Introduction
“An ice cold glass of pure racism”
—White nationalist participating in a milk-chugging party
“Studying human genetic diversity is easier in a society where diversity is clearly valued and celebrated.”
—Dr. John Novembre, University of Chicago evolutionary biologist
In early 2017, shortly after the inauguration of Donald Trump as president of the United States, a group of alt-right White nationalists gathered at the installation of an anti-Trump art piece to hold a “milk party” at which they would chug cartons of cow’s milk together (Harmon, Reference Harmon2018b; Volkov, Reference Volkov2018). The purpose of this display was to highlight and celebrate lactose tolerance, a genetic trait thought to be more common in White people than in others. Unsurprisingly, in addition to consuming lactose together, the White nationalists at this gathering voiced their feelings of racial superiority with racist, antisemitic, sexist, and homophobic rants. Their ideology of racial hierarchy was bolstered, in their view, by science that draws a correlation between White racial identity and the genetic basis for lactose tolerance in adulthood (e.g., Gerbault et al., Reference Gerbault2011; Swagerty et al., Reference Swagerty, Walling and Klein2002).Footnote 1 These views were widely shared and promulgated among alt-right supporters on social media (e.g., the trending Twitter hashtag #milktwitter) and internet discussion boards (e.g., a 4chan board hosted by the user “Enter the Milk Zone” that featured hate speech telling those who “can’t drink milk” to “go back” to the lands of their ancestors).
Alt-right ideology is characterized by valuing White ethnonationalism and a return to “traditional values” while embracing science and technology (Southern Poverty Law Center, n.d.). Indeed, adherents of alt-right ideology rely heavily on the use of social media and the creation of internet memes to connect with one another and communicate their ideas. Furthermore, they endorse racism that they deem to be scientifically justified, considering themselves “race realists” who believe in “human biodiversity”—in other words, a strong belief in the large genetic differences between different “races” (Panofsky et al., Reference Panofsky, Dasgupta and Iturriaga2021). Even if an individual morally objects to the alt-right’s racial ideology, they may have a difficult time disputing such “scientific justification” because the relationship between genes and race is complex and not particularly well understood among most people. Even those who do understand the relationship better—human geneticists—feel deep discomfort about engaging with and disputing the racist interpretations of their science by White nationalists (Harmon, Reference Harmon2018a).
Part of the challenge of thinking about genes and race is that it is perhaps difficult to separate the ideas that (a) racial categories clearly hold individual, social, and political significance and (b) racial categorizations are commonly based on perceptions of physical features and ancestry. And yet the science broadly suggests that these notions should be separated. Geneticists have demonstrated that “racial classifications are inadequate descriptors of the distribution of genetic variation in our species” (Tishkoff & Kidd, Reference Tishkoff and Kidd2004, p. 522). That is, although a miniscule amount of genetic variation across humans may be correlated with socially defined racial categories, such categories are themselves genetically heterogeneous and do not clearly map onto the patterns of human genetic variation in general (Foster & Sharp, Reference Foster and Sharp2004; Jorde & Wooding, Reference Jorde and Wooding2004; Morning, Reference Morning2011). Yudell et al. (Reference Yudell, Roberts, DeSalle and Tishkoff2016) further clarify the distinct notions of heredity and race:
It is important to distinguish ancestry from a taxonomic notion such as race. Ancestry is a process-based concept, a statement about an individual’s relationship to other individuals in their genealogical history; thus, it is a very personal understanding of one’s genomic heritage. Race, on the other hand, is a pattern-based concept that has led scientists and laypersons alike to draw conclusions about hierarchical organization of humans, which connect an individual to a larger preconceived geographically circumscribed or socially constructed group. (p. 565)
It is perhaps unsurprising that for their brand of racial ideology, alt-righters and other White nationalists focus far less on the genetic science that debunks notions of clear racial classifications than on some minor “racial” correlations (like lactose tolerance). But it may be that thinking about genetics through a racial or intergroup lens could have a similar impact on the racial attitudes of the broader population—perhaps especially because of the complexities of the science. Recent research suggests that Americans live in a period of heightened hostile racial rhetoric, and they appear to be increasingly accepting of explicitly negative racial cues (Valentino et al., Reference Valentino, Neuner and Vandenbroek2018). Furthermore, racialized cues—subtle or overt—can impact individual attitudes and political decision-making (e.g., Hutchings & Valentino, Reference Hutchings and Valentino2004; Hutchings & Jardina, Reference Hutchings and Jardina2009; Mendelberg, Reference Mendelberg2001). In this research, we explore how information that is meant to convey scientific findings in an objective, factual, and nonideological manner can impact racial attitudes. Although science itself can often be politicized (e.g., Druckman, Reference Druckman2017), here we suggest that in an intergroup context, the mere communication of scientific information—even without engaging in explicit politicization—can have insidious effects on individuals’ racial attitudes. That is, we argue that subtle shifts in the coverage and explanation of scientific findings can lead to shifts in broader group perceptions that are socially and politically impactful. Our research is thus motivated by the notion that “language matters, and the scientific language of race has a considerable influence on how the public (which includes scientists) understands human diversity” (Yudell et al., Reference Yudell, Roberts, DeSalle and Tishkoff2016, p. 565).
Modernizing old-fashioned biological racism
Recent research suggests that old-fashioned racism is back (Huddy & Feldman, Reference Huddy and Feldman2009; Jardina & Piston, Reference Jardina, Piston and Redlawsk2019; Jardina & Piston, Reference Jardina and Piston2021; Newman et al., Reference Newman2021; Piston, Reference Piston2010; Tesler, Reference Tesler2013; Valentino et al., Reference Valentino, Neuner and Vandenbroek2018). Explicit, old-fashioned racism that forms the basis for the ideology of White supremacy was particularly accepted and commonplace in the pre–civil rights era, and it is grounded in basic beliefs about the biological inferiority of Black people to White people. The consequences of such beliefs include a social preference for distance between Whites and Blacks, as well as political preferences for formalized, policy-based racial segregation and discrimination (Bobo & Kluegel, Reference Bobo, Kluegel, Tuch and Martin1997; McConahay, Reference McConahay, Dovidio and Gaertner1986; McConahay & Hough, Reference McConahay and Hough1976). In contrast, modern racism (also variably called and measured as symbolic racism or racial resentment), which became more commonplace in the post–civil rights era, is justified primarily by a “moral” feeling that Black people violate traditional American values—rather than being biologically inferior per se (e.g., Kinder & Sears, Reference Kinder and Sears1981). Whereas for decades in the post–civil rights period, the racial politics literature largely suggested that “old racism” had gone underground in favor of a more symbolic and implicit “modern racism” (e.g., Mendelberg, Reference Mendelberg2001), contemporary evidence in today’s political landscape suggests the resurgence of the acceptability of explicit racism (e.g., Valentino et al., Reference Valentino, Neuner and Vandenbroek2018).Footnote 2 Indeed, in national surveys, over 40% of Americans express the belief that the economic and educational gap between White and Black people can be explained at least “a little” by fundamental genetic differences of race (Huddy & Feldman, Reference Huddy and Feldman2009). Clearly, at minimum, Americans are open to the idea that biology justifies inequality.
Although the measurement and conceptualization of racial attitudes vary considerably,Footnote 3 our research focuses on the potentially justifying role of “neutral” biological science findings on negative intergroup attitudes. Therefore, our approach in two experiments is to assess both old-fashioned and modern forms of racism as potential outcomes for thinking about biology through a racialized lens—in part as an initial exploration of how “old” and “new” might meet in the scientific arena.
There is reason to think that the presentation of ostensibly objective scientific information regarding genetics—especially information that reinforces the perception of biological differences between racial groups—leaves an opening for justification of racial disparities. First, genetic science has proliferated in recent decades since the inception of the Human Genome Project in 1990, and this genomic revolution has been accompanied by increased media coverage. A content analysis of coverage by the New York Times and the Associated Press between 1985 and 2008 shows not only significant increases in articles about genes, but parallel increases in discussions of race in such articles (Phelan et al., Reference Phelan, Link and Feldman2013). Accordingly, surveys suggest that among Americans in general—even across ideological, racial, and socioeconomic lines—there are high levels of endorsement of genes as a cause of health and social outcomes (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Smith and Hibbing2018; Shostak et al., Reference Shostak, Freese, Link and Phelan2009).
Second, genetic science findings, particularly about health, are presented as objective, factual, and nonideological. Phelan et al. (Reference Phelan, Link and Feldman2013) find that news articles that report on genetic differences along racial lines in the context of health (versus other types of outcomes) are less likely to mention issues of racism or ethics, but more likely to affirm genes as causal. These articles therefore suggest that race is not only a valid but a critical way to categorize people, communicated with the authority of science and medicine (Morning, Reference Morning2008). That is, scientific research and media coverage that endorse any genetic bases of racial difference (health or otherwise) could lead to broader perceptions that there are more general biological differences between racial groups (see Duster, Reference Duster2003).Footnote 4 Certainly, it appears that such generalizations motivate not only the theatrics of White nationalists’ “milk parties,” but also provide the veneer of scientific legitimacy to their broader ideology of racial hierarchy.
Finally, when racial group membership is presented as biologically determined, racial inequities are seen as more acceptable, and interest in social interactions with racial out-group members decreases (Williams & Eberhardt, Reference Williams and Eberhardt2008). Critically, such effects are largely independent of individual differences in racial prejudice, which suggests that genetic justifications for racial inequality may not simply be a tool of the alt-right, but rather a cognitive tendency of the broader public.
Modern advances in science and technology may thus play an outsize—if unintended—role in the resurgence of old racism by providing “evidence” for claiming fundamental differences along racial lines, as well as the ideological tools to justify racialized inequities. Our research builds on the foundation in the racial attitudes literature to examine how framing scientific findings in ways that allude to genetically based racial differences in health and behavior might have a more general impact on racial attitudes and political preferences.
The psychological and attitudinal consequences of genetic attributions
Why exactly would genetic attributions be associated with racism? In general, people tend to be genetic essentialists—that is, learning about genetic contributions for human outcomes leads to cognitive biases to perceive those outcomes as immutable and determined, having a specific cause, homogeneous, discrete, and natural (Dar-Nimrod & Heine, Reference Dar-Nimrod and Heine2011). In beliefs about race and inequality, such cognitive biases can be broken down into the constituent components of biological determinism and racial essentialism to form an “ideological double helix” that shapes racial attitudes (Byrd & Hughey, Reference Byrd and Hughey2015).
Biological determinism is the idea that genes are destiny. When people are told that differences in traits and outcomes are due to genetic differences, they frequently express those outcomes to be inevitable (Jeong, Reference Jeong2007; Phelan et al., Reference Phelan, Cruz-Rojas and Reiff2002). This means that when the concept of race is geneticized, race is perceived as inherited and caused by clear biological markers.Footnote 5 But even among gene-based diseases (which represent only a small subset of gene-based outcomes), only about 2% are monogenic—that is, a particular gene holds a one-to-one relationship with an outcome (e.g., Huntington’s disease; Jablonka & Lamb, Reference Jablonka and Lamb2005; Zoghbi & Orr, Reference Zoghbi and Orr2000). However, the vast majority of genotype-phenotype relationships are highly complex. Phenotypes (i.e., observable outcomes) typically emerge from the interaction of many genes, when certain environmental conditions are present, and personal choice factors regarding one’s environment that may be both influenced by genes and influence genetic expression. Indeed, many of the ways in which genes relate to human outcomes can be considered “weak genetic explanations” (Turkheimer, Reference Turkheimer1998), in which an outcome is known to have a genetic basis but the specific mechanisms that translate genotype to phenotype are unknown. Decades of genetics research suggest that nearly all human behaviors are, to some extent, heritable, including cigarette smoking (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Thornton and Pedersen2000), divorce (Jocklin et al., Reference Jocklin, McGue and Lykken1996), voting behavior (Fowler et al., Reference Fowler, Baker and Dawes2008), and political ideology (Alford et al., Reference Alford, Funk and Hibbing2005). But it is important to keep in mind that the specific genes involved, the precise pathways from genes to outcome, and the particular environmental conditions conducive to genetic expression are complex and not yet well understood. As noted earlier, racial categories do not map onto clear genetic markers (Tishkoff & Kidd, Reference Tishkoff and Kidd2004; Yudell et al., Reference Yudell, Roberts, DeSalle and Tishkoff2016), but belief in genetic determinism is positively associated with prejudice, negative racial stereotyping, and nationalism (Keller, Reference Keller2005).
Racial essentialism follows from the belief that genetic variation is immutable and innate, and so any associated outcomes are also immutable and innate—that is, genes form the “essence” of who a person is. In other words, genetic essentialism “reduces the self to a molecular entity, equating human beings, in all their social, historical, and moral complexity, with their genes” (Nelkin & Lindee, Reference Nelkin and Lindee1995, p. 2). This translates to the group level, such that perceived differences between social groups, insofar as they are attributed to biological causes, are seen as natural differences in group “essence” (Levens et al., Reference Leyens, Rodriguez‐Perez, Rodriguez‐Torres, Gaunt, Paladino, Vaes and Demoulin2001). Despite evidence that racial and ethnic “essences” do not exist, the perception of difference along biological essence persists (Gil-White, Reference Gil-White2001). Essentialist thinking has been shown to increase perceptions of group homogeneity and stereotype endorsement about social and racial groups (Bastian & Haslam, Reference Bastian and Haslam2006; Haslam et al., Reference Haslam, Bastian, Bain and Kashima2006; Haslam et al., Reference Haslam, Rothschild and Ernst2000, Reference Haslam, Rothschild and Ernst2002; Hong et al., Reference Hong, Levy, Chiu, Hong, Levy and Chiu2003). For instance, compared to perceptions of race as socially constructed, essentialist views of race lead to greater perceived social distance and less cultural overlap between Asian Americans and American identity and culture (No et al., Reference No, Hong, Liao, Lee, Wood and Chao2008). Moreover, research by Kimel et al. (Reference Kimel, Huesmann, Kunst and Halperin2016) demonstrates that emphasizing genetic differences between ethnic in-group and out-group members can increase behavioral aggression and support for hawkish policies.
Clearly, there are psychological and political reasons to be cautious and concerned about widespread and simplified dissemination of genetics research. Indeed, Schmalor et al. (Reference Schmalor, Cheung and Heine2021) suggest that scientific narratives (i.e., genetics research that describes the geographic clustering of particular allele frequencies) can increase beliefs that group stereotypes are largely due to genetics. Are there ways to mitigate the psychological and attitudinal biases that can stem from reports of genetics research in racialized contexts? Dar-Nimrod and Heine (Reference Dar-Nimrod and Heine2011), in an extensive review of genetic essentialism, point out that “although the scientific importance of genetic research is beyond dispute, taking steps to ensure a reduction in the undesirable cognitions and behaviors that have so far dogged the study of genetics will go a long way towards fulfilling the great promise encompassed in such research.” (p. 25) We respond to the call by Dar-Nimrod and Heine, who note the lack of experimental studies on the open question of whether the negative effects of deterministic, essentialist thinking on racial attitudes can be reduced through interventions that frame genetics research in more contextualized ways.Footnote 6 There is reason to think that such interventions could be effective, with at least one study showing that messages portraying mental illness as an outcome of both genes and environment (versus purely genes) reduced perceptions of danger associated with schizophrenics (Walker & Read, Reference Walker and Read2002). Along these lines, we investigate whether intergroup attitudes can be modulated by varying the type of narrative regarding the relationship between genes and social group outcomes.
The current research
We have two primary goals with the current research, which we address with two experiments. The first goal is to investigate whether it is possible to frame genetic findings in a way that reduces the negative impact on racial attitudes. Specifically, we compare frames of the same racialized genetics research as either deterministic (i.e., simple and causal) or conditional (i.e., complex and contingent on multiple genes and gene by environment interactions). Some previous experimental studies suggest that conditional or contextualized genetic explanations for mental illness and obesity can improve attitudes regarding those outcomes—albeit without the racialized context (Teachman et al., Reference Teachman, Gapinski, Brownell, Rawlins and Jeyaram2003; Walker & Read, Reference Walker and Read2002). We extend the existing work by investigating whether such conditional narratives can also lead to more tolerant attitudes if genetic attributions are made for racial group differences in health and behavioral outcomes. We expect that deterministic genetic narratives will replicate much of the prior work examining genetic essentialism in racial contexts by increasing racist attitudes. We also explore the possibility that conditional genetic narratives will reduce the negative impact on racial attitudes of presenting racialized genetic differences.
A second, more exploratory aim is to conduct a theoretically stringent test of the idea that genetic narratives in racial contexts will lead to higher levels of racism. That is, the literature on intergroup effects of genetic determinism and essentialism largely suggests that genetic explanations for racial differences lead quite consistently to more stereotyping, perceptions of out-group homogeneity, and prejudice (Condit et al., Reference Condit, Parrott, Bates, Bevan and Achter2004; Dar-Nimrod & Heine, Reference Dar-Nimrod and Heine2011; Heine et al., Reference Heine, Dar-Nimrod, Cheung and Proulx2017). However, genetic attributions for some outcomes appear to lead to more tolerant attitudes—that is, “born that way” narratives can elicit greater sympathy for and diminished condemnation of members of typically stigmatized groups. For example, genetic (versus behavioral) explanations for obesity led to lower implicit anti-fat attitudes and greater explicit pro-fat attitudes (Teachman et al., Reference Teachman, Gapinski, Brownell, Rawlins and Jeyaram2003). In addition, genetic explanations for violence or criminality have been associated with lower perceptions of culpability and preferences for less punitive consequences (Cheung & Heine, Reference Cheung and Heine2015; Heath et al., Reference Heath, Stone, Darley and Grannemann2003; Monterosso et al., Reference Monterosso, Royzman and Schwartz2005). Therefore, we examine genetic attributions for outcomes that have been associated with greater tolerance (i.e., obesity and violence) in a racialized context to test the potential strength or persistence of racist attitudes. Our expectation is that the negative attitudinal effects of linking race and genetics may override any positive attitudinal effects of linking traits and behaviors (like obesity and violence) to genetics, consistent with maintenance of the existing racial hierarchy and justification of racialized ideology (Jost & Banaji, 2004; Pratto et al., Reference Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth and Malle1994).
Study 1
Our initial study examines the effects of deterministic versus conditional narratives about racialized genetic associations with obesity on a measure of modern racial prejudice, as well as policy attitudes in the domain of health care. Specifically, we make use of modified news articles that highlight racial variation in the prevalence of genetic variants related to obesity. With this study, we aim to test two primary hypotheses:
H1: Individuals who read news articles emphasizing a deterministic genetic narrative about obesity will be more likely to report negative attitudes toward African Americans and more negative attitudes toward comprehensive health care policy (than those who receive conditional narratives or unrelated news articles).
H2: Individuals who read news articles emphasizing a conditional genetic narrative about obesity will be less likely to report negative attitudes toward African Americans and less negative attitudes toward comprehensive health care policy (than those who receive deterministic narratives or unrelated news articles).
Methods
Participants and experimental design
Data for our initial study were collected in May 2018 with an undergraduate student sample (N = 146). Participants were recruited to participate in a survey on how “media coverage of academic research impacts the public’s attitudes” for course credit. Several demographic questions that have been shown to be correlated with both racism and deterministic attitudes (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Smith and Hibbing2018) were asked of the respondents following the post-treatment measures. Survey respondents were asked to best describe their marital status (single, married, widowed, separated, divorced, remarried, living together). A majority of the respondents were single (93.15%), followed by living together (4.79%), married (1.37%), and divorced (0.68%). Respondents were asked to report their age (M = 21.40, SD = 2.51); the highest level of education they had achieved (61.90% had some college, 19.73% had completed college, 12.93% had completed high school, 2.04 reported other, 1.36% had completed some technical school, 1.36% had completed some high school, 0.68% had completed elementary school); their gender (46.26% reported being female, 51.02% male, 0.68% gender queer/gender nonconforming, 0.68% transgender female, and 1.36% transgender male); and their income (open response, a majority of respondents reported an income less than $50,000 a year). In addition, respondents were asked to rank their political ideology along the liberalism/conservatism scale (from strongly liberal = 1 to strongly conservative = 7; M = 3.44, SD = 1.53).Footnote 7 Summary statistics for each item are reported in Appendix A, Table 2.
Note: Robust standard errors in parentheses.
† White is the reference category.
** p < .01;
* p < .05;
+ p < .1 (two-tailed).
Participants were randomly assigned to one of three possible conditions, all of which entailed reading a news article (see Appendix A, Table 1 for the distribution of treatments across respondents). The two experimental conditions varied the framing of a genetics research narrative (deterministic versus conditional), and the control condition text was unrelated to genetics research (see Appendix A for full treatment materials).
Deterministic obesity condition
Participants assigned to the deterministic condition read an article titled “If you’re obese, a single gene may be to blame,” which described a genetics study linking a gene called ankyrin-B to “larger-than-normal” fat cells. The text emphasized a deterministic relationship between the gene and obesity, stating that “a single gene could be at the root of why some people are overweight.” The article also described the gene as affecting 8.4% of African Americans compared to 1.3% of Caucasians, and it was accompanied by a picture of a torso of an overweight Black person.
Conditional obesity condition
Participants assigned to the conditional condition read an article titled “If you’re obese, genes in addition to diet and exercise may be to blame,” which described the same genetics study linking ankyrin-B to “oversized” fat cells. Like the deterministic health treatment article, the conditional treatment article was also accompanied by a picture of a torso of an overweight Black person, and it stated that the proportion of African Americans carrying the ankyrin-B gene was higher than that of Caucasians. However, this article included conditional language regarding the research, stating that “there’s a lot more research on the gene that still needs to be done. Researchers will need to look into the family histories, physical characteristics, and metabolism of those with forms of the gene in order to truly figure out how it will affect people in addition to their exercise and diet habits.” That is, the conditional treatment highlighted that the outcome is conditioned by other environmental factors in addition to genetic factors, as well as personal choice.
Control condition
Participants assigned to the control condition read an article unrelated to genetics research. The control condition text was instead about summer homework, titled “Summer homework: Seeing vacation homework from the perspectives of educator and parent.” The article discussed the purpose and procrastination of summer homework, as well as other kinds of learning that can occur over summer vacations without assigned schoolwork. The purpose of this condition was to provide a baseline assessment of participants’ racial attitudes and policy preferences without priming them with genetics research regarding health, and without priming perceptions of African Americans.
Outcome measures
Following the experimental treatment, all participants responded to a multiple-choice manipulation check item to ensure they had accurately read and understood the main themes of the article they were given.Footnote 8 Then participants responded to attitudinal measures that were presented in random order (see Appendix A for all item wordings).Footnote 9
Symbolic racism
Racial attitudes were measured using the eight-item symbolic racism scale (Henry & Sears, Reference Henry and Sears2002) designed to assess the extent to which the respondent expresses a modern form of racism, predicated upon the belief that African Americans in present-day America do not experience racial discrimination and thus restitution is no longer justified (e.g., “It’s really a matter of some people not trying hard enough; if blacks would only try harder they could be just as well off as whites” on a range from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree).Footnote 10 A scale was constructed using the average score of the eight items (see Appendix A for the complete scale items and coding rules); higher values reflect more racist attitudes toward African Americans.
Health care policy attitudes
We expected that genetically deterministic perceptions of obesity could also affect attitudes about health care policy, including access to health care. Thus, we assessed health policy attitudes by asking respondents their degree of support for universal, guaranteed health care. Specifically, we asked respondents the extent to which they agreed with this statement: “Some people say that health care should be a right for all people and not a privilege for those who are insured by their workplace or participate in some other private plan. Others say that the tax burden in this country is already high and it is unreasonable to expect people who are paying a part of their own private insurance plan to also pay for other people. How do you feel about universal, guaranteed health care?” (1 = strongly oppose universal, guaranteed health care to 7 = strongly support universal, guaranteed health care).
Results
Treatment effects on symbolic racism
In Table 1, we report the results of ordinary least squares regression models (with robust standard errors in parentheses) that assess the effects of the deterministic and conditional treatments on symbolic racism. Consistent with our expectations (H1), the results suggest that those who read the deterministic narrative are more likely to express symbolically racist views of African Americans than those who read the control narrative (p < .05, two-tailed), controlling for respondent race/ethnicity, age, education, conservatism, gender, and income. However, we also find that individuals who receives the conditional narrative emphasizing genetic factors in addition to environmental factors and personal choice are marginally more likely to report higher levels of symbolic racism than the control group (p < .10, two-tailed), while accounting for respondent race/ethnicity and other covariates, which is inconsistent with our expectation (H2).
Treatment effects on health care policy attitudes
Next, we examine the association between the treatment conditions and support for universal health care. As reported in Model 2 of Table 1, the results suggest that reading the deterministic narrative about obesity is associated with significantly lower support for universal health care compared to the control condition (p < .05, two-tailed), while additionally accounting for differences in respondent race/ethnicity, age, education, conservatism, gender, and income. On the other hand, the conditional narrative is not associated with lower support for universal health care compared to the control (p > .10, two-tailed).
The results of Study 1 provide partial support for our hypotheses. That is, when respondents are exposed to a deterministic genetic narrative about obesity in a racialized context, they express higher levels of symbolic racism. But the conditional genetic narrative also leads to marginally higher expressions of racism. It may be that simply priming the reader by referencing race in any form increases expressions of racism. Perhaps because both the deterministic and conditional narratives highlight genetic differences between racial groups (relative to an unrelated control condition), such genetic narratives unsurprisingly elicit greater racism. We see a greater effect of the conditional genetic treatment on policy attitudes, however. Whereas the deterministic genetic treatment leads to more restrictive views on universal health care, the conditional genetic treatment does not lead to any more restrictive views on universal health care than the control treatment. These initial results thus suggest the possibility that contextualizing the effects of genes in racialized narratives could affect consequential downstream attitudes.
Study 2
The second study builds on our initial study in several ways, addressing questions that were left open in Study 1. First, because our first study used a between-subjects design, we cannot know whether participants’ racial attitudes were changed by the treatments or whether they were simply primed. Indeed, it is possible to interpret the increased expressions of racism in both the deterministic and conditional treatments simply as evidence of racial priming. Repeated measures designs can assess change in attitudes, and they have been shown to increase precision without altering treatment effects (Clifford et al., Reference Clifford, Sheagley and Piston2021). Therefore, we implement a two-wave study in which we measure racial attitudes before and after treatment to assess whether participants’ racial attitudes change as a function of the genetic narrative.
Second, we further increase the precision of our treatment effects by applying a more stringent control group comparison. We do this by (1) disentangling the racial and genetic explanation effects in our treatment conditions and (2) aligning the topic of the control text to be closer to the treatment texts (instead of a completely unrelated topic).
Third, we expand our examination of the effects of genetic explanations on racial attitudes by exploring a measure of overt racism. If, as we suspect, racialized genetic science contributes to heightened racial hostility, then we should also observe greater willingness to express explicitly racist views when given genetic attributions for racial differences. We further seek to understand potential changes in racist attitudes by considering the role of essentialist beliefs—the perception that differences between racial groups are differences in innate and immutable features. That is, we explore essentialist beliefs as a potential mechanism by which genetic narratives may be associated with heightened racism.
Finally, we examine a different outcome domain for which a genetic explanation is given—violence. In addition to increasing the generalizability of genetic narrative effects by assessing a behavioral outcome unrelated to health, the purpose of exploring violence in this study is to examine a behavioral domain in which there are strong racial stereotypes of Black criminality, but which has itself been associated with lower levels of punitiveness given genetic explanations (e.g., Cheung & Heine, Reference Cheung and Heine2015).
Drawing on the results from Study 1, we preregistered our hypotheses, which are largely parallel to our expectations in the initial study.Footnote 11 We expect broadly that exposure to genetic research that is explained in a deterministic way will lead to increased expressions of racism (H1). However, we expect that this effect will emerge specifically when the genetic explanation is racialized (i.e., the text reports differences in the prevalence of the genetic variant between White and Black racial groups; H1a). That is, although genetic attributions for violence might lead to increased sympathy for perpetrators of violence in the absence of a racialized context (Heine et al., Reference Heine, Dar-Nimrod, Cheung and Proulx2017), we anticipate that highlighting racial differences in genetic markers for violence will override such sympathy and instead lead to more hostile racial attitudes.
In contrast, we tentatively hypothesize that conditional genetic narratives—across racial and non-racial contexts—will prevent the heightened racism (H2). That is, we explore the possibility that providing contextualized explanations of genetic associations leads to no change in racial attitudes, effectively diminishing the attitudinal impact of genetic attributions for racial differences.
Methods
Participants and experimental design
Data were collected on a national sample matched to the U.S. Census (N ~ 2,500) via Dynata. Wave 1 of the data was collected near the end of August 2019.Footnote 12 These same respondents were then asked to complete Wave 2 of the study approximately two weeks later, September 9, 2019. In total, 2,504 people completed both waves of the survey. The survey comprised two separate experiments; we describe one experiment here, in which participants received one of five treatments (N = 1,276).Footnote 13 Participants were surveyed on our items of interest (i.e., expressions of overt racism) and other pre-treatment demographic questionsFootnote 14 in Wave 1 and then were reassessed on these items following the random assignment of the five conditions (four treatments; one control) in Wave 2.
Experimental conditions
The experimental treatments varied along two primary dimensions: (1) whether the explanation for the outcome is described as caused by a specific genetic variant (deterministic) or whether genetic effects are conditioned by other environmental conditions, genetic factors, and personal choice (conditional); and (2) whether racial categories are mentioned as part of the discussion of the incidence of a genetic variant associated with violent behavior (i.e., race versus no race). The control condition was on the subject of violence but without any reference to genetics or race (see Appendix B for full treatment materials). Post-treatment, subjects were again asked to report their racial attitudes.
Deterministic violence condition—Race
Participants assigned to the deterministic violence condition read a news article titled “‘Warrior gene’ may contribute to violent crime, studies say,” which described real genetics research that found a link between genes and violent behavior. The text emphasized the role of the gene, MAOA, in manifestations of “extremely violent behavior.” In addition, this text referenced the distribution of this gene by race: “Researchers found that more African Americans carry the problematic form of the gene than Caucasians. 8.4% of African Americans and 1.3% of Caucasians carry forms of the gene, which include millions of Americans.”
Deterministic violence condition—No race
Participants assigned to the deterministic violence condition read a news article titled “‘Warrior gene’ may contribute to violent crime, studies say,” which described genetics research that found a link between genes and violent behavior. The text emphasized the role of the gene, MAOA, in manifestations of “extremely violent behavior” but made no reference to the distribution of this gene by race.
Conditional violence condition—Race
Those assigned to the conditional violence condition read a similar news article titled “‘Warrior gene’ may interact with environment to contribute to violent crime, studies say,” which described similar genetics research linking MAOA to violent behavior, but this article emphasized the “complexity of the issue” given a large environmental component and quoted an expert who asserted that “these genes … most emphatically do not predetermine you for a life of crime.” In addition, this text referenced the distribution of this gene by race: “Researchers found that more African Americans carry the problematic form of the gene than Caucasians. 8.4% of African Americans and 1.3% of Caucasians carry forms of the gene, which include millions of Americans.”
Conditional violence condition—No race
Those assigned to the conditional violence condition read a similar news article titled “‘Warrior gene’ may interact with environment to contribute to violent crime, studies say,” which described similar genetics research linking MAOA to violent behavior, but this article emphasized the “complexity of the issue” given a large environmental component and quoted an expert who asserted that “these genes … most emphatically do not predetermine you for a life of crime.” Moreover, this treatment did not reference any distribution of this gene by race.
Control condition
Those assigned to the control condition read an article about violent crime as a function of environmental influences (i.e., trauma), but without reference to genetics or race. See Appendix B for full treatment materials.
Outcome measures
Overt racism
We employed a measure of overt racial prejudice adapted by Huddy and Feldman (Reference Huddy and Feldman2009) from long-standing questions in the General Social Survey. Of particular interest to us, this measure assesses the extent to which the respondent believes “fundamental genetic differences between the races” serve as explanations for economic disparities among Blacks compared to Whites, more closely assessing the mechanism through which deterministic genetic narratives can lead to overtly racist attitudes (i.e., “On average, African Americans have lower income and worse housing than white people. How much of the economic difference between blacks and whites occurs because of fundamental genetic differences between the races?” measured on a 5-point Likert scale from “none” to “a great deal”). We administered this measure (composed of six items in total) in Wave 1 (pre-treatment) and Wave 2 (post-treatment) and calculated the average response across the items for each subject in Wave 1 and Wave 2; the dependent variable in the analyses below is the within-subject mean difference in reported overt racism post-treatment (Wave 2) – pre-treatment (Wave 1).
Essentialism
We suspected that deterministic genetic narratives were also likely to increase essentialist beliefs that genes make up the core of who a person is. Therefore, we administered a scale of essentialist beliefs in Wave 2 (post-treatment) using the average response reported across the eight-item Bastian and Haslam (Reference Bastian and Haslam2006) essentialism scale (e.g., “There are different types of people and with enough scientific knowledge these different ‘types’ can be traced back to genetic causes”; on a scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree). The mean response across all items was approximately 4.27 (SD = 0.87).
Results
Treatment effects on overt racism
Table 2 reports the results of a linear regression model assessing the effects of within-subjects treatment on expressions of overt racism. In our models, we regressed our measure of change in overt racism on the experimental treatments in Model 1 of Table 2 and included our demographic covariates in Model 2.
† White is the reference category.
** p < .01;
* p < .05;
+ p < .1 (two-tailed).
The results indicate that expressions of overt racism increased within subjects following exposure to the deterministic genetic treatment (compared to the control treatment), but only in a racialized context that specifically indicated the prevalence of this gene in the African American population (p < .05, two-tailed, in Model 1 and Model 2). Figure 1 displays the marginal effects from Model 2 reported in Table 2. Moreover, in additional robustness checks (reported in Appendix B, Table 3), we collapse the treatments (race versus no race) into deterministic and conditional (versus control). We find that deterministic treatments in general significantly increase expressions of overt racism relative to the control condition (p < .05, two-tailed). In contrast, conditional treatments do not have a statistically significant effect on expressions of overt racism relative to the control condition (p > .10, two-tailed).
Note: Robust standard errors in parentheses.
† White is the reference category.
** p < .01;
* p < .05;
+ p < .1 (two-tailed).
Furthermore, the results suggest that providing contextualized explanations for genetic associations—even in a racialized context—mitigates the heightened racist response. If referencing race alone was the primary driver of increased expressions of racism, then we would expect people receiving the conditional narratives in a racialized context (Conditional—Race) to also be significantly more likely to report racist attitudes than those who receive the control news article. This is not the case (p > .10, two-tailed), suggesting that although there may be some compounding effect of genes and race in the racialized deterministic narratives, conditional language about the effects of genes can disconnect the link between genetic attributions and racism.
Next, we further probe the mechanisms linking deterministic treatment of genetic effects to expressions of racism. We do so by examining the relationship between deterministic treatment and essentialist attitudes. Table 3 reports the results of a linear regression in which our dependent variable is essentialist attitudes (measured post-treatment) using the average response in the eight-item Bastian and Haslam (Reference Bastian and Haslam2006) essentialism scale. Individuals who received the deterministic treatments with and without explicit reference to race expressed more essentialist attitudes than those who received the control condition (p < .01, two-tailed). In addition, individuals who received the conditional treatment specifying racial allelic differences were somewhat more likely to report essentialist attitudes (p < .10, two-tailed), but not when race is not mentioned (p > .10, two-tailed). Put another way, it appears that it is only when genetic effects are contextualized in the absence of racial cues that essentialist beliefs remain unchanged.
In Table 4, we model the potential correlation between expressions of overt racism and essentialist beliefs using a simultaneous regression model. By explicitly modeling this potential correlation, we should get more efficient estimators and are less likely to report insignificant associations (Lewis & Linzer, Reference Lewis and Linzer2005). The simultaneous regression model results in Table 4 show that, indeed, the error terms in the two models are correlated (p < .01, two-tailed, in Model 1 and Model 2), suggesting that there is a correlation between expressions of overt racism and essentialist beliefs. When taking this into account by statistically modeling the joint dependence, we see that there remain statistically significant effects of the deterministic race narrative on expressions of both overt racism and essentialist beliefs. However, there are no longer statistically significant effects of the conditional race treatment on essentialist attitudes (p > .10, two-tailed). Moreover, the conditional race treatment is not statistically significantly associated with expressions of overt racism (p > .10, two-tailed). Together, these results suggest that deterministic narratives may lead to an increase in the expression of overt racism through increased essentialist beliefs.
† White is the reference category
** p < .01;
* p < .05;
+ p < .1 (two-tailed).
Discussion
With the current research, we sought to understand how portrayals of genetics research on health and behavioral outcomes can impact racial attitudes. Critically, we investigated the contrasting effects of media narratives that emphasize a deterministic, causal narrative of the role of genes versus a narrative intervention that provides a more conditional and complex explanation of the role of genes in phenotypic outcomes. Specifically, we examined the effects on prejudice and policy attitudes from deterministic and conditional genetic narratives in the domains of obesity and violence in media articles that implicated racialized minorities.
Across two studies, the experimental results suggest that media portrayals (and perhaps even academic, scientific portrayals) of genetics research explaining important outcomes like health and violence can impact the public’s racial attitudes—and even their policy preferences in Study 1. Although simply reporting on genetics research on obesity (whether deterministic or conditional) in Study 1 appeared to negatively influence anti-Black bias, we also observe some evidence that narrative nuance matters. Hinshaw and Stier (Reference Hinshaw and Stier2008) suggest that genetic mechanisms tend to be prioritized over other explanations (such as the environment; see also Schmalor et al., Reference Schmalor, Cheung and Heine2021), but our results suggest this does not have to be the case. Rather, in Study 2, our results indicate that despite increased anti-Black attitudes following reports of racialized genetics research, the pernicious attitudinal effects of such reporting can also be mitigated with explanations of polygenic, environmental, and personal choice factors. Thus, our research joins a chorus of scientists and academics in cautioning against carelessness in reporting or tendencies to sensationalize such genetics research in the media.
Highlighting genetic contributions to health and behavior may render racially minoritized group members particularly more vulnerable to prejudice. Although our focus in this research was to examine the effects of genetic explanations on racial attitudes toward Blacks, it remains to be seen whether prejudiced and punitive attitudes would also emerge toward other social or racial groups, including racial majority group members, when described in genetically deterministic ways. Although some prior research suggests that genetic attributions are associated with increased tolerance for disadvantaged and vulnerable groups (e.g., homosexuals, drug addicts, obese people; Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Smith and Hibbing2018), it may be that such tolerant attitudes are mostly reserved for groups that are explicitly or implicitly perceived as prototypically White (even if they are low status in other ways). Schneider et al. (Reference Schneider, Smith and Hibbing2018) also find that genetic attributions for individual characteristics tend not to be associated with negative racial attitudes, but there may be a distinction to be made between genetic attributions for individuals versus groups. For instance, Suhay and Jayaratne (Reference Suhay and Jayaratne2013) find that genetic attributions for racial group (as opposed to individual) differences in characteristics like intelligence and violence are associated with ideological beliefs that served to reinforce existing racial inequalities. Another open question for future research is how genetic attributions for positive outcomes or stereotypes may affect racial attitudes, in part because research on positive stereotypes largely suggests that such favorable group stereotypes are societally pervasive and influential in perpetuating systemic inequality (Czopp et al., Reference Czopp, Kay and Cheryan2015). We believe our studies take one step toward disentangling the intersectional effects of race and genetic attribution on attitudes, but there is much to explore in future work.
Together, our studies explore both modern and old-fashioned forms of anti-Black racism as a function of racialized genetic attributions, and our results suggest that genetic explanations have the potential to impact different types of racial attitude expressions. But, of course, this is merely a starting point for future investigation into the conditions under which such connections are made, as well as what kinds of ideological consequences may follow. For example, Banks and Valentino (Reference Banks and Valentino2012) show that old-fashioned racism is associated with feelings of disgust, whereas modern racism is associated with feelings of anger or resentment. Investigation of such discrete emotions may provide clues to how genetic attributions can be associated with general negative racial out-group attitudes versus identification with explicitly racialized ideology, like the alt-right’s White nationalism.
It will also be important in future work to further examine the political and policy implications of genetic attributions in intergroup contexts. Here, consistent with prior research that suggests genetic framing of ethnic out-group members impacts support for conflict policies (Kimel et al., Reference Kimel, Huesmann, Kunst and Halperin2016), we show some preliminary evidence that the way genetic explanations are framed can impact pertinent policy attitudes. However, much remains to be explored, as we believe, like Huddy and Feldman (Reference Huddy and Feldman2009), that “the political power of racial prejudice remains an important issue” (p. 441). We hope to engage in further research that not only probes the effects of genetic attributions in different outcome domains on broader policy attitudes, but also on perceptions of and adherence to racialized ideology itself. If, for example, learning about genetic science associated with racial categories is an initial step toward endorsement of White nationalist ideologies, then identifying how such genetic justifications contribute to the formation of such ideological beliefs will be particularly important to understand.
There are also implications of this research for both education and media communication. Not only has the frequency of genetic science reporting in the news media increased significantly since the start of the Human Genome Project—including reports focused on highlighting racialized genetic differences (Phelan et al., Reference Phelan, Link and Feldman2013)—but genetic science forms the basis for the reemergence of discussions of race in high school biology textbooks in the 1990s (Morning, Reference Morning2008). Whereas there were no discussions of race in the context of health disorders in textbooks from 1952 to 1962, discussions of racial differences in biology appear in 93% of contemporary textbooks (from 1993 to 2002; Morning, Reference Morning2008). Indeed, disputes about “racial superiority” based on biological justifications are occurring in contemporary high school classrooms where individuals typically have their first contact with scientific education about genetics. In this way, supposedly neutral scientific information may be forming the kernel for racialized ideology in adolescents, without clear scientific guidance on whether such genetic justifications can be debunked (Harmon, Reference Harmon2018a). Moreover, increasingly accessible and popular direct-to-consumer genetic ancestry tests have also been shown to reinforce essentialist views on racial differences in the general population (Phelan et al., Reference Phelan, Link, Zelner and Yang2014), as well among White nationalists (Panofsky & Donovan, Reference Panofsky and Donovan2019). For these reasons, we suspect that the wide availability of information on genetic science—in the absence of careful interpretation—has the potential to strengthen and mobilize negative racial attitudes in the broader public.
Finally, and relatedly, an important future direction for this work will be to probe what type of language is most effective in mitigating the negative effects of linking genetics to health or behavior. Schneider et al. (Reference Schneider, Smith and Hibbing2018) break down the types of attributions that people tend to make for human characteristics as genetic, environmental, and personal choice. We would like to explore in subsequent studies how different types of conditional language in genetic narratives might contribute to prejudice reduction—for instance, by highlighting polygenic, environmental, and personal choice factors separately. Indeed, the relationship between genetic attributions, behavioral outcomes, and social group characteristics make for complex perceptions of and attitudes toward those social groups. It will be important as this research moves forward to examine the ways in which genetic attributions can contribute to continued and exacerbated social inequalities—as well as the ways in which illuminating the role of genes in human behavior may lead to greater tolerance, respect, and societal equality.
Data availability statement
This article earned the Open Data, Open Materials, and Preregistered open science badges. The data, replication code and materials, and preregistration for this article are available at https://osf.io/59kqg/.
Author contributions
H. Hannah Nam and Katherine Sawyer contributed equally to this article.
Appendix A: Study 1 Survey Items
Symbolic Racism Scale (Henry and Sears Reference Henry and Sears 2002 ). We recoded the following items in accordance with the recommendations by Henry and Sears (Reference Henry and Sears2002): items 1, 2, 4, and 8 were recoded so that a 1 = 4, 2 = 3, 3 = 2, and 4 = 1. Item 3 was recoded so that 1 = 3, 2 = 1, and 3 = 2.
Please read the statements below and chose the response most similar to your own:
______ 1. It’s really a matter of some people not trying hard enough; if blacks would only try harder they could be just as well off as whites.
-
1 Strongly agree
-
2 Somewhat agree
-
3 Somewhat disagree
-
4 Strongly disagree
______ 2. Irish, Italian, Jewish and many other minorities overcame prejudice and worked their way up. Blacks should do the same.
-
1 Strongly agree
-
2 Somewhat agree
-
3 Somewhat disagree
-
4 Strongly disagree
______ 3. Some say that black leaders have been trying to push too fast. Others feel that they haven’t pushed fast enough. What do you think?
-
1 Trying to push very much too fast
-
2 Going too slowly
-
3 Moving at about the right speed
______ 4. How much of the racial tension that exists in the United States today do you think blacks are responsible for creating?
-
1 All of it
-
2 Most
-
3 Some
-
4 Not much at all
______ 5. How much discrimination against blacks do you feel there is in the United States today, limiting their chances to get ahead?
-
1 A lot
-
2 Some
-
3 Just a little
-
4 None at all
______ 6. Generations of slavery and discrimination have created conditions that make it difficult for blacks to work their way out of the lower class.
-
1 Strongly agree
-
2 Somewhat agree
-
3 Somewhat disagree
-
4 Strongly disagree
______ 7. Over the past few years, blacks have gotten less than they deserve.
-
1 Strongly agree
-
2 Somewhat agree
-
3 Somewhat disagree
-
4 Strongly disagree
______ 8. Over the past few years, blacks have gotten more economically than they deserve.
-
1 Strongly agree
-
2 Somewhat agree
-
3 Somewhat disagree
-
4 Strongly disagree
Support of universal health care
Some people say that health care should be a right for all people and not a privilege only for those who are insured by their workplace or participate in some other private plan. Others say that the tax burden in this country is already high and it is unreasonable to expect people who are paying a part of their own private insurance plan to also pay for other people. How do you feel about universal, guaranteed health care?
-
7 Strongly support universal, guaranteed health care
-
6 Somewhat support universal, guaranteed health care
-
5 Slightly support universal, guaranteed health care
-
4 Neither support or oppose universal, guaranteed health care
-
3 Slightly oppose universal, guaranteed health care
-
2 Somewhat oppose universal, guaranteed health care
-
1 Strongly oppose universal, guaranteed health care
Health treatment—Deterministic
Health treatment—Conditional
Control condition
Research debriefing form
Thank you very much for participating in our study!
In this study, we are interested in examining whether media portrayals of genetics research has any impact on people’s social and political attitudes. You may have read an excerpt of a article that described the genetic basis for some behavior or health outcome, or you may have read an article unrelated to genetics at all. The article excerpt presented in the study was an adaptation of real articles in the media but were edited to emphasize various aspects of the role of genetics and the environment. We expect that when genetic explanations for behavior are provided that may lead to differences in people’s attitudes toward the described groups. We apologize for not explaining the study’s primary intent at the outset; if participants had been alerted to the purpose, responding could have been biased by this knowledge. Your participation was helpful in increasing our understanding of how media portrayals of genetics research may affect social attitudes.
Thank you again for your valuable participation!
Appendix A. Study 1: Empirical analyses
Appendix B: Study 2 Survey Items
Overt Racism (Feldman and Huddy Reference Feldman and Huddy2010 ). Reverse-code items 1, 2.
On average, African Americans have lower income and worse housing than white people. How much of the economic difference between blacks and whites:
Occurs because most Blacks do not have the chance to get a good education?
-
○ None (1)
-
○ A little (2)
-
○ Don’t know (3)
-
○ Some (4)
-
○ A great deal (5)
Can be explained by discrimination against blacks?
-
○ None (1)
-
○ A little (2)
-
○ Don’t know (3)
-
○ Some (4)
-
○ A great deal (5)
Occurs because most blacks just don’t have the motivation or will power to perform well?
-
○ None (1)
-
○ A little (2)
-
○ Don’t know (3)
-
○ Some (4)
-
○ A great deal (5)
Occurs because most blacks do not teach their children the values and skills which are required to be successful in school?
-
○ None (1)
-
○ A little (2)
-
○ Don’t know (3)
-
○ Some (4)
-
○ A great deal (5)
Is due to racial differences in intelligence?
-
○ None (1)
-
○ A little (2)
-
○ Don’t know (3)
-
○ Some (4)
-
○ A great deal (5)
Occurs because of fundamental genetic differences between the races?
-
○ None (1)
-
○ A little (2)
-
○ Don’t know (3)
-
○ Some (4)
-
○ A great deal (5)
Essentialism Scale (Bastian and Haslam Reference Bastian and Haslam2006). Reverse-code items 2,3,6,8.
Please indicate the degree to which you agree with the following statements from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree):
The kind of person someone is can be largely attributed to their genetic inheritance.
-
○ Strongly disagree (1)
-
○ Disagree (2)
-
○ Somewhat disagree (3)
-
○ Neither agree nor disagree (4)
-
○ Somewhat agree (5)
-
○ Agree (6)
-
○ Strongly agree (7)
Very few traits that people exhibit can be traced back to their biology.
-
○ Strongly disagree (1)
-
○ Disagree (2)
-
○ Somewhat disagree (3)
-
○ Neither agree nor disagree (4)
-
○ Somewhat agree (5)
-
○ Agree (6)
-
○ Strongly agree (7)
I think that genetic predispositions have little influence on the kind of person someone is.
-
○ Strongly disagree (1)
-
○ Disagree (2)
-
○ Somewhat disagree (3)
-
○ Neither agree nor disagree (4)
-
○ Somewhat agree (5)
-
○ Agree (6)
-
○ Strongly agree (7)
Whether someone is one kind of person or another is determined by their biological makeup.
-
○ Strongly disagree (1)
-
○ Disagree (2)
-
○ Somewhat disagree (3)
-
○ Neither agree nor disagree (4)
-
○ Somewhat agree (5)
-
○ Agree (6)
-
○ Strongly agree (7)
There are different types of people and with enough scientific knowledge these different ‘types’ can be traced back to genetic causes.
-
○ Strongly disagree (1)
-
○ Disagree (2)
-
○ Somewhat disagree (3)
-
○ Neither agree nor disagree (4)
-
○ Somewhat agree (5)
-
○ Agree (6)
-
○ Strongly agree (7)
A person’s attributes are something that can’t be attributed to their biology.
-
○ Strongly disagree (1)
-
○ Disagree (2)
-
○ Somewhat disagree (3)
-
○ Neither agree nor disagree (4)
-
○ Somewhat agree (5)
-
○ Agree (6)
-
○ Strongly agree (7)
With enough scientific knowledge, the basic qualities that a person has could be traced back to, and explained by, their biological makeup.
-
○ Strongly disagree (1)
-
○ Disagree (2)
-
○ Somewhat disagree (3)
-
○ Neither agree nor disagree (4)
-
○ Somewhat agree (5)
-
○ Agree (6)
-
○ Strongly agree (7)
A person’s traits are never determined by their genes.
-
○ Strongly disagree (1)
-
○ Disagree (2)
-
○ Somewhat disagree (3)
-
○ Neither agree nor disagree (4)
-
○ Somewhat agree (5)
-
○ Agree (6)
-
○ Strongly agree (7)
Violence treatment—Deterministic, race
“Warrior gene” may contribute to violent crime, studies say
By Jane Roberts
July 19, 2018
Whether criminals are born with an innate tendency to hurt others, are prone to mental disorders, or are molded by factors such as childhood trauma, a history of abuse or too many violent video games is a persistent and complicated question.
Now, new research suggests that genetics may in fact contribute to a propensity for violent criminal behavior.
In a study published Tuesday in the journal Molecular Psychiatry, researchers examined the genes of 895 people found guilty of crimes ranging from non-violent offenses such as drug or property crimes to severely violent offenses such as homicide and battery. They found that a variant of the gene—called MAOA and dubbed the “warrior gene”—was linked to “extremely violent behavior,” defined as having committed at least 10 homicides, attempted homicides or batteries. The MAOA gene plays a role in the metabolism of the neurotransmitter of dopamine that helps regulate emotions and reactions to pleasure and rewards.
A strong relationship between behavior and the “warrior gene” was not present among non-violent offenders. Even when the researchers accounted for factors such as personality disorders, childhood maltreatment or substance abuse, the effects were still specific to violent offenders.
The relationship between genetics and violent behavior was strongest for the 78 people in the study who were classified as “extremely violent offenders.” The people in this group committed a total of 1154 murders, manslaughters, attempted homicides and batteries.
Other researchers have also reported that this variant of the MAOA gene was less common among Caucasians (34 percent) and more common among African Americans (59 percent). Such studies further suggest that the prevalence of the “warrior gene” is quite variable across the population.
Violence treatment—Conditional, race
“Warrior gene” may interact with environment to contribute to violent crime, studies say
By Jane Roberts
July 19, 2018
Whether criminals are born with an innate tendency to hurt others, are prone to mental disorders, or are molded by factors such as childhood trauma, a history of abuse or too many violent video games is a persistent and complicated question.
Now, new research suggests that genetics, in combination with environmental factors, may in fact contribute to a propensity for violent criminal behavior.
In a study published Tuesday in the journal Molecular Psychiatry, researchers examined the genes of 895 people found guilty of crimes ranging from non-violent offenses such as drug or property crimes to severely violent offenses such as homicide and battery. They found that a variant of the gene—called MAOA and dubbed the “warrior gene”—was linked to “extremely violent behavior,” defined as having committed at least 10 homicides, attempted homicides or batteries. The MAOA gene plays a role in the metabolism of the neurotransmitter of dopamine that helps regulate emotions and reactions to pleasure and rewards.
Revealing the true complexity of the issue, in another study, published in 2012 in the journal Science, investigators found a relationship between convictions of violent crimes and a combination of low-activity MAOA plus childhood maltreatment. That is, the low-activity MAOA gene was only associated with violent crime convictions when the individuals had also experienced a history of adversity and maltreatment.
Other researchers have also reported that this variant of the MAOA gene was less common among Caucasians (34 percent) and more common among African Americans (59 percent). Such studies further suggest that the prevalence of the “warrior gene” is quite variable across the population.
In an interview with the BBC, Jan Schnupp, a neuroscientist at the University of Oxford called for caution in interpreting such studies, saying that up to half the population—most of whom do not commit violent crimes—could have one of the genes that the studies linked to violent behavior.
“To call these alleles ‘genes for violence’ would therefore be a massive exaggeration,” Schnupp said. “In combination with many other factors these genes may make it a little harder for you to control violent urges, but they most emphatically do not predetermine you for a life of crime.”
Violence treatment—Deterministic, no race
“Warrior gene” may contribute to violent crime, studies say
By Jane Roberts
July 19, 2018
Whether criminals are born with an innate tendency to hurt others, are prone to mental disorders, or are molded by factors such as childhood trauma, a history of abuse or too many violent video games is a persistent and complicated question.
Now, new research suggests that genetics may in fact contribute to a propensity for violent criminal behavior.
In a study published Tuesday in the journal Molecular Psychiatry, researchers examined the genes of 895 people found guilty of crimes ranging from non-violent offenses such as drug or property crimes to severely violent offenses such as homicide and battery. They found that a variant of the gene—called MAOA and dubbed the “warrior gene”—was linked to “extremely violent behavior,” defined as having committed at least 10 homicides, attempted homicides or batteries. The MAOA gene plays a role in the metabolism of the neurotransmitter of dopamine that helps regulate emotions and reactions to pleasure and rewards.
A strong relationship between behavior and the gene was not present among non-violent offenders. Even when the researchers accounted for factors such as personality disorders, childhood maltreatment or substance abuse, the effects were still specific to violent offenders.
The relationship between genetics and violent behavior was strongest for the 78 people in the study who were classified as “extremely violent offenders.” The people in this group committed a total of 1154 murders, manslaughters, attempted homicides and batteries.
Violence treatment—Conditional, no race
“Warrior gene” may interact with environment to contribute to violent crime, studies say
By Jane Roberts
July 19, 2018
Whether criminals are born with an innate tendency to hurt others, are prone to mental disorders, or are molded by factors such as childhood trauma, a history of abuse or too many violent video games is a persistent and complicated question.
Now, new research suggests that genetics, in combination with environmental factors, may in fact contribute to a propensity for violent criminal behavior.
In a study published Tuesday in the journal Molecular Psychiatry, researchers examined the genes of 895 people found guilty of crimes ranging from non-violent offenses such as drug or property crimes to severely violent offenses such as homicide and battery. They found that a variant of the gene—called MAOA and dubbed the “warrior gene”—was linked to “extremely violent behavior,” defined as having committed at least 10 homicides, attempted homicides or batteries. The MAOA gene plays a role in the metabolism of the neurotransmitter of dopamine that helps regulate emotions and reactions to pleasure and rewards.
Revealing the true complexity of the issue, in another study, published in 2012 in the journal Science, investigators found a relationship between convictions of violent crimes and a combination of low-activity MAOA plus childhood maltreatment. That is, the low-activity MAOA gene was only associated with violent crime convictions when the individuals had also experienced a history of adversity and maltreatment.
In an interview with the BBC, Jan Schnupp, a neuroscientist at the University of Oxford called for caution in interpreting such studies, saying that up to half the population—most of whom do not commit violent crimes—could have one of the genes that the studies linked to violent behavior.
“To call these alleles ‘genes for violence’ would therefore be a massive exaggeration,” Schnupp said. “In combination with many other factors these genes may make it a little harder for you to control violent urges, but they most emphatically do not predetermine you for a life of crime.”
Violence control
Traumatic events may contribute to violent crime, studies say
By Jane Roberts
July 19, 2018
Whether criminals are born with an innate tendency to hurt others, are prone to mental disorders, or are molded by factors such as childhood trauma, a history of abuse or too many violent video games is a persistent and complicated question.
Now, new research suggests that experiencing traumatic events may in fact contribute to a propensity for violent criminal behavior.
In a study published Tuesday in the journal Sociological Inquiry, researchers examined 895 people found guilty of crimes ranging from non-violent offenses such as drug or property crimes to severely violent offenses such as homicide and battery. They found that prior life experience with trauma—described as “an event or set of circumstances that is experienced by an individual as physically or emotionally harmful or life threatening and that has lasting adverse effects on the individual’s functioning and well-being”—was linked to “extremely violent behavior,” defined as having committed at least 10 homicides, attempted homicides or batteries.
Trauma is a common experience for individuals in American communities and has no boundaries with regard to age, gender, socioeconomic status, race, ethnicity, or sexual orientation. Research has shown that traumatic experiences are associated with both behavioral and physical conditions, especially those traumatic events that occur during childhood. Because these events can have a negative impact not only on the affected individual but also harm to the larger community, researchers note it is of great importance to understand the nature and impact of trauma, as well as avenues for healing trauma.
Research debriefing form
Thank you very much for participating in our study!
In this study, we are interested in examining whether media portrayals of genetics research has any impact on people’s social and political attitudes. You may have read an excerpt of a article that described the genetic basis for some behavior or health outcome, or you may have read an article unrelated to genetics at all. The article excerpt presented in the study was an adaptation of real articles in the media but were edited to emphasize various aspects of the role of genetics and the environment. We expect that when genetic explanations for behavior are provided that may lead to differences in people’s attitudes toward the described groups. We apologize for not explaining the study’s primary intent at the outset; if participants had been alerted to the purpose, responding could have been biased by this knowledge. Your participation was helpful in increasing our understanding of how media portrayals of genetics research may affect social attitudes.
Thank you again for your valuable participation!
Appendix B: Study 2 Empirical Analyses
Note: Robust standard errors in parentheses.