of ‘flower’ and ‘nose’ with a Tokyo accent.] In Ni-hongo akusento no kenkyū. [Studies on Japanese accent.] Tokyo: Sanseido. 182201.Google Scholar
Syrett, Kristen & Kawahara, Shigeto (2013). Production and perception of listener- oriented clear speech in child language. Journal of Child Language 41. 13731389.Google Scholar
Taylor, Dennis Q. (1975). The inadequacy of bipolarity and distinctive features: the German ‘voiced/voiceless’ consonants. In Reich, Peter A. (ed.) The Second LACUS Forum. Columbia, S.C.: Hornbeam. 107119.Google Scholar
Thomas, Erik R. (2000). Spectral differences in /ai/ offsets conditioned by voicing of the following consonant. JPh 28. 125.Google Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. (1987). An introduction to Japanese phonology. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. (1995). Final accent vs. no accent: utterance-final neutralization in Tokyo Japanese. JPh 23. 487499.Google Scholar
Warner, Natasha, Good, Erin, Jongman, Allard & Sereno, Joan (2006). Orthographic vs. morphological incomplete neutralization effects. JPh 34. 285293.Google Scholar
Warner, Natasha, Jongman, Allard, Sereno, Joan & Kemps, Rachèl (2004). Incomplete neutralization and other sub-phonemic durational differences in production and perception: evidence from Dutch. JPh 32. 251276.Google Scholar
Yu, Alan C. L. (2007). Understanding near mergers: the case of morphological tone in Cantonese. Phonology 24. 187214.Google Scholar
Yu, Alan C. L. (2011). Contrast reduction. In Goldsmith, John, Riggle, Jason & Yu, Alan C. L. (eds.) The handbook of phonological theory. 2nd edn. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. 291318.Google Scholar
Zsiga, Elizabeth C. (2000). Phonetic alignment constraints: consonant overlap and palatalization in English and Russian. JPh 28. 69102.Google Scholar
Zue, Victor W. & Laferriere, Martha (1979). Acoustical study of medial /t,d/ in American English. JASA 66. 10391050.Google Scholar