Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T06:12:33.617Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Abnormal dynamic functional connectivity of hippocampal subregions associated with working memory impairment in melancholic depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 December 2021

Lai Shunkai
Affiliation:
Medical Imaging Center, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China Department of Psychiatry, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Ting Su
Affiliation:
Medical Imaging Center, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China Institute of Molecular and Functional Imaging, Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Shuming Zhong
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Guangmao Chen
Affiliation:
Medical Imaging Center, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China Institute of Molecular and Functional Imaging, Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Yiliang Zhang
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Hui Zhao
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Pan Chen
Affiliation:
Medical Imaging Center, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China Institute of Molecular and Functional Imaging, Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Guixian Tang
Affiliation:
Medical Imaging Center, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China Institute of Molecular and Functional Imaging, Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Zhangzhang Qi
Affiliation:
Medical Imaging Center, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China Institute of Molecular and Functional Imaging, Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Jiali He
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Yunxia Zhu
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Sihui Lv
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Zijin Song
Affiliation:
School of Management, Jinan University, Guangzhou 510316, China
Haofei Miao
Affiliation:
Institute of Molecular and Functional Imaging, Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Yilei Hu
Affiliation:
School of Management, Jinan University, Guangzhou 510316, China
Yanbin Jia*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
Ying Wang*
Affiliation:
Medical Imaging Center, First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China Institute of Molecular and Functional Imaging, Jinan University, Guangzhou 510630, China
*
Author for correspondence: Ying Wang, E-mail: johneil@vip.sina.com; Yanbin Jia, E-mail: yanbinjia2006@163.com
Author for correspondence: Ying Wang, E-mail: johneil@vip.sina.com; Yanbin Jia, E-mail: yanbinjia2006@163.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Previous studies have demonstrated structural and functional changes of the hippocampus in patients with major depressive disorder (MDD). However, no studies have analyzed the dynamic functional connectivity (dFC) of hippocampal subregions in melancholic MDD. We aimed to reveal the patterns for dFC variability in hippocampus subregions – including the bilateral rostral and caudal areas and its associations with cognitive impairment in melancholic MDD.

Methods

Forty-two treatment-naive MDD patients with melancholic features and 55 demographically matched healthy controls were included. The sliding-window analysis was used to evaluate whole-brain dFC for each hippocampal subregions seed. We assessed between-group differences in the dFC variability values of each hippocampal subregion in the whole brain and cognitive performance on the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB). Finally, association analysis was conducted to investigate their relationships.

Results

Patients with melancholic MDD showed decreased dFC variability between the left rostral hippocampus and left anterior lobe of cerebellum compared with healthy controls (voxel p < 0.005, cluster p < 0.0125, GRF corrected), and poorer cognitive scores in working memory, verbal learning, visual learning, and social cognition (all p < 0.05). Association analysis showed that working memory was positively correlated with the dFC variability values of the left rostral hippocampus-left anterior lobe of the cerebellum (r = 0.338, p = 0.029) in melancholic MDD.

Conclusions

These findings confirmed the distinct dynamic functional pathway of hippocampal subregions in patients with melancholic MDD, and suggested that the dysfunction of hippocampus-cerebellum connectivity may be underlying the neural substrate of working memory impairment in melancholic MDD.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The latest nationwide survey of mental disorders showed the lifetime prevalence rates of major depressive disorder (MDD) at 3.4% in the general population of China [95% confidence interval (CI): 2.9–3.9%] (Huang et al., Reference Huang, Wang, Wang, Liu, Yu, Yan and Wu2019). Meanwhile, the World Health Organization has predicted MDD to become the first-leading cause of the global burden of disease in 2030 (Collins et al., Reference Collins, Patel, Joestl, March, Insel, Daar and Stein2011). Due to the inherently heterogeneous of MDD, the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the pathophysiology become quite complex. Melancholic features, a subtype of MDD in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) with highly homogeneities, characterized by lack of reactivity, psychomotor retardation, agitation, weight loss and inappropriate guilt (Duckworth, Reference Duckworth2015). Tondo et al., found a prevalence of DSM-5 melancholic features in MDD patients at intake measured with the 21-item version of the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (21-HDRS) of 33.9% (Tondo, Vazquez, & Baldessarini, Reference Tondo, Vazquez and Baldessarini2020). Moreover, patients with the melancholic depressive subtype demonstrated a higher risk of suicidality, greater depression severity and worsen cognitive performance than those MDD patients without melancholic features (Caldieraro et al., Reference Caldieraro, Baeza, Pinheiro, Ribeiro, Parker and Fleck2013; Jeon et al., Reference Jeon, Peng, Chua, Srisurapanont, Fava, Bae and Hong2013; Roca et al., Reference Roca, Monzon, Vives, Lopez-Navarro, Garcia-Toro, Vicens and Gili2015). In addition to differences in psychological characteristics, these two major subtypes of depression may also differ in pathophysiological mechanisms including inflammatory, metabolic, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, hypothalamic-pituitary-thyroid (HPT) axis and brain function (Duval et al., Reference Duval, Mokrani, Monreal-Ortiz, Fattah, Champeval, Schulz and Macher2006; Lamers et al., Reference Lamers, Vogelzangs, Merikangas, de Jonge, Beekman and Penninx2013; Shan et al., Reference Shan, Cui, Liu, Li, Huang, Tang and Xie2021; Soriano-Mas et al., Reference Soriano-Mas, Hernandez-Ribas, Pujol, Urretavizcaya, Deus, Harrison and Cardoner2011; Vassilopoulou et al., Reference Vassilopoulou, Papathanasiou, Michopoulos, Boufidou, Oulis, Kelekis and Lykouras2013). Therefore, it is important to elucidate the pathogenic mechanisms underlying melancholic MDD, developing precise treatment strategies matching their unique biological characteristics and achieving the goal of ‘cognitive remission’.

Accumulating evidence has implicated that the structural changes of the hippocampus may be associated with the neural physiopathology of melancholic MDD. A recent study found a reduced left hippocampal volume in Met66 carriers in MDD patients with melancholic features when comprised of Val66 homozygotes (Cardoner et al., Reference Cardoner, Soria, Gratacos, Hernandez-Ribas, Pujol, Lopez-Sola and Soriano-Mas2013), as well as the reduced hippocampal volumes in the older population with melancholic depression (Hickie et al., Reference Hickie, Naismith, Ward, Turner, Scott, Mitchell and Parker2005). However, the inconsistent results suggested that there were no significant differences in hippocampal volume between groups of melancholic and other subtype depressed participants (Greenberg, Payne, MacFall, Steffens, & Krishnan, Reference Greenberg, Payne, MacFall, Steffens and Krishnan2008; Rusch, Abercrombie, Oakes, Schaefer, & Davidson, Reference Rusch, Abercrombie, Oakes, Schaefer and Davidson2001; Vasilopoulou et al., Reference Vasilopoulou, Papathanasiou, Michopoulos, Boufidou, Oulis, Nikolaou and Lykouras2011; Vassilopoulou et al., Reference Vassilopoulou, Papathanasiou, Michopoulos, Boufidou, Oulis, Kelekis and Lykouras2013). This inconsistency could arise from most neuroimaging studies of MDD patients with or without melancholic features who have considered the hippocampus as a single homogeneous structure. Based on the cytoarchitectonic characteristics of the hippocampal, Fan and colleagues suggested that it can be divided into the following major subregions: rostral and caudal hippocampus nuclei (Fan et al., Reference Fan, Li, Zhuo, Zhang, Wang, Chen and Jiang2016). The rostral hippocampus is closely linked to general memory processes including both learning, memory, encoding and retrieval, meanwhile, the caudal hippocampus is implicated more in spatial processing (Carr, Rissman, & Wagner, Reference Carr, Rissman and Wagner2010; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Barron, Kirby, Bottenhorn, Hill, Murphy and Fox2015; Zeidman & Maguire, Reference Zeidman and Maguire2016). Abnormalities in hippocampus subregion-based networks or volume have been found in post-traumatic stress disorder (Lazarov, Zhu, Suarez-Jimenez, Rutherford, & Neria, Reference Lazarov, Zhu, Suarez-Jimenez, Rutherford and Neria2017; Malivoire, Girard, Patel, & Monson, Reference Malivoire, Girard, Patel and Monson2018; Suarez-Jimenez et al., Reference Suarez-Jimenez, Zhu, Lazarov, Mann, Schneier, Gerber and Markowitz2020), Alzheimer's disease (Bai et al., Reference Bai, Xie, Watson, Shi, Yuan, Wang and Zhang2011) and chronic stress population (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Wei, Han, Jin, Xu, Dong and Peng2019). However, very few studies investigate hippocampus subregion-based dysfunction in melancholic MDD. Due to the underlying functional differences between the anterior and posterior hippocampus, further studies are encouraged to investigate hippocampus dysfunction at a subregional level in melancholic MDD.

Recently, investigations of depression-related differences using resting-state functional connectivity (FC) have begun to emerge. Regarding static FC, previous studies have revealed the aberrant FC between the hippocampal subregions and cortical and subcortical regions or associated neural circuits in MDD (Cao et al., Reference Cao, Liu, Xu, Li, Gao, Sun and Zhang2012; Fateh et al., Reference Fateh, Long, Duan, Cui, Pang, Farooq and Chen2019). However, most static FC studies on MDD implicitly assumed that FC was stationary throughout the entire resting scan period. It has been shown that human brain connectivity is dynamic and associated with ongoing rhythmic activity over time rather than stationarity(Allen et al., Reference Allen, Damaraju, Plis, Erhardt, Eichele and Calhoun2014; Reinen et al., Reference Reinen, Chen, Hutchison, Yeo, Anderson, Sabuncu and Holmes2018). The dynamic functional connectivity (dFC) analysis could provide abundant information about the time-varying functional architecture of specific regions, and could be a powerful supplement to static FC (Han et al., Reference Han, Wu, Wang, Sun, Ding, Cao and Zhou2018; Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Whitfield-Gabrieli, Dillon, Goer, Beltzer, Minkel and Pizzagalli2016). dFC is sensitive to behavioral performance and emotional measures, and also be a sensitive prognostic indicator of disease progression in neuropsychiatric disorders including Alzheimer's disease, depression, and schizophrenia (Greicius, Reference Greicius2008; Liao et al., Reference Liao, Li, Duan, Cui, Chen and Chen2018; Shirer, Ryali, Rykhlevskaia, Menon, & Greicius, Reference Shirer, Ryali, Rykhlevskaia, Menon and Greicius2012). Therefore, the investigation of dFC may provide a nuanced view of the disrupted brain communication in MDD and a better understanding of the pathological mechanisms underlying this disorder. Of note, previous studies revealed abnormal dFC variability between the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and insular regions (Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Whitfield-Gabrieli, Dillon, Goer, Beltzer, Minkel and Pizzagalli2016; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Wang, Huang, Jia, Zheng, Zhong and Huang2020), between the mPFC and posterior cingulate cortex (Wise et al., Reference Wise, Marwood, Perkins, Herane-Vives, Joules, Lythgoe and Arnone2017), between mPFC and parahippocampal gyrus (Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser, Whitfield-Gabrieli, Dillon, Goer, Beltzer, Minkel and Pizzagalli2016), and between the default mode network (DMN) and central executive network (Demirtas et al., Reference Demirtas, Tornador, Falcon, Lopez-Sola, Hernandez-Ribas, Pujol and Deco2016), as well as the greater dFC strengths in the precentral gyrus (Pang et al., Reference Pang, Zhang, Cui, Yang, Lu, Chen and Chen2020). These results consistently suggested that the alterations of dFC between regions of the DMN and areas of the prefrontal cortex or insula or hippocampus are believed to play important roles in emotional regulation and also may underlie the neural mechanisms of MDD (Long et al., Reference Long, Cao, Yan, Chen, Li, Castellanos and Liu2020). However, we are only beginning to understand the precise anatomy of the hippocampus in humans, little is known about the abnormal dFC variability at its subregional level of melancholic MDD patients.

Cognitive impairment is acknowledged as a core feature of clinical manifestations of all MDD subtypes. Patients with melancholic MDD showed prominent cognitive deficits including verbal memory, executive function, visual learning, attention, working memory and processing speed (Bosaipo, Foss, Young, & Juruena, Reference Bosaipo, Foss, Young and Juruena2017; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Xu, Lu, Ouyang, Dang, Lorenzo-Seva and Lee2014; Withall, Harris, & Cumming, Reference Withall, Harris and Cumming2010; Zaninotto et al., Reference Zaninotto, Solmi, Veronese, Guglielmo, Ioime, Camardese and Serretti2016). Previous studies indicated that patients fail to regain full functional recovery even in a euthymic state, which may be partly attributed to cognitive deficits (Pan et al., Reference Pan, Park, Brietzke, Zuckerman, Rong, Mansur and McIntyre2019; Woo, Rosenblat, Kakar, Bahk, & McIntyre, Reference Woo, Rosenblat, Kakar, Bahk and McIntyre2016). The hippocampal formation is heterogeneous and consists of different subregions that are complexly interacted with diverse brain areas, which form the neuroanatomical network of emotion regulation and cognitive processing (Bremner, Vythilingam, Vermetten, Vaccarino, & Charney, Reference Bremner, Vythilingam, Vermetten, Vaccarino and Charney2004; Drevets, Reference Drevets2000; Fateh et al., Reference Fateh, Long, Duan, Cui, Pang, Farooq and Chen2019; Rive et al., Reference Rive, van Rooijen, Veltman, Phillips, Schene and Ruhe2013). For instance, reduced hippocampal volumes were associated with visual and verbal memory deficit in patients with melancholic depression (Hickie et al., Reference Hickie, Naismith, Ward, Turner, Scott, Mitchell and Parker2005), and executive dysfunction in MDD (Frodl et al., Reference Frodl, Schaub, Banac, Charypar, Jager, Kummler and Meisenzahl2006; Khan et al., Reference Khan, Ryali, Bhat, Prakash, Srivastava and Khanam2015). A previous study suggested that the longitudinal changes in FC between the left cornu ammonis of the hippocampus and posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus were positively correlated with cognitive impairment in remitted late-onset depression (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Yuan, Bai, Shu, You, Li and Zhang2015). And the less posterior-DMN-hippocampal connectivity was associated with higher cognitive reactivity and rumination in MDD (Figueroa et al., Reference Figueroa, Mocking, van Wingen, Martens, Ruhe and Schene2017). A recent study also found the abnormal resting-state FC of hippocampal subfields may be related to the impairment of working memory in MDD patients (Hao et al., Reference Hao, Zhong, Ma, Xu, Kong, Wu and Wang2020). Moreover, the FC between the rostral hippocampus and the inferior part of the lateral occipital cortex mediated the negative relationship between cortisol and visuospatial memory in healthy young adults (Hakamata et al., Reference Hakamata, Komi, Sato, Izawa, Mizukami, Moriguchi and Tagaya2019). Taken together, early works suggested that changes in FC of the hippocampus can be related to the cognitive deficits in depression, but much remains unknown about the neurocognitive significance of dFC. Brain dynamics reflect the neural system's functional capacity and the spontaneous fluctuations in moment-to-moment behavioral variability (Kucyi, Hove, Esterman, Hutchison, & Valera, Reference Kucyi, Hove, Esterman, Hutchison and Valera2017), and these fluctuations may involve in a wide range of cognitive processes and emotional regulation. Previous studies have indicated that decreased FC variability in the DMN is associated with slower processing speed and executive function impairment in bipolar disorder patients (Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Kovacevic, Dev, Lu, Liu and Eyler2017). Unfortunately, little is known about the alerted dFC variability in hippocampal subregions that may underlie the melancholic MDD-relevant cognitive impairment.

To address these questions, we collected several resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) data from 42 unmedicated melancholic MDD patients and 55 matched controls to detected the dFC alterations in hippocampal subregions in the present study. Meanwhile, a cognitive assessment was conducted using the Chinese version of the Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB). We hypothesized that melancholic MDD patients would exhibit an abnormal dFC variability of the hippocampal subregions, and hope to probe the neurobiological signature of this refined depression subtype. And our second objective was to explore the association between the abnormal dFC variability and the cognitive performance of this disorder.

Materials and methods

Participants

A total of 55 right-handed, unmedicated, melancholic features MDD patients between the ages of 17 and 35 years from the psychiatry department of First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou, China enrolled in this cross-sectional trial. Two experienced psychiatrists (YJ and SZ, with 23 years and 6 years of experience in clinical psychiatry, respectively) followed the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) diagnostic criteria for the presence of melancholic features in major depressive episodes of MDD, based on eight items selected from the 24-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (24-item HDRS). In accord with DSM-5 criteria, all patients met the criteria for MDD with melancholic features, which required (1) pervasive anhedonia and/or nonreactive mood and (2) three (or more) of the following: characteristic depressive mood, regularly worse in the morning, early morning awakening, marked psychomotor agitation or retardation, significant anorexia or weight loss, and excessive or inappropriate guilt.

All subjects included in comparisons with melancholic features had intake 24-item HDRS total scores of ⩾ 20 (moderate-severe depression) and the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) total score <7 was able to participate. Participants were excluded if they met one of the following criteria: (1) other serious psychiatric disorders and symptoms (with the exception of MDD and anxiety disorders/symptoms); (2) a history of the use of any psychotropic medication, psychotherapy, or electroconvulsive therapy; (3) a history of neurological or organic brain disorder; (4) a history of alcohol/substance abuse or dependence; and (5) any physical illness demonstrated by personal history or clinical or laboratory examinations, pregnancy, or postpartum depression. Finally, 10 patients were excluded due to the following reasons: other psychiatric disorders rather than melancholic MDD (n = 4) based on a Chinese version of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID), confirmed medical diseases (n = 1), inability to comprehend consent procedures or refusal to provide consent forms (n = 2), and later switch to bipolar disorder patients in the 12-month longitudinal follow-up (n = 3).

Besides, 55 right-handed volunteers who participated as healthy controls (HCs) were recruited from Jinan University and the community. They were carefully screened through a diagnostic interview, the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Nonpatient Edition (SCID-NP), to rule out the presence of current or past psychiatric illness in self or first-degree relatives or past substance abuse/dependence.

We employed the 24-item HDRS and YMRS to obtain a comprehensive measure of depressive and mania symptoms. Meanwhile, the 14-item Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HAMA) was used to assess the severity of anxiety symptoms. The two psychiatrists with 23 years and 6 years of experience in clinical psychiatry attended a training session on the use of the 24-item HDRS, YMRS and HAMA before the start of the current study. After training, the inter-rater correlation coefficient of 24-item HDRS, YMRS and HAMA total scores between two raters was over 0.8.

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, China. All participants signed informed consent forms after reviewing a full written and verbal explanation of the study. And the neuropsychological assessment, and MRI scanning was completed within 48 h of initial contact.

Cognitive assessments

Cognitive function was qualified using the MCCB (Shi et al., Reference Shi, Kang, Yao, Ma, Li, Liang and Yu2015). The final MCCB battery requires approximately 70 min to administer and it consists of Trail Making Test Part A; Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: Symbol coding; Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT); Wechsler Memory Scale Spatial span; Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (NAB): Mazes; Brief Visuospatial Memory Test; Category fluency; Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT): Managing Emotions; and the Continuous Performance Test: Identical Pairs. MCCB was used to evaluate seven domains of information processing speed, attention/alertness, working memory, verbal learning, visual learning, reasoning and problem-solving and social cognition, with a global composite score. Of note, the clinical validity and test-retest reliability were established in both healthy controls and MDD patients (Liang et al., Reference Liang, Yu, Ma, Luo, Zhang, Sun and Zhang2020; Shi et al., Reference Shi, Kang, Yao, Ma, Li, Liang and Yu2015). Specifically, the effect size for test-retest reliability of nine cognitive subtests varied from 0.73 to 0.94 and the Cronbach's alpha of each item in the internal consistency analysis was ranged from 0.78 to 0.83. And two graduate students attended a training session on the use of the MCCB cognitive battery. After training, the inter-rater correlation coefficient of MCCB between two raters was over 0.8.

Image acquisition and preprocessing

All MRI data were gathered on a GE Discovery MR750 3.0T System with an 8-channel phased-array head coil. The participants were scanned in a supine, head-first position with symmetrically placed cushions on both sides of the head to decrease motion. During the scanning, the participants were instructed to relax with their eyes closed without falling asleep. After the experiment, each participant confirmed not having fallen asleep.

The rs-fMRI data were acquired using a gradient-echo echo-planar imaging sequence with the following parameters: time repetition (TR)/time echo (TE) = 2000/25 ms; flip angle = 90°; voxel size = 3.75 × 3.75 × 3 mm3; field of view (FOV) = 240 × 240 mm2; matrix = 64 × 64; slice thickness/gap = 3.0/1.0 mm; 35 axial slices covering the whole brain; and 210 volumes acquired in 7 min. In addition, a three-dimensional brain volume imaging (3D-BRAVO) sequence covering the whole brain was used for structural data acquisition with the following parameters: TR/ TE = 8.2/3.2 ms; flip angle = 12°; bandwidth = 31.25 Hz; slice thickness/gap = 1.0/0 mm; matrix = 256 × 256; FOV = 240 × 240 mm2; NEX = 1; and acquisition time = 3 min 45 s. Routine MRI examination images were also collected for excluding any anatomic abnormality. All participants were found by two experienced neuroradiologists (ZQ and ZL, with 5 and 3 years of experience in neuroimaging, respectively) to confirm that there were no brain structural abnormalities.

Functional image data preprocessing

The preprocessing was conducted using Data Processing Assistant for Resting-State fMRI (DPABI_V3.0, http://restfmri.net/forum/DPABI) (Yan, Wang, Zuo, & Zang, Reference Yan, Wang, Zuo and Zang2016) which is based on Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). For each subject, the first 10 images of the rs-fMRI dataset were discarded to ensure steady-state longitudinal magnetization. The remaining 200 images were first slice-time corrected and then were realigned to the first image for correcting for inter-TR head motion. This realignment correction provided a record of the head motion within the rs-fMRI scan. All subjects should have no more than 2 mm maximum displacement in any plane, 2° of angular motion as well as 0.2 mm in mean frame-wise displacement (FD) (Jenkinson, Bannister, Brady, & Smith, Reference Jenkinson, Bannister, Brady and Smith2002). The individual T1 structural images were segmented (white matter, gray matter, and cerebrospinal fluid) using a segmentation toolbox. Then, the DARTEL toolbox was used to create a study-specific template for accurate normalization. Then, resting-state functional images were co-registered to the structural images and transformed into standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, resliced to a voxel size of 3 × 3 × 3 mm3 resolution. The data were removed linear trend and passed through a band-pass filter of 0.01–0.1 Hz. Several spurious covariates and their temporal derivatives were then regressed out from the time course of each voxel, including the signals of the brain global mean, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid as well as the Friston-24 parameters of head motion.

Dynamic functional connectivity variability analysis

Following previous work (Fan et al., Reference Fan, Li, Zhuo, Zhang, Wang, Chen and Jiang2016), seed-based dFC analyses were performed by placing regions of interest (ROIs) within four masks (bilateral rostral hippocampus and bilateral caudal hippocampus) using Brainnetome atlas (http://atlas.brainnetome.org/bnatlas.php) (Fig. 1). The dFC variability characteristics of the hippocampus were calculated using the sliding-window method based on the Temporal Dynamic Analysis (TDA) toolkits integrated into the DPABI software (http://rfmri.org/DPABI). The Hamming sliding window was selected for the whole-brain blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal time series; 50 TRs window length and step width of 1 TRs were selected for dFC analysis. The minimum window length should be no less than 1/f min (1/0.01 s = 100 s) according to previous studies (Leonardi & Van De Ville, Reference Leonardi and Van De Ville2015; Li, Duan, Cui, Chen, & Liao, Reference Li, Duan, Cui, Chen and Liao2019); the f min was defined as the minimum frequency of time series. Shorter window lengths might increase the risk of introducing spurious fluctuations in the observed dFC. The window length of 50 TRs (100 s) was selected to compute the temporal variability of FC because a longer window length might hinder the description of the temporal variability dynamics. Also, other window lengths (30 TRs and 70 TRs) and shifting steps (1 TRs) were tried to further examine their possible effects on dFC results (Liao et al., Reference Liao, Wu, Xu, Ji, Zhang, Zang and Lu2014). In total, 151 sliding windows of dFC were obtained (each sliding window matrix is 61 × 73 × 61 × 100s). For each sliding window, correlation maps were produced by computing the temporal correlation coefficient between the truncated time series of the hippocampus subregions and all the other voxels. Consequently, 151 sliding window correlation maps were obtained for each individual. To improve the normality of the correlation distribution, each correlation map was converted into z-value maps using Fisher's r-to-z transformation. Then, the dFC maps were computed by calculating the standard deviation of 151 sliding-window z-value maps. Then, z-standardization was applied for the dFC maps. Finally, all the dFC maps were smoothed using a 6 mm full width at half maximum Gaussian kernel.

Fig. 1. Four seeds of the hippocampus in the bilateral hemisphere; L (R), left (right) hemisphere.

Statistical analysis

All indicators (i.e. demographics, and cognitive function) were measured for normal distributions by goodness-of-fit testing (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, Levine's test of equality of error variances) using SPSS 24.0 software (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA). When comparing group differences in terms of demographics and clinical data, t test was used if continuous variables were normal; likewise, the Mann–Whitney U test was used if continuous variables were skewed. A χ2 test was used to compare the gender differences between the two groups.

We tested for group differences on the seven cognitive domains plus the MCCB composite score using a multivariate analysis of covariance with a subject type (melancholic MDD v. healthy controls) as a fixed factor and including age and education levels as a covariate. Bonferroni correction was applied to account for multiple testing, with the threshold for significance was set to p < 0.006 (adjusted α  = 0.006, 0.05/8).

The one-sample t test was performed to demonstrate the within-group dFC variability distribution of each subregion in patients with melancholic MDD and HCs (p < 0.05, uncorrected). To further examine the difference in dFC variability patterns between patients with melancholic MDD and HCs, a two-sample t test was performed on the standard deviation in the z value at each voxel within the union mask of one-sample t test results of the two groups. Age, gender and years of education were included as nuisance covariates in the comparisons. The cluster-level multiple comparison correction was conducted based on the Gaussian random field (GRF) theory (voxel p < 0.005; cluster p < 0.05/4 = 0.0125, corrected).

Once the significant group differences in dFC variability were observed in each subregion of the hippocampus, the Spearman correlation coefficient was calculated between the dFC variability values and MCCB T-scores (overall and specific domains) in patients with melancholic MDD. Also, we calculated the Spearman correlation coefficient between the demographic and clinical variables (age, education levels, onset age of illness, total number of MDD episodes, number of previous MDD episodes, duration of illness, 24-item HDRS score, and HAMA score), abnormal dFC variability values and MCCB T-scores (overall and specific domains) in the melancholic MDD group. All tests were two-tailed, and the significant level was set at a p value less than 0.05.

Validation analysis

Another 2 supplementary window lengths (30 TRs and 70 TRs) were applied to validate the main results of dFC with the window length of 50 TRs.

Results

Demographic information

Table 1 shows the demographic and clinical information for all the study participants. Three patients with melancholic MDD and none control participants were excluded from further analyses because of excessive head motion during the image acquisition. Finally, the participants were 42 patients with melancholic MDD and 55 healthy controls. There were no significant differences in age, sex, or education levels between the melancholic MDD group and HCs group (all p > 0.05).

Table 1. Demographic and clinical data of participants

MDD, major depressive disorder; HCs, healthy controls; 24-item HDRS, 24 item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; YMRS, Young Manic Rating Scale; HAMA, 14-item Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HAMA); MCCB, the Chinese version of the Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) Consensus Cognitive Battery; s.d., standard deviation.

Values are reported as mean (s.d., standard deviation).

a χ2 test.

b Mann–Whitney U test.

c Multivariate analysis of covariance.

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Group differences of cognitive function

The cognitive performance results of MCCB scores in patients with melancholic MDD and HCs are shown in Table 1. Compared with the HCs, patients with melancholic MDD showed significantly lower overall composite T-score (F = 25.347, p < 0.001), speed of processing (F = 5.663, p = 0.02), working memory (F = 9.457, p = 0.003), verbal learning (F = 14.229, p < 0.001), visual learning (F = 9.944, p = 0.002) and social cognition (F = 28.347, p < 0.001) at p < 0.05. After Bonferroni correction (adjusted α = 0.006, 0.05/8), those results were still significantly different between groups except for the domain of processing speed.

Dynamic functional connectivity variability of the hippocampal subregions

The one-sample t test revealed the dFC variability patterns for each hippocampal subregion in two groups (Fig. 2). The dFC spatial distribution in the melancholic MDD group were similar to those of the HCs group (p < 0.05, uncorrected for visual inspection). In both groups, the dFC of the bilateral rostral hippocampus were mainly located in the superior and middle frontal gyrus, cingulate gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus, temporal lobe, parietal lobe, postcentral, occipital regions and anterior and posterior lobe of the cerebellum, and the dFC of the bilateral caudal hippocampus were mainly located in the precuneus, temporal lobe, anterior and middle cingulate, hippocampus, parahippocampal, insula and anterior lobe of the cerebellum. However, statistical analysis revealed that compared with the HCs group, the melancholic MDD group exhibited decreased dFC variability between the left rostral hippocampus and left anterior lobe of the cerebellum (voxel p < 0.005, cluster p < 0.0125, GRF corrected). No significant differences were found in the whole dFC of the right rostral hippocampus and bilateral caudal hippocampus between the melancholic MDD group and the HCs group (Table 2; Fig. 3).

Fig. 2. dFC patterns of the bilateral rostral hippocampus (rHipp) and the bilateral caudal hippocampus (cHipp) in melancholic MDD patients and HCs (p < 0.05, uncorrected). The color bar represents a dynamic functional connection. dFC, dynamic functional connectivity; MDD, major depressive disorder; HCs, healthy controls.

Fig. 3. Significant dFC differences between the two groups for hippocampus seed, respectively (voxel p < 0.005, cluster p < 0.0125, GRF corrected). The color bar indicates the t values from the two-sample t test analysis. dFC, dynamic functional connectivity; rHipp, the rostral hippocampus; GRF, Gaussian random field; L (R), left (right) hemisphere.

Table 2. The areas of significantly different dFC between the melancholic MDD patients and the HCs (voxel p < 0.005, cluster p < 0.0125, GRF corrected)

dFC, dynamic functional connectivity; MDD, Major depressive disorder; HCs, healthy controls; GRF, Gaussian random field.

Correlation analyses

A significant positive correlation was observed between the dFC variability values of the left rostral hippocampus- left anterior lobe of the cerebellum and working memory T -score (r = 0.338, p = 0.029) only in patients with melancholic MDD (Fig. 4). After correcting for age and education levels, this correlation was still existing (r = 0.329, p = 0.038). But there were no significant correlations between demographic and clinical characteristics and dFC variability values between the left rostral hippocampus- left anterior lobe of the cerebellum in patients with melancholic MDD (all p > 0.05). Additionally, the verbal learning was negatively correlated with the 24-HDRS scores (r = −0.403, p = 0.008), but there were no significant correlations between other demographic and clinical characteristics (age, education levels, onset age of illness, total number of MDD episodes, number of previous MDD episodes, duration of illness, and HAMA scores) and MCCB cognitive domains in patients with melancholic MDD (all p > 0.05).

Fig. 4. Positive correlation between the abnormal dFC variability values and working memory T-score (r = 0.338, p = 0.029). dFC, dynamic functional connectivity; rHipp, the rostral hippocampus; L (R), left (right) hemisphere.

Validation results

The validation results in 30 TRs sliding window length between the two groups also showed melancholic MDD patients exhibited decreased dFC variability values between the left rostral hippocampus and left anterior lobe of the cerebellum (online Supplementary Table S1 and Fig. S1). However, there were no significant differences in 70 TRs sliding window length and static FC between the two groups using the hippocampus as the ROIs.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this study was the first to investigate the whole-brain dFC of hippocampal subregions in unmedicated patients with melancholic MDD, as well as to explore the relationship between the abnormal dFC variability values and the cognitive performance of this disorder. The main findings of this study showed melancholic MDD have decreased dFC variability values between the left rostral hippocampus and left anterior lobe of cerebellum than that in healthy controls. Our results also indicated that the melancholic MDD patients may have a profile of widespread cognitive impairments, showing in the domains of working memory, verbal learning, visual learning and social cognition, as well as MCCB composite scores. And verbal learning was negatively correlated with the 24-HDRS scores. Furthermore, correlation analysis showed that the decreased dFC variability values between the left rostral hippocampus and left anterior lobe of the cerebellum were positively correlated to working memory impairment.

Decreased dFC variability values of the subgenual hippocampus in melancholic MDD

A growing number of studies suggested that the cerebellum and hippocampus play an important role in cognition processing and emotional regulation (Anacker & Hen, Reference Anacker and Hen2017; Guo et al., Reference Guo, Liu, Dai, Jiang, Zhang, Yu and Xiao2013; Reshetnikov et al., Reference Reshetnikov, Kovner, Lepeshko, Pavlov, Grinkevich and Bondar2020; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Xu, Liu, Ji, Wu, Wang and Yu2017). In our current study, patients with melancholic MDD showed decreased dFC variability between the left rostral hippocampus and left anterior lobe of the cerebellum relevant to healthy controls. The previous study has indicated that the melancholic group had a greater number of early life stress (ELS) events than the non-melancholic patients (Quinn, Dobson-Stone, Outhred, Harris, & Kemp, Reference Quinn, Dobson-Stone, Outhred, Harris and Kemp2012), and the left hippocampus is more sensitive to stressful events than the right hippocampus (Saleh et al., Reference Saleh, Potter, McQuoid, Boyd, Turner, MacFall and Taylor2017; Teicher, Anderson, & Polcari, Reference Teicher, Anderson and Polcari2012). Interesting, the previous study has also pointed out that the left hippocampus is typically more affected than the right hippocampus in depression and other psychiatric disorders (Small, Schobel, Buxton, Witter, & Barnes, Reference Small, Schobel, Buxton, Witter and Barnes2011). Moreover, the anterior hippocampus mainly contributes to emotional reactions (Therriault et al., Reference Therriault, Wang, Mathotaarachchi, Pascoal, Parent, Beaudry and Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging2019), and this hippocampus subregion also differed between the depressed patients and controls (Ballmaier et al., Reference Ballmaier, Narr, Toga, Elderkin-Thompson, Thompson, Hamilton and Kumar2008; Posener et al., Reference Posener, Wang, Price, Gado, Province, Miller and Csernansky2003). Collectively, the dysfunction of the left anterior hippocampus may explain why the melancholic MDD patients suffered greater depression severity than typical MDD.

Moreover, previous studies found that the melancholic MDD patients were associated with lower BDNF levels (Patas et al., Reference Patas, Penninx, Bus, Vogelzangs, Molendijk, Elzinga and Oude Voshaar2014; Primo de Carvalho Alves & Sica da Rocha, Reference Primo de Carvalho Alves and Sica da Rocha2018) and the Met66 carriers in melancholic MDD patients paralleled with a reduced left hippocampal volume (Cardoner et al., Reference Cardoner, Soria, Gratacos, Hernandez-Ribas, Pujol, Lopez-Sola and Soriano-Mas2013). These changes may cause the left hippocampus to reduce its functional connections to other cortical regions due to synaptic depletion. Meanwhile, recent studies have also reported important functional interactions between the cerebellum and the hippocampal formation (Igloi et al., Reference Igloi, Doeller, Paradis, Benchenane, Berthoz, Burgess and Rondi-Reig2015; Onuki, Van Someren, De Zeeuw, & Van der Werf, Reference Onuki, Van Someren, De Zeeuw and Van der Werf2015; O'Reilly, Beckmann, Tomassini, Ramnani, & Johansen-Berg, Reference O'Reilly, Beckmann, Tomassini, Ramnani and Johansen-Berg2010; Watson et al., Reference Watson, Obiang, Torres-Herraez, Watilliaux, Coulon, Rochefort and Rondi-Reig2019). Functional brain networks demonstrate significant temporal variability and dynamic reconfiguration, and this ability of specific regions to dynamically connectivity may play an important role in cognitive flexibility and behavioral adaptability (Bray, Arnold, Levy, & Iaria, Reference Bray, Arnold, Levy and Iaria2015; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Cheng, Liu, Zhang, Lei, Yao and Feng2016). The melancholic MDD showed decreased dFC in hippocampus-cerebellum signifies a lack of flexibility changes in spontaneous brain activity and neural communication over time, which may be a neural maker of melancholic features. The reduced hippocampal volumes have been found in the melancholic depression (Cardoner et al., Reference Cardoner, Soria, Gratacos, Hernandez-Ribas, Pujol, Lopez-Sola and Soriano-Mas2013; Hickie et al., Reference Hickie, Naismith, Ward, Turner, Scott, Mitchell and Parker2005), which can predict a slower recovery after treatment initiation (Soriano-Mas et al., Reference Soriano-Mas, Hernandez-Ribas, Pujol, Urretavizcaya, Deus, Harrison and Cardoner2011). The evidence showed an inverse correlation between the volume of the deep white matter hyperintensities and hippocampal volume, as well as a direct influence on the connectivity properties of this important cerebral region (Porcu et al., Reference Porcu, Operamolla, Scapin, Garofalo, Destro, Caneglias and Saba2020), and thus influence the state and dynamic connectivity between hippocampal and cerebellum. Furthermore, a recent study suggested modulating the function of the hippocampus–cerebellum circuit may be a potential therapeutic strategy for depressive symptoms in epilepsy patients (Peng et al., Reference Peng, Mao, Yin, Sun, Wang, Zhang and Wang2018). Taken together, these findings suggest that impaired hippocampus-cerebellum circuits function might contribute to the pathogenesis of melancholic MDD and may provide a potential target for therapeutic intervention.

Impairments of MCCB cognitive performance in melancholic MDD

In the current study, we found the melancholic MDD have significantly lower scores in the cognitive domains of working memory, verbal learning, visual learning and social cognition than that in healthy controls, which are consistent with most of the previous studies evaluating cognitive performance in patients with melancholic MDD (Day et al., Reference Day, Gatt, Etkin, DeBattista, Schatzberg and Williams2015; Quinn, Harris, Felmingham, Boyce, & Kemp, Reference Quinn, Harris, Felmingham, Boyce and Kemp2012; Withall et al., Reference Withall, Harris and Cumming2010). These findings suggested that the melancholic MDD showed a profile of widespread cognitive impairments relative to healthy controls, and that are independent of the severity of symptoms (Linden, Jackson, Subramanian, Healy, & Linden, Reference Linden, Jackson, Subramanian, Healy and Linden2011).

Our results indicated that the melancholic MDD patients were mainly involved in memory deficit, in line with other studies (Bosaipo et al., Reference Bosaipo, Foss, Young and Juruena2017; Zaninotto et al., Reference Zaninotto, Solmi, Veronese, Guglielmo, Ioime, Camardese and Serretti2016). And the effect size of these memory domains was medium to large (Cohen's d range: 0.72 to 1.05) confirmed similar findings in previous studies (Austin et al., Reference Austin, Mitchell, Wilhelm, Parker, Hickie, Brodaty and Hadzi-Pavlovic1999; Withall et al., Reference Withall, Harris and Cumming2010). Verbal learning was assessed by the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT) also defined as verbal memory. Similarly, a previous study found worse performance in tests measuring verbal working memory with melancholic MDD compared with healthy controls (Austin, Mitchell, & Goodwin, Reference Austin, Mitchell and Goodwin2001). Meanwhile, in the domains of visual-spatial memory, and verbal working memory, melancholic depressives also performed significantly worse than healthy controls (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Xu, Lu, Ouyang, Dang, Lorenzo-Seva and Lee2014). Moreover, Linden and colleagues found an emotional bias on working memory performance in the melancholic depression group (Linden et al., Reference Linden, Jackson, Subramanian, Healy and Linden2011). According to the cognitive theories (Mathews & MacLeod, Reference Mathews and MacLeod2005; Ridout, Astell, Reid, Glen, & O'Carroll, Reference Ridout, Astell, Reid, Glen and O'Carroll2003), the patients with melancholic depression posit a bias for negative or sad information, the capacity-limited memory system was full of unrelated negative emotional materials, resulting in the brain dysfunction in shifting processing, updating and inhibiting, and thus damaged the working memory. After remission from depression, melancholic depression patients could recover their visual-spatial memory and verbal working memory function to the level of healthy controls (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Xu, Lu, Ouyang, Dang, Lorenzo-Seva and Lee2014). Indeed, our results also revealed a negative correlation between verbal learning and 24-HDRS scores. Therefore, changing the sad bias in working memory may become a potential direction or measure for future targeted treatment and psychological interventions.

Correlations between abnormal dFC variability values and cognitive deficits

In our present study, the decreased dFC variability values between the left rostral hippocampus and left anterior lobe of the cerebellum was positively correlated with working memory deficit in patients with melancholic MDD. Previous numerous fMRI and positron emission tomography (PET) studies have been reliably demonstrated that cerebellar engagement in working memory tasks (Beneventi, Barndon, Ersland, & Hugdahl, Reference Beneventi, Barndon, Ersland and Hugdahl2007; Guell, Gabrieli, & Schmahmann, Reference Guell, Gabrieli and Schmahmann2018; Hautzel, Mottaghy, Specht, Muller, & Krause, Reference Hautzel, Mottaghy, Specht, Muller and Krause2009; Hayter, Langdon, & Ramnani, Reference Hayter, Langdon and Ramnani2007; Ng et al., Reference Ng, Kao, Chan, Chew, Chuang and Chen2016). Of note, the rostral hippocampus is closely linked to general memory processes including both learning, memory, encoding and retrieval (Carr et al., Reference Carr, Rissman and Wagner2010; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Barron, Kirby, Bottenhorn, Hill, Murphy and Fox2015; Zeidman & Maguire, Reference Zeidman and Maguire2016). And accumulating evidence suggested that functionally intact cerebellar-hippocampal interactions underlie spatial memory processing and coding (McNaughton, Battaglia, Jensen, Moser, & Moser, Reference McNaughton, Battaglia, Jensen, Moser and Moser2006). Importantly, the persistent neural activity in the hippocampus is critical for working memory processing (Boran et al., Reference Boran, Fedele, Klaver, Hilfiker, Stieglitz, Grunwald and Sarnthein2019; Yonelinas, Reference Yonelinas2013). And working memory processing also relies on persistent neural activity in a widespread neural network of brain areas (Boran et al., Reference Boran, Fedele, Klaver, Hilfiker, Stieglitz, Grunwald and Sarnthein2019; Kim, Reference Kim2019). In the human neural network, human brain connectivity is dynamic and associated with ongoing rhythmic activity over time and these dynamic properties provide high-level flexibility in cognitive function processing (Dehaene et al., Reference Dehaene, Naccache, Cohen, Bihan, Mangin, Poline and Riviere2001; Yu & Dayan, Reference Yu and Dayan2005). Recent studies have also indicated that the dFC are indeed related to cognitive function, and may supersede traditional neuroimaging measures in explaining cognitive variance (Douw, Wakeman, Tanaka, Liu, & Stufflebeam, Reference Douw, Wakeman, Tanaka, Liu and Stufflebeam2016; Hellyer, Jachs, Clopath, & Leech, Reference Hellyer, Jachs, Clopath and Leech2016). In addition, two recent studies on the multiple sclerosis brain discovered that the disrupted cerebellar dFC was related to worse working memory and processing speed (Schoonheim et al., Reference Schoonheim, Douw, Broeders, Eijlers, Meijer and Geurts2021), and the lower (both left and right) hippocampus dFC was also correlated with memory dysfunction (van Geest et al., Reference van Geest, Hulst, Meijer, Hoyng, Geurts and Douw2018). Furthermore, the decreased FC between the left hippocampus and the left anterior cerebellum were well as correlated with cognitive dysfunction in patients with obstructive sleep apnea (Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Liu, Luo, Li, Peng, Zong and Ouyang2020). Whereas, the higher levels of brain dynamics are an important indicator for better cognitive performance in healthy subjects, such as working memory, cognitive flexibility, executive function and processing speed (Braun et al., Reference Braun, Schafer, Walter, Erk, Romanczuk-Seiferth, Haddad and Bassett2015; Cole et al., Reference Cole, Reynolds, Power, Repovs, Anticevic and Braver2013; Douw et al., Reference Douw, Wakeman, Tanaka, Liu and Stufflebeam2016; McIntosh, Kovacevic, & Itier, Reference McIntosh, Kovacevic and Itier2008; Nomi et al., Reference Nomi, Vij, Dajani, Steimke, Damaraju, Rachakonda and Uddin2017). Therefore, these findings would explain the potential mechanism of decreased hippocampus-cerebellum dynamic paralleled with working memory impairment in patients with melancholic MDD.

The hippocampus and cerebellum are functionally connected in a bidirectional manner and helping to the formation of spatial memory (McNaughton et al., Reference McNaughton, Battaglia, Jensen, Moser and Moser2006; Passot, Sheynikhovich, Duvelle, & Arleo, Reference Passot, Sheynikhovich, Duvelle and Arleo2012; Rochefort, Lefort, & Rondi-Reig, Reference Rochefort, Lefort and Rondi-Reig2013). Critically, studies have demonstrated an important dependence of the cerebellum on the hippocampus that the cerebellum supplied by the hippocampus in the coding of time and, declarative and episodic memory (Burgess, Maguire, & O'Keefe, Reference Burgess, Maguire and O'Keefe2002; Eichenbaum, Reference Eichenbaum2014; Zeidler, Hoffmann, & Krook-Magnuson, Reference Zeidler, Hoffmann and Krook-Magnuson2020). Evidence from rats' experiments indicated a crucial role for the cerebellum in hippocampus-dependent spatial memory (Netrakanti et al., Reference Netrakanti, Cooper, Dere, Poggi, Winkler, Brose and Ehrenreich2015). Meanwhile, Bohne and colleagues also confirmed the connectivity map between the hippocampus and cerebellum in mice and strengthen the notion of the cerebellum's involvement in cognitive functions (Bohne, Schwarz, Herlitze, & Mark, Reference Bohne, Schwarz, Herlitze and Mark2019). The left hippocampus and cerebellar interact during the prediction of spatio-temporal aspects of voluntary movements which are related more closely to spatial cognition (Onuki et al., Reference Onuki, Van Someren, De Zeeuw and Van der Werf2015; Stoodley, Valera, & Schmahmann, Reference Stoodley, Valera and Schmahmann2012). Therefore, despite the cerebellum's hypothesized role in working memory, without hippocampal support, the cerebellum appears unable to keep information about the conditioned stimulus ‘on-line’ (Kuper et al., Reference Kuper, Kaschani, Thurling, Stefanescu, Burciu, Goricke and Timmann2016; McNaughton et al., Reference McNaughton, Battaglia, Jensen, Moser and Moser2006) and thus disrupts working memory (Zeidler et al., Reference Zeidler, Hoffmann and Krook-Magnuson2020).

Finally, our results also found the melancholic MDD patients performed worse social cognitive performance compared to healthy participants. Recent evidence indicated that both the acute and remission stage of MDD illness exhibited social cognitive deficits (Knight & Baune, Reference Knight and Baune2019; LeMoult, Joormann, Sherdell, Wright, & Gotlib, Reference LeMoult, Joormann, Sherdell, Wright and Gotlib2009), which also contribute to functional deficits in occupational functioning, interpersonal relationships, and self-perceived quality of life (Weightman, Air, & Baune, Reference Weightman, Air and Baune2014). Moreover, the effect size of social cognition impairment was large (Cohen's d = 1.23) in melancholic MDD patients, suggesting that acute social cognitive deficits may be greater in currently melancholic depressed individuals. This aspect of social cognition should be considered a prime target in adjunctive cognitive and psychosocial treatments (Knight & Baune, Reference Knight and Baune2017; McIntyre & Lee, Reference McIntyre and Lee2016), achieving the goal of full functional recovery.

Limitations

However, some limitations to the present study should also be considered. First, this study was designed as a cross-sectional study, the progressive changes did not be observed. Second, there is no comparison of intelligence quotient (IQ) differences between the two groups. And it would be helpful that take the premorbid IQ into account when assessing the differences in cognitive performance between the two groups in future studies. Third, we compared these cognitive and dFC variability differences between the melancholic MDD patients and healthy controls only, but no non-melancholic MDD patients were included. A previous study found that the atypical MDD patients exhibited significantly decreased dynamic FC of the cerebellar subregions connecting with the superior temporal gyrus, dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex, ventral medial prefrontal cortex and visual area (Zhu et al., Reference Zhu, Yang, Zhang, Wang, Wang, Zhang and Zhu2020). Another study suggested that melancholic depression exhibited decreased effective connectivity between the right frontoparietal and insula networks compared with no-melancholic depression (Hyett, Breakspear, Friston, Guo, & Parker, Reference Hyett, Breakspear, Friston, Guo and Parker2015). Consequently, a direct comparison of dynamic connectivity between melancholic and typical MDD using a larger homogeneous sample is encouraged and the present findings might not apply to other MDD subtypes. Furthermore, previous studies reported that melancholic depressed patients may demonstrate different serum concentrations of inflammatory cytokine and cortisol in comparison with non-melancholic features (Kaestner et al., Reference Kaestner, Hettich, Peters, Sibrowski, Hetzel, Ponath and Rothermundt2005; Karlovic, Serretti, Vrkic, Martinac, & Marcinko, Reference Karlovic, Serretti, Vrkic, Martinac and Marcinko2012; Primo de Carvalho Alves & Sica da Rocha, Reference Primo de Carvalho Alves and Sica da Rocha2020). Accumulating evidence suggests that the cortisol levels, thyroid hormones and inflammatory cytokine levels may be associated with the functional connectivity of depressed-related brain regions (Felger et al., Reference Felger, Li, Haroon, Woolwine, Jung, Hu and Miller2016; Peters et al., Reference Peters, Jenkins, Stange, Bessette, Skerrett, Kling and Langenecker2019; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Chen, Zhong, Jia, Xia, Lai and Liu2018), and visuospatial memory (Hakamata et al., Reference Hakamata, Komi, Sato, Izawa, Mizukami, Moriguchi and Tagaya2019). However, it remains unclear how hippocampal connectivity is involved in the relationship between cortisol and visuospatial memory in melancholic MDD. Next, we can also explore the underlying complex interactions between these blood biomarkers and the brain functional abnormalities in patients with melancholic MDD.

Conclusions

In summary, our results indicate that the decreased dFC variability values between the left rostral hippocampus and left anterior lobe of the cerebellum may signify an underlying neural substrate of working memory impairment in melancholic MDD. And mapping subregional hippocampal abnormalities and their cognitive correlates may provide a potential direction for future interventions of this MDD subtype.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721004906.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the patients, volunteers, and their families whose participation made this work possible.

Financial support

Funding for this work was provided by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No: 81801347; 81971597; 81671351; 81671670 and 82102003), Planned Science and Technology Project of Guangdong Province, China (No: 2017B020227011), National Key Research and Development Program of China (2020YFC2005700), Project in Basic Research and Applied Basic Research in General Colleges and Universities of Guangdong, China (2018KZDXM009) and Natural Science Foundation of Guangdong Province, China (No: 2021A1515011034). The founders have not played any roles in study design, data collection, analysis, manuscript writing and decision to publish.

Author contributions

Yanbin Jia and Ying Wang designed the trial and prepared the manuscript. Lai Shunkai and Ting Su contributed equally to this work, and are the first co-authors. Yanbin Jia, Shunkai Lai, Shuming Zhong, Ying Wang, Ting Su, Yiliang Zhang, Hui Zhao, Guanmao Chen, Pan Chen, Guixian Tang, Zhangzhang Qi, Jiali He, Yunxia Zhu, Sihui Lv, Zijing Song, Haofei Miao, Yilei Hu, and Hanglin Ran acquired the data. Shunkai Lai and Ting Su carried out the statistical analyses, drafted the initial manuscript. All authors interpreted the data, revised the paper critically for important intellectual content, approved the final version, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Conflict of interest

Each author has declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the study presented here.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Footnotes

*

Lai Shunkai and Ting Su contributed equally to this work.

References

Allen, E. A., Damaraju, E., Plis, S. M., Erhardt, E. B., Eichele, T., & Calhoun, V. D. (2014). Tracking whole-brain connectivity dynamics in the resting state. Cerebral Cortex, 24(3), 663676. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs352CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anacker, C., & Hen, R. (2017). Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and cognitive flexibility – linking memory and mood. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(6), 335346. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.45CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Austin, M. P., Mitchell, P., & Goodwin, G. M. (2001). Cognitive deficits in depression: Possible implications for functional neuropathology. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 200206. doi: 10.1192/bjp.178.3.200CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Austin, M. P., Mitchell, P., Wilhelm, K., Parker, G., Hickie, I., Brodaty, H., … Hadzi-Pavlovic, D. (1999). Cognitive function in depression: A distinct pattern of frontal impairment in melancholia? Psychological Medicine, 29(1), 7385. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798007788CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bai, F., Xie, C., Watson, D. R., Shi, Y., Yuan, Y., Wang, Y., … Zhang, Z. (2011). Aberrant hippocampal subregion networks associated with the classifications of aMCI subjects: A longitudinal resting-state study. PLoS One, 6(12), e29288. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029288CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ballmaier, M., Narr, K. L., Toga, A. W., Elderkin-Thompson, V., Thompson, P. M., Hamilton, L., … Kumar, A. (2008). Hippocampal morphology and distinguishing late-onset from early-onset elderly depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(2), 229237. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030506CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beneventi, H., Barndon, R., Ersland, L., & Hugdahl, K. (2007). An fMRI study of working memory for schematic facial expressions. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 48(2), 8186. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2007.00536.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohne, P., Schwarz, M. K., Herlitze, S., & Mark, M. D. (2019). A new projection from the deep cerebellar nuclei to the hippocampus via the ventrolateral and laterodorsal thalamus in mice. Frontiers in Neural Circuits, 13, 51. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2019.00051CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boran, E., Fedele, T., Klaver, P., Hilfiker, P., Stieglitz, L., Grunwald, T., & Sarnthein, J. (2019). Persistent hippocampal neural firing and hippocampal-cortical coupling predict verbal working memory load. Science Advances, 5(3), eaav3687. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aav3687CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosaipo, N. B., Foss, M. P., Young, A. H., & Juruena, M. F. (2017). Neuropsychological changes in melancholic and atypical depression: A systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 73, 309325. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braun, U., Schafer, A., Walter, H., Erk, S., Romanczuk-Seiferth, N., Haddad, L., … Bassett, D. S. (2015). Dynamic reconfiguration of frontal brain networks during executive cognition in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(37), 1167811683. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1422487112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bray, S., Arnold, A. E., Levy, R. M., & Iaria, G. (2015). Spatial and temporal functional connectivity changes between resting and attentive states. Human Brain Mapping, 36(2), 549565. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22646CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, J. D., Vythilingam, M., Vermetten, E., Vaccarino, V., & Charney, D. S. (2004). Deficits in hippocampal and anterior cingulate functioning during verbal declarative memory encoding in midlife major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(4), 637645. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.4.637CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgess, N., Maguire, E. A., & O'Keefe, J. (2002). The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron, 35(4), 625641. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00830-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldieraro, M. A., Baeza, F. L., Pinheiro, D. O., Ribeiro, M. R., Parker, G., & Fleck, M. P. (2013). Clinical differences between melancholic and nonmelancholic depression as defined by the CORE system. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54(1), 1115. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.05.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cao, X., Liu, Z., Xu, C., Li, J., Gao, Q., Sun, N., … Zhang, K. (2012). Disrupted resting-state functional connectivity of the hippocampus in medication-naive patients with major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 141(2–3), 194203. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.03.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardoner, N., Soria, V., Gratacos, M., Hernandez-Ribas, R., Pujol, J., Lopez-Sola, M., … Soriano-Mas, C. (2013). Val66Met BDNF genotypes in melancholic depression: Effects on brain structure and treatment outcome. Depression and Anxiety, 30(3), 225233. doi: 10.1002/da.22025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, V. A., Rissman, J., & Wagner, A. D. (2010). Imaging the human medial temporal lobe with high-resolution fMRI. Neuron, 65(3), 298308. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J., Wei, Z., Han, H., Jin, L., Xu, C., Dong, D., … Peng, Z. (2019). An effect of chronic stress on prospective memory via alteration of resting-state hippocampal subregion functional connectivity. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 19698. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-56111-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, M. W., Reynolds, J. R., Power, J. D., Repovs, G., Anticevic, A., & Braver, T. S. (2013). Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nature Neuroscience, 16(9), 13481355. doi: 10.1038/nn.3470CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, P. Y., Patel, V., Joestl, S. S., March, D., Insel, T. R., Daar, A. S., … Stein, D. J. (2011). Grand challenges in global mental health. Nature, 475(7354), 2730. doi: 10.1038/475027aCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Day, C. V., Gatt, J. M., Etkin, A., DeBattista, C., Schatzberg, A. F., & Williams, L. M. (2015). Cognitive and emotional biomarkers of melancholic depression: An iSPOT-D report. Journal of Affective Disorders, 176, 141150. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.01.061CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Bihan, D. L., Mangin, J. F., Poline, J. B., & Riviere, D. (2001). Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 4(7), 752758. doi: 10.1038/89551CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demirtas, M., Tornador, C., Falcon, C., Lopez-Sola, M., Hernandez-Ribas, R., Pujol, J., … Deco, G. (2016). Dynamic functional connectivity reveals altered variability in functional connectivity among patients with major depressive disorder. Human Brain Mapping, 37(8), 29182930. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23215CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douw, L., Wakeman, D. G., Tanaka, N., Liu, H., & Stufflebeam, S. M. (2016). State-dependent variability of dynamic functional connectivity between frontoparietal and default networks relates to cognitive flexibility. Neuroscience, 339, 1221. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.09.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drevets, W. C. (2000). Neuroimaging studies of mood disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 48(8), 813829. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)01020-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duckworth, K. (2015). Understanding mental disorders: Your guide to DSM-5. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(9), 916916. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15070879CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duval, F., Mokrani, M. C., Monreal-Ortiz, J. A., Fattah, S., Champeval, C., Schulz, P., & Macher, J. P. (2006). Cortisol hypersecretion in unipolar major depression with melancholic and psychotic features: Dopaminergic, noradrenergic and thyroid correlates. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31(7), 876888. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.04.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eichenbaum, H. (2014). Time cells in the hippocampus: A new dimension for mapping memories. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(11), 732744. doi: 10.1038/nrn3827CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, L., Li, H., Zhuo, J., Zhang, Y., Wang, J., Chen, L., … Jiang, T. (2016). The human brainnetome atlas: A new brain atlas based on connectional architecture. Cerebral Cortex, 26(8), 35083526. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fateh, A. A., Long, Z., Duan, X., Cui, Q., Pang, Y., Farooq, M. U., … Chen, H. (2019). Hippocampal functional connectivity-based discrimination between bipolar and major depressive disorders. Psychiatry Research Neuroimaging, 284, 5360. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2019.01.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felger, J. C., Li, Z., Haroon, E., Woolwine, B. J., Jung, M. Y., Hu, X., & Miller, A. H. (2016). Inflammation is associated with decreased functional connectivity within corticostriatal reward circuitry in depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 21(10), 13581365. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.168CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figueroa, C. A., Mocking, R. J. T., van Wingen, G., Martens, S., Ruhe, H. G., & Schene, A. H. (2017). Aberrant default-mode network-hippocampus connectivity after sad memory-recall in remitted-depression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(11), 18031813. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frodl, T., Schaub, A., Banac, S., Charypar, M., Jager, M., Kummler, P., … Meisenzahl, E. M. (2006). Reduced hippocampal volume correlates with executive dysfunctioning in major depression. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 31(5), 316323.Google ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, D. L., Payne, M. E., MacFall, J. R., Steffens, D. C., & Krishnan, R. R. (2008). Hippocampal volumes and depression subtypes. Psychiatry Research, 163(2), 126132. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.12.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greicius, M. (2008). Resting-state functional connectivity in neuropsychiatric disorders. Current Opinion in Neurology, 21(4), 424430. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e328306f2c5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guell, X., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Schmahmann, J. D. (2018). Triple representation of language, working memory, social and emotion processing in the cerebellum: Convergent evidence from task and seed-based resting-state fMRI analyses in a single large cohort. Neuroimage, 172, 437449. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.082CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guo, W., Liu, F., Dai, Y., Jiang, M., Zhang, J., Yu, L., … Xiao, C. (2013). Decreased interhemispheric resting-state functional connectivity in first-episode, drug-naive major depressive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 41, 2429. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.11.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hakamata, Y., Komi, S., Sato, E., Izawa, S., Mizukami, S., Moriguchi, Y., … Tagaya, H. (2019). Cortisol-related hippocampal-extrastriate functional connectivity explains the adverse effect of cortisol on visuospatial retrieval. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 109, 104310. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.04.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, X., Wu, X., Wang, Y., Sun, Y., Ding, W., Cao, M., … Zhou, Y. (2018). Alterations of resting-state static and dynamic functional connectivity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in subjects with internet gaming disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 41. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hao, Z. Y., Zhong, Y., Ma, Z. J., Xu, H. Z., Kong, J. Y., Wu, Z., … Wang, C. (2020). Abnormal resting-state functional connectivity of hippocampal subfields in patients with major depressive disorder. BMC Psychiatry, 20(1), 71. doi: 10.1186/s12888-020-02490-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hautzel, H., Mottaghy, F. M., Specht, K., Muller, H. W., & Krause, B. J. (2009). Evidence of a modality-dependent role of the cerebellum in working memory? An fMRI study comparing verbal and abstract n-back tasks. Neuroimage, 47(4), 20732082. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayter, A. L., Langdon, D. W., & Ramnani, N. (2007). Cerebellar contributions to working memory. Neuroimage, 36(3), 943954. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hellyer, P. J., Jachs, B., Clopath, C., & Leech, R. (2016). Local inhibitory plasticity tunes macroscopic brain dynamics and allows the emergence of functional brain networks. Neuroimage, 124(Pt A), 8595. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.069CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hickie, I., Naismith, S., Ward, P. B., Turner, K., Scott, E., Mitchell, P., … Parker, G. (2005). Reduced hippocampal volumes and memory loss in patients with early- and late-onset depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 197202. doi: 10.1192/bjp.186.3.197CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, Y., Wang, Y., Wang, H., Liu, Z., Yu, X., Yan, J., … Wu, Y. (2019). Prevalence of mental disorders in China: A cross-sectional epidemiological study. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 6(3), 211224. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30511-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyett, M. P., Breakspear, M. J., Friston, K. J., Guo, C. C., & Parker, G. B. (2015). Disrupted effective connectivity of cortical systems supporting attention and interoception in melancholia. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(4), 350358. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2490CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Igloi, K., Doeller, C. F., Paradis, A. L., Benchenane, K., Berthoz, A., Burgess, N., & Rondi-Reig, L. (2015). Interaction between hippocampus and cerebellum crus I in sequence-based but not place-based navigation. Cerebral Cortex, 25(11), 41464154. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., & Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage, 17(2), 825841. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)91132-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeon, H. J., Peng, D., Chua, H. C., Srisurapanont, M., Fava, M., Bae, J. N., … Hong, J. P. (2013). Melancholic features and hostility are associated with suicidality risk in Asian patients with major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 148(2-3), 368374. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.01.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaestner, F., Hettich, M., Peters, M., Sibrowski, W., Hetzel, G., Ponath, G., … Rothermundt, M. (2005). Different activation patterns of proinflammatory cytokines in melancholic and non-melancholic major depression are associated with HPA axis activity. Journal of Affective Disorders, 87(2-3), 305311. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.03.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, R. H., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Dillon, D. G., Goer, F., Beltzer, M., Minkel, J., … Pizzagalli, D. A. (2016). Dynamic resting-state functional connectivity in major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(7), 18221830. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.352CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karlovic, D., Serretti, A., Vrkic, N., Martinac, M., & Marcinko, D. (2012). Serum concentrations of CRP, IL-6, TNF-alpha and cortisol in major depressive disorder with melancholic or atypical features. Psychiatry Research, 198(1), 7480. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khan, S. A., Ryali, V., Bhat, P. S., Prakash, J., Srivastava, K., & Khanam, S. (2015). The hippocampus and executive functions in depression. Industrial Psychiatry Journal, 24(1), 1822. doi: 10.4103/0972-6748.160920CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H. (2019). Neural activity during working memory encoding, maintenance, and retrieval: A network-based model and meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 40(17), 49124933. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24747CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knight, M. J., & Baune, B. T. (2017). Psychosocial dysfunction in major depressive disorder-rationale, design, and characteristics of the cognitive and emotional recovery training program for depression (CERT-D). Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8, 280. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00280CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knight, M. J., & Baune, B. T. (2019). Social cognitive abilities predict psychosocial dysfunction in major depressive disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 36(1), 5462. doi: 10.1002/da.22844CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kucyi, A., Hove, M. J., Esterman, M., Hutchison, R. M., & Valera, E. M. (2017). Dynamic brain network correlates of spontaneous fluctuations in attention. Cerebral Cortex, 27(3), 18311840. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw029Google ScholarPubMed
Kuper, M., Kaschani, P., Thurling, M., Stefanescu, M. R., Burciu, R. G., Goricke, S., … Timmann, D. (2016). Cerebellar fMRI activation increases with increasing working memory demands. Cerebellum, 15(3), 322335. doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0703-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamers, F., Vogelzangs, N., Merikangas, K. R., de Jonge, P., Beekman, A. T., & Penninx, B. W. (2013). Evidence for a differential role of HPA-axis function, inflammation and metabolic syndrome in melancholic versus atypical depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 18(6), 692699. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.144CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarov, A., Zhu, X., Suarez-Jimenez, B., Rutherford, B. R., & Neria, Y. (2017). Resting-state functional connectivity of anterior and posterior hippocampus in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 94, 1522. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.06.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeMoult, J., Joormann, J., Sherdell, L., Wright, Y., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Identification of emotional facial expressions following recovery from depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(4), 828833. doi: 10.1037/a0016944CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonardi, N., & Van De Ville, D. (2015). On spurious and real fluctuations of dynamic functional connectivity during rest. NeuroImage, 104, 430436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, J., Duan, X., Cui, Q., Chen, H., & Liao, W. (2019). More than just statics: Temporal dynamics of intrinsic brain activity predicts the suicidal ideation in depressed patients. Psychological Medicine, 49(5), 852860. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718001502CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liang, S., Yu, W., Ma, X., Luo, S., Zhang, J., Sun, X., … Zhang, Y. (2020). Psychometric properties of the MATRICS consensus cognitive battery (MCCB) in Chinese patients with major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 265, 132138. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.01.052CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liao, W., Li, J., Duan, X., Cui, Q., Chen, H., & Chen, H. (2018). Static and dynamic connectomics differentiate between depressed patients with and without suicidal ideation. Human Brain Mapping, 39(10), 41054118. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24235CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liao, W., Wu, G. R., Xu, Q., Ji, G. J., Zhang, Z., Zang, Y. F., & Lu, G. (2014). DynamicBC: A MATLAB toolbox for dynamic brain connectome analysis. Brain Connectivity, 4(10), 780790. doi: 10.1089/brain.2014.0253CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, K., Xu, G., Lu, W., Ouyang, H., Dang, Y., Lorenzo-Seva, U., … Lee, T. M. (2014). Neuropsychological performance in melancholic, atypical and undifferentiated major depression during depressed and remitted states: A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 168, 184191. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.06.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linden, S. C., Jackson, M. C., Subramanian, L., Healy, D., & Linden, D. E. (2011). Sad benefit in face working memory: An emotional bias of melancholic depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 135(1-3), 251257. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.08.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Long, Y., Cao, H., Yan, C., Chen, X., Li, L., Castellanos, F. X., … Liu, Z. (2020). Altered resting-state dynamic functional brain networks in major depressive disorder: Findings from the REST-meta-MDD consortium. Neuroimage Clinical, 26, 102163. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102163CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malivoire, B. L., Girard, T. A., Patel, R., & Monson, C. M. (2018). Functional connectivity of hippocampal subregions in PTSD: Relations with symptoms. BMC Psychiatry, 18(1), 129. doi: 10.1186/s12888-018-1716-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 167195. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143916CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, A. R., Kovacevic, N., & Itier, R. J. (2008). Increased brain signal variability accompanies lower behavioral variability in development. PLOS Computational Biology, 4(7), e1000106. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntyre, R. S., & Lee, Y. (2016). Cognition in major depressive disorder: A ‘systemically important functional index’ (SIFI). Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 29(1), 4855. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000221CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNaughton, B. L., Battaglia, F. P., Jensen, O., Moser, E. I., & Moser, M. B. (2006). Path integration and the neural basis of the ‘cognitive map’. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(8), 663678. doi: 10.1038/nrn1932CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Netrakanti, P. R., Cooper, B. H., Dere, E., Poggi, G., Winkler, D., Brose, N., & Ehrenreich, H. (2015). Fast cerebellar reflex circuitry requires synaptic vesicle priming by munc13-3. Cerebellum, 14(3), 264283. doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0645-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ng, H. B., Kao, K. L., Chan, Y. C., Chew, E., Chuang, K. H., & Chen, S. H. (2016). Modality specificity in the cerebro-cerebellar neurocircuitry during working memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 305, 164173. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.02.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nguyen, T. T., Kovacevic, S., Dev, S. I., Lu, K., Liu, T. T., & Eyler, L. T. (2017). Dynamic functional connectivity in bipolar disorder is associated with executive function and processing speed: A preliminary study. Neuropsychology, 31(1), 7383. doi: 10.1037/neu0000317CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nomi, J. S., Vij, S. G., Dajani, D. R., Steimke, R., Damaraju, E., Rachakonda, S., … Uddin, L. Q. (2017). Chronnectomic patterns and neural flexibility underlie executive function. Neuroimage, 147, 861871. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Onuki, Y., Van Someren, E. J., De Zeeuw, C. I., & Van der Werf, Y. D. (2015). Hippocampal-cerebellar interaction during spatio-temporal prediction. Cerebral Cortex, 25(2), 313321. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht221CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Reilly, J. X., Beckmann, C. F., Tomassini, V., Ramnani, N., & Johansen-Berg, H. (2010). Distinct and overlapping functional zones in the cerebellum defined by resting-state functional connectivity. Cerebral Cortex, 20(4), 953965. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pan, Z., Park, C., Brietzke, E., Zuckerman, H., Rong, C., Mansur, R. B., … McIntyre, R. S. (2019). Cognitive impairment in major depressive disorder. CNS Spectrums, 24(1), 2229. doi: 10.1017/S1092852918001207CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pang, Y., Zhang, H., Cui, Q., Yang, Q., Lu, F., Chen, H., … Chen, H. (2020). Combined static and dynamic functional connectivity signatures differentiating bipolar depression from major depressive disorder. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 54(8), 832842. doi: 10.1177/0004867420924089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Passot, J. B., Sheynikhovich, D., Duvelle, E., & Arleo, A. (2012). Contribution of cerebellar sensorimotor adaptation to hippocampal spatial memory. PLoS One, 7(4), e32560. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0032560CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patas, K., Penninx, B. W., Bus, B. A., Vogelzangs, N., Molendijk, M. L., Elzinga, B. M., … Oude Voshaar, R. C. (2014). Association between serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor and plasma interleukin-6 in major depressive disorder with melancholic features. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 36, 7179. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2013.10.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peng, W., Mao, L., Yin, D., Sun, W., Wang, H., Zhang, Q., … Wang, X. (2018). Functional network changes in the hippocampus contribute to depressive symptoms in epilepsy. Seizure, 60, 1622. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2018.06.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, A. T., Jenkins, L. M., Stange, J. P., Bessette, K. L., Skerrett, K. A., Kling, L. R., … Langenecker, S. A. (2019). Pre-scan cortisol is differentially associated with enhanced connectivity to the cognitive control network in young adults with a history of depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 104, 219227. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.03.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porcu, M., Operamolla, A., Scapin, E., Garofalo, P., Destro, F., Caneglias, A., … Saba, L. (2020). Effects of white matter hyperintensities on brain connectivity and hippocampal volume in healthy subjects according to their localization. Brain Connectivity, 10(8), 436447. doi: 10.1089/brain.2020.0774CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posener, J. A., Wang, L., Price, J. L., Gado, M. H., Province, M. A., Miller, M. I., … Csernansky, J. G. (2003). High-dimensional mapping of the hippocampus in depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(1), 8389. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.83CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Primo de Carvalho Alves, L., & Sica da Rocha, N. (2018). Lower levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor are associated with melancholic psychomotor retardation among depressed inpatients. Bipolar Disorders, 20(8), 746752. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12636CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Primo de Carvalho Alves, L., & Sica da Rocha, N. (2020). Different cytokine patterns associate with melancholia severity among inpatients with major depressive disorder. Therapeutic Advances in Psychopharmacology, 10, 2045125320937921. doi: 10.1177/2045125320937921CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quinn, C. R., Dobson-Stone, C., Outhred, T., Harris, A., & Kemp, A. H. (2012). The contribution of BDNF and 5-HTT polymorphisms and early life stress to the heterogeneity of major depressive disorder: A preliminary study. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 46(1), 5563. doi: 10.1177/0004867411430878CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quinn, C. R., Harris, A., Felmingham, K., Boyce, P., & Kemp, A. (2012). The impact of depression heterogeneity on cognitive control in major depressive disorder. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 46(11), 10791088. doi: 10.1177/0004867412461383CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reinen, J. M., Chen, O. Y., Hutchison, R. M., Yeo, B. T. T., Anderson, K. M., Sabuncu, M. R., … Holmes, A. J. (2018). The human cortex possesses a reconfigurable dynamic network architecture that is disrupted in psychosis. Nature Communications, 9(1), 1157. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03462-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reshetnikov, V. V., Kovner, A. V., Lepeshko, A. A., Pavlov, K. S., Grinkevich, L. N., & Bondar, N. P. (2020). Stress early in life leads to cognitive impairments, reduced numbers of CA3 neurons and altered maternal behavior in adult female mice. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 19(3), e12541. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12541CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ridout, N., Astell, A., Reid, I., Glen, T., & O'Carroll, R. (2003). Memory bias for emotional facial expressions in major depression. Cognition and Emotion, 17(1), 101122. doi: 10.1080/02699930302272CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rive, M. M., van Rooijen, G., Veltman, D. J., Phillips, M. L., Schene, A. H., & Ruhe, H. G. (2013). Neural correlates of dysfunctional emotion regulation in major depressive disorder. A systematic review of neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10 Pt 2), 25292553. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, J. L., Barron, D. S., Kirby, L. A., Bottenhorn, K. L., Hill, A. C., Murphy, J. E., … Fox, P. T. (2015). Neurofunctional topography of the human hippocampus. Human Brain Mapping, 36(12), 50185037. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22987CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roca, M., Monzon, S., Vives, M., Lopez-Navarro, E., Garcia-Toro, M., Vicens, C., … Gili, M. (2015). Cognitive function after clinical remission in patients with melancholic and non-melancholic depression: A 6 month follow-up study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 171, 8592. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.09.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rochefort, C., Lefort, J. M., & Rondi-Reig, L. (2013). The cerebellum: A new key structure in the navigation system. Frontiers in Neural Circuits, 7, 35. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rusch, B. D., Abercrombie, H. C., Oakes, T. R., Schaefer, S. M., & Davidson, R. J. (2001). Hippocampal morphometry in depressed patients and control subjects: Relations to anxiety symptoms. Biological Psychiatry, 50(12), 960964. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01248-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saleh, A., Potter, G. G., McQuoid, D. R., Boyd, B., Turner, R., MacFall, J. R., & Taylor, W. D. (2017). Effects of early life stress on depression, cognitive performance and brain morphology. Psychological Medicine, 47(1), 171181. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716002403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoonheim, M. M., Douw, L., Broeders, T. A., Eijlers, A. J., Meijer, K. A., & Geurts, J. J. (2021). The cerebellum and its network: Disrupted static and dynamic functional connectivity patterns and cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Multiple Sclerosis, 27(13), 20312039. doi: 10.1177/1352458521999274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shan, X., Cui, X., Liu, F., Li, H., Huang, R., Tang, Y., … Xie, G. (2021). Shared and distinct homotopic connectivity changes in melancholic and non-melancholic depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 287, 268275. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.03.038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, C., Kang, L., Yao, S., Ma, Y., Li, T., Liang, Y., … Yu, X. (2015). The MATRICS consensus cognitive battery (MCCB): Co-norming and standardization in China. Schizophrenia Research, 169(1-3), 109115. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.09.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shirer, W. R., Ryali, S., Rykhlevskaia, E., Menon, V., & Greicius, M. D. (2012). Decoding subject-driven cognitive states with whole-brain connectivity patterns. Cerebral Cortex, 22(1), 158165. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr099CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Small, S. A., Schobel, S. A., Buxton, R. B., Witter, M. P., & Barnes, C. A. (2011). A pathophysiological framework of hippocampal dysfunction in ageing and disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(10), 585601. doi: 10.1038/nrn3085CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soriano-Mas, C., Hernandez-Ribas, R., Pujol, J., Urretavizcaya, M., Deus, J., Harrison, B. J., … Cardoner, N. (2011). Cross-sectional and longitudinal assessment of structural brain alterations in melancholic depression. Biological Psychiatry, 69(4), 318325. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoodley, C. J., Valera, E. M., & Schmahmann, J. D. (2012). Functional topography of the cerebellum for motor and cognitive tasks: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 59(2), 15601570. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.065CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suarez-Jimenez, B., Zhu, X., Lazarov, A., Mann, J. J., Schneier, F., Gerber, A., … Markowitz, J. C. (2020). Anterior hippocampal volume predicts affect-focused psychotherapy outcome. Psychological Medicine, 50(3), 396402. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719000187CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H., Anderson, C. M., & Polcari, A. (2012). Childhood maltreatment is associated with reduced volume in the hippocampal subfields CA3, dentate gyrus, and subiculum. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(9), E563E572. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1115396109Google ScholarPubMed
Therriault, J., Wang, S., Mathotaarachchi, S., Pascoal, T. A., Parent, M., & Beaudry, T.Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging, I. (2019). Rostral-caudal hippocampal functional convergence is reduced across the Alzheimer's disease spectrum. Molecular Neurobiology, 56(12), 83368344. doi: 10.1007/s12035-019-01671-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tondo, L., Vazquez, G. H., & Baldessarini, R. J. (2020). Melancholic versus nonmelancholic major depression compared. Journal of Affective Disorders, 266, 760765. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.01.139CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Geest, Q., Hulst, H. E., Meijer, K. A., Hoyng, L., Geurts, J. J. G., & Douw, L. (2018). The importance of hippocampal dynamic connectivity in explaining memory function in multiple sclerosis. Brain and Behavior, 8(5), e00954. doi: 10.1002/brb3.954CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vasilopoulou, K., Papathanasiou, P., Michopoulos, J., Boufidou, F., Oulis, P., Nikolaou, C., … Lykouras, L. (2011). [A volumetric study of brain structures in subtypes of depression]. Psychiatrike = Psychiatriki, 22(2), 120131.Google ScholarPubMed
Vassilopoulou, K., Papathanasiou, M., Michopoulos, I., Boufidou, F., Oulis, P., Kelekis, N., … Lykouras, L. (2013). A magnetic resonance imaging study of hippocampal, amygdala and subgenual prefrontal cortex volumes in major depression subtypes: Melancholic versus psychotic depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 146(2), 197204. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.09.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, J., Wang, Y., Huang, H., Jia, Y., Zheng, S., Zhong, S., … Huang, R. (2020). Abnormal dynamic functional network connectivity in unmedicated bipolar and major depressive disorders based on the triple-network model. Psychological Medicine, 50(3), 465474. doi: 10.1017/S003329171900028XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., Chen, G., Zhong, S., Jia, Y., Xia, L., Lai, S., … Liu, T. (2018). Association between resting-state brain functional connectivity and cortisol levels in unmedicated major depressive disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 105, 5562. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.08.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Z., Yuan, Y., Bai, F., Shu, H., You, J., Li, L., & Zhang, Z. (2015). Altered functional connectivity networks of hippocampal subregions in remitted late-onset depression: A longitudinal resting-state study. Neuroscience Bulletin, 31(1), 1321. doi: 10.1007/s12264-014-1489-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, T. C., Obiang, P., Torres-Herraez, A., Watilliaux, A., Coulon, P., Rochefort, C., … Rondi-Reig, L. (2019). Anatomical and physiological foundations of cerebello-hippocampal interaction. Elife, 8, e41896. doi: 10.7554/eLife.41896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weightman, M. J., Air, T. M., & Baune, B. T. (2014). A review of the role of social cognition in major depressive disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 179. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wise, T., Marwood, L., Perkins, A. M., Herane-Vives, A., Joules, R., Lythgoe, D. J., … Arnone, D. (2017). Instability of default mode network connectivity in major depression: A two-sample confirmation study. Translational Psychiatry, 7(4), e1105. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.40CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Withall, A., Harris, L. M., & Cumming, S. R. (2010). A longitudinal study of cognitive function in melancholic and non-melancholic subtypes of major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 123(1-3), 150157. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.07.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woo, Y. S., Rosenblat, J. D., Kakar, R., Bahk, W. M., & McIntyre, R. S. (2016). Cognitive deficits as a mediator of poor occupational function in remitted major depressive disorder patients. Clinical Psychopharmacology and Neuroscience, 14(1), 116. doi: 10.9758/cpn.2016.14.1.1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, L. Y., Xu, F. C., Liu, C., Ji, Y. F., Wu, J. M., Wang, Y., … Yu, Y. Q. (2017). Relationship between cerebellar structure and emotional memory in depression. Brain and Behavior, 7(7), e00738. doi: 10.1002/brb3.738CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yan, C. G., Wang, X. D., Zuo, X. N., & Zang, Y. F. (2016). DPABI: Data processing & analysis for (resting-state) brain imaging. Neuroinformatics, 14(3), 339351. doi: 10.1007/s12021-016-9299-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yonelinas, A. P. (2013). The hippocampus supports high-resolution binding in the service of perception, working memory and long-term memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 254, 3444. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.05.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, A. J., & Dayan, P. (2005). Uncertainty, neuromodulation, and attention. Neuron, 46(4), 681692. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaninotto, L., Solmi, M., Veronese, N., Guglielmo, R., Ioime, L., Camardese, G., & Serretti, A. (2016). A meta-analysis of cognitive performance in melancholic versus non-melancholic unipolar depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 201, 1524. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.04.039CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeidler, Z., Hoffmann, K., & Krook-Magnuson, E. (2020). HippoBellum: Acute cerebellar modulation alters hippocampal dynamics and function. Journal of Neuroscience, 40(36), 69106926. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0763-20.2020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeidman, P., & Maguire, E. A. (2016). Anterior hippocampus: The anatomy of perception, imagination and episodic memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(3), 173182. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2015.24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, J., Cheng, W., Liu, Z., Zhang, K., Lei, X., Yao, Y., … Feng, J. (2016). Neural, electrophysiological and anatomical basis of brain-network variability and its characteristic changes in mental disorders. Brain, 139(Pt 8), 23072321. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww143CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, L., Liu, G., Luo, H., Li, H., Peng, Y., Zong, D., & Ouyang, R. (2020). Aberrant hippocampal network connectivity is associated with neurocognitive dysfunction in patients With moderate and severe obstructive sleep apnea. Frontiers in Neurology, 11, 580408. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.580408CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, D. M., Yang, Y., Zhang, Y., Wang, C., Wang, Y., Zhang, C., … Zhu, J. (2020). Cerebellar-cerebral dynamic functional connectivity alterations in major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 275, 319328. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.06.062CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Four seeds of the hippocampus in the bilateral hemisphere; L (R), left (right) hemisphere.

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographic and clinical data of participants

Figure 2

Fig. 2. dFC patterns of the bilateral rostral hippocampus (rHipp) and the bilateral caudal hippocampus (cHipp) in melancholic MDD patients and HCs (p < 0.05, uncorrected). The color bar represents a dynamic functional connection. dFC, dynamic functional connectivity; MDD, major depressive disorder; HCs, healthy controls.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Significant dFC differences between the two groups for hippocampus seed, respectively (voxel p < 0.005, cluster p < 0.0125, GRF corrected). The color bar indicates the t values from the two-sample t test analysis. dFC, dynamic functional connectivity; rHipp, the rostral hippocampus; GRF, Gaussian random field; L (R), left (right) hemisphere.

Figure 4

Table 2. The areas of significantly different dFC between the melancholic MDD patients and the HCs (voxel p < 0.005, cluster p < 0.0125, GRF corrected)

Figure 5

Fig. 4. Positive correlation between the abnormal dFC variability values and working memory T-score (r = 0.338, p = 0.029). dFC, dynamic functional connectivity; rHipp, the rostral hippocampus; L (R), left (right) hemisphere.

Supplementary material: File

Shunkai et al. supplementary material

Shunkai et al. supplementary material

Download Shunkai et al. supplementary material(File)
File 537.3 KB