Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T11:11:34.408Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

General Practitioners’ coronary risk estimates, decisions to start lipid-lowering treatment, gender and length of clinical experience: their interactions in primary prevention

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 January 2013

Federico Vancheri*
Affiliation:
Internal Medicine, Ospedale S.Elia, Caltanissetta, Sicily, Italy Center for Family and Community Medicine, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Lars-Erik Strender
Affiliation:
Center for Family and Community Medicine, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Lars G. Backlund
Affiliation:
Center for Family and Community Medicine, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
*
Correspondence to: Federico Vancheri, Internal Medicine, Ospedale S.Elia, viale Luigi Monaco, 93100 Caltanissetta, Sicily, Italy. Email: federico.vancheri@ki.se
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aim

We investigated whether the risk estimates of General Practitioners (GPs) and their treatment decisions mutually influence each other and whether factors not related to the patient's risk, such as the gender and length in clinical practice, interact.

Background

The quantitative assessment of the absolute risk of developing coronary heart disease (CHD) and the decision to start treatment with lipid-lowering drugs are crucial tasks in the primary prevention of CHD.

Methods

Nine clinical vignettes, four rated high-risk and five rated low-risk according to the Framingham equation, were mailed to three groups of 90 randomly selected GPs in Stockholm. One group (R) was asked to estimate the risk of CHD within 10 years on a visual analogue scale. A second group (R + D) was asked to estimate the risk and to specify whether they would recommend a pharmacological lipid-lowering treatment. A third group (D) only to indicate whether they would recommend treatment.

Results

Response rate ranged from 42.2% to 45.6%. The median risk estimates were higher in the R group than in the R + D group (difference not statistically significant). R + D group showed higher proportions of correct decisions to start treatment compared with the R group (86.2% versus 77.5%, P = 0.19). More correct decisions were made by female doctors (OR 1.77, 95% CI 1.19–2.61, P = 0.004) and by less experienced doctors (OR 0.97, 95% CI 0.95–0.99, P = 0.016).

Conclusions

The task of making CHD risk estimates and the task of making decisions whether to start lipid-lowering treatment do not seem to influence each other. The gender of physicians and the length of clinical experience seem to affect treatment decisions. Female GPs and less experienced GPs are more likely to make correct decisions. However, the relatively low response rate to the questionnaires may limit the generalizability of these results.

Type
Research
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Introduction

The primary prevention of coronary heart disease (CHD) is based on the assessment of the individual's absolute risk of developing CHD rather than on the value of any specific risk factor, and preventive treatment should be considered if the patient's absolute risk exceeds a certain cut-off point (Expert Panel, 2001). Therefore, the key factor in proper CHD prevention is combining quantitative risk assessments with decisions about whether or not to treat individual patients. Although there is extensive knowledge about how to manage cardiovascular risk, the quality of preventive care is suboptimal, especially in high-risk subjects (Durrington et al., Reference Durrington, Prais, Bhatnagar, France, Crowley, Khan and Morgan1999; Grundy et al., Reference Grundy, Pasternak, Greenland, Smith and Fuster1999; Ford et al., Reference Ford, Mokdad, Giles and Mensah2003; Erhardt and Hobbs, Reference Erhardt and Hobbs2007; Doroodchi et al., Reference Doroodchi, Abdolrasulnia, Foster, Foster, Turakhia, Skelding, Sagar and Casebeer2008).

Risk assessment tools, such as charts or computer programs, have been developed and are recommended in the identification of high-risk subjects, but their use in clinical practice is limited, and clinicians are more likely to make their own assessment subjectively (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Stewart and Hertog2006; Eichler et al., Reference Eichler, Zoller, Tschudi and Steurer2007).

We have previously observed that the decision to start pharmacological treatment with lipid-lowering drugs does not come in a straightforward way from the doctor's estimate of the patient's risk (Vancheri et al., Reference Vancheri, Strender, Bring, Montgomery, Skaner and Backlund2008). When the relationship between physicians’ subjective risk estimates and decisions to treat with respect to a defined cut-off level was investigated using clinical vignettes, we found that in simulated cases with high actual risk level, there was a high rate of decision to treat even when the physicians’ own quantitative estimate was below the risk rate defining the cut-off level to start pharmacological treatment. This observation may indicate that in high-risk cases the decision to start pharmacological treatment is to some degree independent from the physicians’ own quantitative risk estimate. Therefore, risk estimates and treatment decisions may be partially independent. Other studies have documented a discordance between knowledge and action in medical decision making (Redelmeier and Shafir, Reference Redelmeier and Shafir1995; Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Kushniruk, Yale and Patel1999). Within studies of physician's risk estimates and treatment decisions, it is not known whether the task of making a decision about treatment influences the quantitative risk estimate and vice versa.

Moreover, although the influences of the gender of physicians and their clinical experience on management of patients at risk for cardiovascular events have been investigated, there is limited information about their role in the area of risk estimates and treatment decisions in primary CHD prevention (Choudhry et al., Reference Choudhry, Fletcher and Soumerai2005; Christian et al., Reference Christian, Mills, Simpson and Mosca2006; Baguet et al., Reference Baguet, Fiquet, Yau and Mounier-Vehier2007; Berthold et al., Reference Berthold, Gouni-Berthold, Bestehorn, Böhm and Krone2008; Baumhäkel et al., Reference Baumhäkel, Müller and Böhm2009; Tabenkin et al., Reference Tabenkin, Eaton, Roberts, Parker, McMurray and Borkan2010; Southern et al., Reference Southern, Bellin and Arnsten2011).

In the present study, we aimed to assess:

  1. 1) whether the risk estimates of General Practitioners (GPs) and their treatment decisions mutually influence each other, that is, whether decisions influence ratings and whether ratings influence decisions;

  2. 2) whether the gender of physicians and the number of years they have been in clinical practice influences risk estimates and treatment decisions.

The answers to the first question are of theoretical interest within the field of decision making in general and should be of importance in the interpretation of previous and future studies in the field of risk estimates and treatment decisions. The second question relates to individual differences in clinical decision making, especially the role of physicians’ gender and the length of clinical experience, and may help explain variations in quality of care.

We investigated three groups of GPs confronted with the same series of simulated clinical case descriptions. Each group had one of the following tasks: risk rating only (R group), risk rating and decisions about pharmacological treatment (R + D group) and treatment decision only (D group). To answer the question about whether decisions influence ratings, risk assessments made by R and R + D groups were compared. To investigate whether ratings influence decisions, we compared decisions made by the R + D and D groups. All comparisons were analyzed in relation to gender and length of clinical experience.

Methods

Setting

This study was conducted in Stockholm, Sweden. The data were collected in 2006.

Sample

A random sample of 270 GPs was drawn from the local database of healthcare professionals, which comprised 828 GPs. Only Family Medicine specialists were included in the study.

Design

The study design was a cross-sectional survey. A questionnaire describing nine clinical cases was mailed to three groups of 90 randomly selected GPs in Stockholm. All physicians received the same set of nine cases in the same order.

One group of GPs (R) was asked to estimate the risk of CHD within 10 years on a visual analogue scale (VAS), between 0% and 100%, without using a risk table or any other decision support. The risk categories currently indicated in the Framingham-based tables (low <5%, mild 5–10%, moderate 10–20%, high 20–40% and very high risk >40%) were provided as anchorage points within the scale. We chose the older Framingham risk equation because it is the most widely used method for assessment of cardiovascular risk and is the basis for most other risk prediction methods (Cooney et al., Reference Cooney, Dudina, Whincup, Capewell, Menotti, Jousilahti, Njolstadi, Oganov, Thomsen, Tverdral, Wedel, Wilhelmsen and Graham2009). The cardiovascular risk assessed using Framingham was compared with the SCORE algorithm, recently introduced in Europe (De Backer et al., Reference De Backer, Ambrosioni, Borch-Johnsen, Brotons, Cifkova and Dallongeville2003), producing the same results regarding the relation to the respective cut-off values and almost identical ranking of the cases in terms of risk.

A second group of GPs (R + D) was asked to estimate the risk of CHD within 10 years on a VAS and to specify whether or not they would recommend a pharmacological lipid-lowering treatment for the patient, assuming that lifestyle interventions had been tried for at least six months. Figure 1 provides an example of a case as presented to the R + D group.

Figure 1 Example of a case description

A third group (D) was asked only to indicate whether they would recommend a pharmacological lipid-lowering treatment for the patient.

The questionnaires asked for the physicians’ age, gender and length of experience, but remained anonymous to increase the likelihood that answers would be given without the use of risk tables or other decision supports (as the instruction to the doctors prescribed). Because the number of years GPs have been in clinical practice is closely related to their age, we included only the length of experience in the analyses.

Clinical cases

We presented each physician with nine patient cases that incorporated a combination of the variables from the Framingham risk tables: age, sex, systolic blood pressure, cholesterol level and smoking. The patient cases were constructed by two of the authors (L.B., L.-E.S.) based on their own clinical experience. The patients had no history of cardiovascular disease or diabetes, as risk assessment for deciding about initiation of lipid-lowering treatment is not relevant for patients with such conditions; in addition, none had systolic blood pressure levels of above 160 mmHg, as higher values might have led the doctors to consider the treatment of hypertension more relevant than the treatment of hypercholesterolemia. The set of cases was constructed to represent a spectrum of patients with a 10-year risk of a fatal or non-fatal coronary event ranging from high to low, based on the Framingham algorithm (Wood et al., Reference Wood, De Backer, Faergeman, Graham, Mancia and Pyörälä1998). According to this equation, a 10-year absolute CHD risk of 20% or more is the threshold for pharmacological lipid-lowering treatment. Therefore, 20% was used as the cut-off point for defining high- and low-risk cases in the Results section. The calculated Framingham median score was 17.0 (range 3–45) for all cases combined, 30.5 (range 27–45) for the four high-risk cases and 15.0 (range 3–17) for the five low-risk cases.

To minimize the risk of an anchorage effect, we opened the questionnaire with a medium-risk case and ordered the rest at random.

A summary of the nine cases presented to the doctors is shown in Figure 2, below the box plots.

Figure 2 Box plot of doctors’ risk estimates in the R group (empty bars) and R + D group (filled bars) and summary of the nine cases along with the calculated Framingham risk level. Framingham score is GPs’ risk estimates minus Framingham risk levels. The bottom of the boxes is at the first quartile, the top is at the third quartile and the continuous lines across the boxes are at the median value. The whiskers are drawn to the highest and lowest values that are not considered as outliers. Outliers, marked with dots, are estimates outside these limits. The first five cases are low-risk cases, according to Framingham. The others are high-risk cases, eligible for treatment.

The study was approved by the regional ethics committee in Stockholm (no. 2005/603–31).

Statistical analysis

To account for the clustering effect of each doctor being represented nine times, we used generalized linear models (linear and logistic regression) with robust standard errors for all of the analyses, with nine rows for each doctor for each case. We used multivariable models to test for the effect of Framingham score, experience and gender.

Investigation of risk estimate (R + D and R groups)

For the risk estimates, we used the difference between the doctors’ estimates and the calculated Framingham risk (Framingham score) because it is approximately normally distributed. A multivariable linear regression model was constructed that included (as independent variables) the actual Framingham risk (in order to ascertain how this affected the score), the group (R and R + D), the gender of the doctor and the number of years of experience.

Investigation of treatment decisions (R + D and D groups)

The effect of making a risk estimate on treatment decisions was first assessed by comparing the proportion of decisions made in the R + D and D groups, and the P-value for difference in the proportion of decisions was assessed using a logistic regression model that included both groups with decision as the outcome.

The effect of group experience, Framingham score, gender and experience on the proportion of correct decisions was investigated using multivariable logistic regression with correct decisions as the outcome. ‘No’ decisions were considered correct if the Framingham risk for the case was <20%, and ‘yes’ decisions were considered correct if the risk was ⩾20%.

We tested for interactions between the covariates in both models.

STATA (version 9.2) was used for statistical analysis.

Results

General data

Response rates to the questionnaire were as follows: 41 GPs (45.6%) from the R group (median age 55 years, range 38–69), 38 (42.2%) from the R + D group (median age 54 years, range 43–65) and 41 (45.6%) from the D group (median age 51 years, range 37–63). The percentage of male respondents in each group was 51.2, 41.7 and 50.0, respectively. The median length of clinical experience was similar in the three groups (15 years in the first, range 2–31; 15 years in the second, range 2–30 and 13.5 years in the third, range 1–30).

Ratings

Risk estimates within the R + D group and the R group for each case are shown as a box plot in Figure 2 as differences between the Framingham scores and the GPs’ risk estimates. There was a wide range of estimates, particularly for the high-risk cases. In general, the median estimates in the R group were higher than in the R + D group, especially for the high-risk cases, but the difference between the two groups was statistically significant in only one of the cases.

The median estimates of both groups were lower than the calculated Framingham risks for all nine cases, with the greatest discrepancies in the high-risk cases.

The difference between the doctors’ risk estimates and the calculated Framingham risk (doctors’ risk estimates minus Framingham risk) was not related to group (P = 0.27), gender (P = 0.74) or length of experience (P = 0.57). However, it was significantly related to the calculated Framingham risk (P = 0.04), with the differences getting larger as the Framingham risk increases.

Decisions

To investigate the effect of risk estimates on the task of making a decision, the proportion of decisions to start pharmacological treatment was calculated as the number of ‘yes’ decisions divided by the total number of decisions for each doctor. Overall, about half (48.3%) of the GPs’ decisions in the R + D group were favourable to start a treatment, compared with 44.4% in the D group (P = 0.62). For the five low-risk cases, the female GPs were significantly less willing to treat compared with the male GPs (12.6% versus 24.4%, P = 0.04; Figure 3).

Figure 3 Proportion of ‘yes’ decisions (R + D and D combined groups) by GPs’ gender for each case according to the level of calculated Framingham risk.

The proportions of correct decisions, based on the number of doctors to account for clustering, were higher in the R + D group for high-risk cases (86.2% and 77.5%, respectively), but this difference was not statistically significant (P = 0.19).

Correct decisions decreased with calculated Framingham score of the case, but this decrease was not significant (P = 0.12).

The effect of gender and length of clinical experience on correct decisions was investigated by including both variables as independent variables in the logistic regression model together with Framingham score. Correct decisions were significantly related to gender (being female; OR 1.77, 95% CI 1.19–2.61, P = 0.004) and negatively related to years of clinical experience (OR 0.97, 95% CI 0.95–0.99, P = 0.016). This indicates that correct decisions were more likely to be carried out by less experienced doctors. Figure 4 shows the predicted proportions of correct decisions as a function of clinical experience and gender. Female GPs made a higher rate of correct decisions (87.3% versus 75.5%, P = 0.08).

Figure 4 Plot of probability of correct decisions against the length of clinical experience by gender. The squares (empty = female doctors and filled = male doctors) represent the predicted proportions of correct decisions. Each square represents one to six doctors with the same number of years of clinical practice.

Discussion

Our results suggest that the task of risk rating and the task of deciding whether or not to start a lipid-lowering treatment do not influence each other.

All groups of GPs tended to underestimate risk compared with the calculated Framingham risk, supporting previous observations comparing the estimates of GPs and medical students in similar cases (Backlund et al., Reference Backlund, Bring and Strender2004) and comparing GPs in two European areas with different cardiovascular risk levels (Vancheri et al., Reference Vancheri, Strender, Bring, Montgomery, Skaner and Backlund2008).

There was wide variability in the risk assessments within each group of GPs. This finding is consistent with the results of other studies that have assessed clinicians’ perceptions of cardiovascular risk and the accuracy of their subjective estimates (Dolan et al., Reference Dolan, Bordley and Mushlin1986; Friedmann et al., Reference Friedmann, Brett and Mayo-Smith1996).

Among all the groups of GPs, the largest discordance between the GPs’ risk estimates and the calculated Framingham risk, as well as the lowest rate of correct decisions about treatment, were observed in the high-risk cases. This speaks to the uncertainties that doctors experience when estimating risk and deciding the treatment of high-risk patients, the patients for whom preventive efforts are most important. Preventive efforts in high-risk subjects are important, as the benefit of treatment increases with increased absolute risk. This observation has practical consequences, as the effectiveness of drug treatment in CHD prevention depends on the accuracy with which the clinician estimates risk in individual patients (Durrington et al., Reference Durrington, Prais, Bhatnagar, France, Crowley, Khan and Morgan1999; Grundy et al., Reference Grundy, Pasternak, Greenland, Smith and Fuster1999). Once the risk for a given individual is accurately identified, appropriate interventions exist that substantially reduce cardiac events. This paradigm assumes that decisions about treatment are direct consequences of estimates. However, our results support the opinion that risk assessments and decisions about treatment are complex cognitive processes that involve interactions between doctors’ knowledge, risk perception and the task of decision making (Reyna and Lloyd, Reference Reyna and Lloyd2006; Reyna, Reference Reyna2008).

In the present study, the gender of GPs and the length of their clinical experience were shown to influence their decisions about treatment. Female GPs performed better than male GPs and, in particular, were less prone to start treatment in low-risk cases.

Previous research has suggested that male and female physicians differ in the treatment of patients with heart failure (Baumhäkel et al., Reference Baumhäkel, Müller and Böhm2009), in the control of some risk factors in patients with diabetes (Berthold et al., Reference Berthold, Gouni-Berthold, Bestehorn, Böhm and Krone2008), and in providing preventive care (Henderson and Weisman, Reference Henderson and Weisman2001). It has been proposed that perception and interpretation of clinical symptoms may be different because female physicians tend to have a more patient-centred communication style (Roter et al., Reference Roter, Hall and Aoki2002) and to spend more time with the patient (Bertakis et al., Reference Bertakis, Helms, Callahan, Azari and Robbins1995). However, our study is based on paper-simulated cases, which eliminates gender differences due to the interaction between the physician and the patient. Therefore, we can speculate that the gender differences in treatment decisions observed in our sample may reflect true differences in the decision-making process that are independent of factors related to the physician–patient relationship.

We also found that the length of time in clinical practice seems to affect decisions. Shorter experience was associated with a higher number of correct decisions. These findings support previous reports of lower quality care with increasing years in practice (Choudhry et al., Reference Choudhry, Fletcher and Soumerai2005; Southern et al., Reference Southern, Bellin and Arnsten2011).

There are some limitations to this study. First, the 42–45% response rate, although not unusual for a mail survey of physicians (Castaldo et al., Reference Castaldo, Nester, Wasser, Masiado, Rossi, Young, Napolitano and Sanford Schwartz2005; Christian et al., Reference Christian, Mills, Simpson and Mosca2006; Erhardt and Hobbs, Reference Erhardt and Hobbs2007), compromises generalizability of the study results to all doctors. In addition, the response rate produced relatively small groups for the statistical analyses. In this case, there may be the risk of a type II error, that is, failing to find a true association between the task of risk assessments and the task of treatment decisions because of the small sample size. Second, case vignettes limited to a few variables may not reflect real-life practice. However, the use of case vignettes has been shown to be an effective method to measure the quality of physicians’ practice (Peabody et al., Reference Peabody, Luck, Glassman, Jain, Hansen, Spell and Lee2004; Veloski et al., Reference Veloski, Tai, Evans and Nash2005). Third, we cannot exclude the possibility that physicians responded to the questionnaire in an manner that does not accurately reflect their practice, and we cannot eliminate the possibility that some doctors may have used risk calculators, even though they were instructed not to. Finally, risk assessments and treatment decisions may be influenced by several other factors than what is included in the case vignettes or attributable to the individual doctors. Such environmental factors may be information campaign from health services, the media or by pharmaceutical industries. The possibility of these influences may further limit the generalizability of our results.

Conclusions

GPs seem to underestimate CHD risk when compared with the calculated Framingham risk. Female GPs are more likely to make correct decisions, and GPs with more experience may paradoxically provide lower quality care. These findings may have practical consequences, as they indicate some level of inappropriate CHD primary prevention. Innovative educational approaches are needed to improve the quality of medical decision making.

Acknowledgement

The authors express their appreciation to Rosemary Tate, PhD, for her contribution to the statistical analyses.

References

Backlund, L., Bring, J.Strender, L.-E. 2004: How accurately do general practitioners and students estimate coronary risk in hypercholesterolaemic patients? Primary Health Care Research and Development 5, 145152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baguet, J.-P., Fiquet, B., Yau, C.Mounier-Vehier, C. 2007: Attitudes to and application of the concept of overall cardiovascular risk: comparison of male and female primary care providers. Journal of Human Hypertension 21, 359365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumhäkel, M., Müller, U.Böhm, M. 2009: Influence of gender of physicians and patients on guideline-recommended treatment of chronic heart failure in a cross-sectional study. European Journal of Heart Failure 11, 299303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertakis, K.D., Helms, L.J., Callahan, E.J., Azari, R.Robbins, J.A. 1995: The influence of gender on physician practice style. Medical Care 33, 407416.Google Scholar
Berthold, H.K., Gouni-Berthold, I., Bestehorn, H.P., Böhm, M.Krone, W. 2008: Physician gender is associated with the quality of type 2 diabetes care. Journal of Internal Medicine 264, 340350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castaldo, J., Nester, J., Wasser, T., Masiado, T., Rossi, M., Young, M., Napolitano, J.J.Sanford Schwartz, J. 2005: Physician attitudes regarding cardiovascular risk reduction: the gap between clinical importance, knowledge, and effectiveness. Disease Management 8, 93105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choudhry, N.K., Fletcher, R.H.Soumerai, S.B. 2005: Systematic review: the relationship between clinical experience and quality of health care. Annals of Internal Medicine 142, 260273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christian, A.H., Mills, T., Simpson, S.L.Mosca, L. 2006: Quality of cardiovascular disease preventive care and physician/practice characteristics. Journal of General Internal Medicine 21, 231237.Google Scholar
Cooney, M.T., Dudina, A., Whincup, P., Capewell, S., Menotti, A., Jousilahti, P., Njolstadi, I., Oganov, R., Thomsen, T., Tverdral, A., Wedel, H., Wilhelmsen, L., Graham, I.on behalf of the SCORE Investigators. 2009: Re-evaluating the Rose approach: comparative benefits of the population and high-risk preventive strategies. European Journal of Cardiovascular Prevention & Rehabilitation 16, 541549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Backer, G., Ambrosioni, E., Borch-Johnsen, K., Brotons, C., Cifkova, R., Dallongeville, J.et al. 2003: European guidelines on cardiovascular disease prevention in clinical practice. Third Joint Task Force Of European And other Societies On Cardiovascular Prevention In Clinical Practice. European Heart Journal 24, 16011610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dolan, J.G., Bordley, D.R.Mushlin, A.I. 1986: An evaluation of clinicians’ subjective prior probability estimates. Medical Decision Making 6, 216223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doroodchi, H., Abdolrasulnia, M., Foster, J.A., Foster, E., Turakhia, M.P., Skelding, K.A., Sagar, K.Casebeer, L.L. 2008: Knowledge and attitudes of primary care physicians in the management of patients at risk for cardiovascular events. BMC Family Practice 9, 42.Google Scholar
Durrington, P.N., Prais, H., Bhatnagar, D., France, M., Crowley, V., Khan, J.Morgan, J. 1999: Indications for cholesterol-lowering medication: comparison of risk-assessment methods. Lancet 353, 278281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eichler, K., Zoller, M., Tschudi, P.Steurer, J. 2007: Barriers to apply cardiovascular prediction rules in primary care: a postal survey. BMC Family Practice 8, 1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erhardt, L.R.Hobbs, F.D.R. 2007: A global survey of physicians’ perception on cholesterol management: the From Heart Study. International Journal of Clinical Practice 61, 10781085.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Expert Panel on Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Cholesterol in Adults. 2001: Executive summary of the Third Report of the National Cholesterol Education Program (NCEP) Expert Panel on Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Cholesterol in Adults (Adult Treatment Panel III). The Journal of the American Medical Association 285, 24862497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ford, E.S., Mokdad, A.H., Giles, W.H.Mensah, G.A. 2003: Serum total cholesterol concentration and awareness, treatment and control of hypercholesterolemia among US adults: findings from the National Health And Nutrition Examination Survey, 1999 to 2000. Circulation 107, 21852189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedmann, P.D., Brett, A.S.Mayo-Smith, M.F. 1996: Differences in generalists’ and cardiologists’ perception of cardiovascular risk and the outcomes of preventive therapy in cardiovascular disease. Annals of Internal Medicine 124, 414421.Google Scholar
Graham, I.M., Stewart, M.Hertog, M.L. 2006: on behalf of the Cardiovascular Round Table Task Force Factors impeding the implementation of cardiovascular prevention guidelines: findings from a survey conducted by the European Society of Cardiology. European Journal of Cardiovascular Prevention & Rehabilitation 13, 839845.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grundy, S.M., Pasternak, R., Greenland, P., Smith, S.Fuster, V. 1999: Assessment of cardiovascular risk by use of multiple-risk-factor assessment equations. Circulation 100, 14811492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henderson, J.T.Weisman, C.S. 2001: Physician gender effects on preventive screening and counseling. An analysis of male and female patients’ health care experiences. Medical Care 39, 12811292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, D.R., Kushniruk, A.W., Yale, J.-F.Patel, V.L. 1999: Conceptual knowledge and decision strategies in relation to hypercholesterolemia and coronary heart disease. International Journal of Medical Informatics 55, 159177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peabody, J.W., Luck, J., Glassman, P., Jain, S., Hansen, J., Spell, M.Lee, M. 2004: Measuring the quality of physician practice using clinical vignettes: a prospective validation study. Annals of Internal Medicine 141, 771780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redelmeier, D.A.Shafir, E. 1995: Medical decision making in situations that offer multiple alternatives. The Journal of the American Medical Association 273, 302305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reyna, V.F. 2008: A theory of medical decision making and health: fuzzy trace theory. Medical Decision Making 28, 850865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reyna, V.F.Lloyd, F.J. 2006: Physician decision making and cardiac risk: effect of knowledge, risk perception, risk tolerance, and fuzzy processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied 12, 179195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roter, D.L., Hall, J.A.Aoki, Y. 2002: Physician gender effects in medical communication: a meta-analytic review. The Journal of the American Medical Association 288, 756764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Southern, W.N., Bellin, E.Y.Arnsten, J.H. 2011: Longer lengths of stay and higher risk of mortality among inpatients of physicians with more years in practice. American Journal of Medicine 124, 868874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabenkin, H., Eaton, C.B., Roberts, M.B., Parker, D.R., McMurray, J.H.Borkan, J. 2010: Differences in cardiovascular disease risk management in primary care by sex of physician and patient. Annals of Family Medicine 8, 2532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vancheri, F., Strender, L.-E., Bring, J., Montgomery, H., Skaner, Y.Backlund, L.G. 2008: General practitioners’ coronary risk assessment and lipid-lowering treatment decisions in primary prevention: comparison between two European areas with different cardiovascular risk levels. Primary Health Care Research & Development 9, 248256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veloski, J., Tai, S., Evans, A.S.Nash, D.B. 2005: Clinical vignette-based surveys: a tool for assessing physician practice variation. American Journal of Medical Quality 20, 151157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, D., De Backer, G., Faergeman, O., Graham, I., Mancia, G.Pyörälä, K. 1998: Prevention of coronary heart disease in clinical practice. Recommendations of the Second Joint Task Force of European and other Societies on Coronary Prevention. European Heart Journal 19, 14341503.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Example of a case description

Figure 1

Figure 2 Box plot of doctors’ risk estimates in the R group (empty bars) and R + D group (filled bars) and summary of the nine cases along with the calculated Framingham risk level. Framingham score is GPs’ risk estimates minus Framingham risk levels. The bottom of the boxes is at the first quartile, the top is at the third quartile and the continuous lines across the boxes are at the median value. The whiskers are drawn to the highest and lowest values that are not considered as outliers. Outliers, marked with dots, are estimates outside these limits. The first five cases are low-risk cases, according to Framingham. The others are high-risk cases, eligible for treatment.

Figure 2

Figure 3 Proportion of ‘yes’ decisions (R + D and D combined groups) by GPs’ gender for each case according to the level of calculated Framingham risk.

Figure 3

Figure 4 Plot of probability of correct decisions against the length of clinical experience by gender. The squares (empty = female doctors and filled = male doctors) represent the predicted proportions of correct decisions. Each square represents one to six doctors with the same number of years of clinical practice.