Introduction
Decline in cognitive state is a central feature of ageing, and severe deterioration in cognitive function has frequently been associated with poorer quality of life and worse performance on physical tasks (Tabbarah et al., Reference Tabbarah, Crimmins and Seeman2002). Accelerated decline in cognitive state also has an influential and adverse impact upon the psychological, social, emotional and financial status of the individual, which can subsequently contribute to heightened levels of burden and distress (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson2007). Cognitive symptoms are common in affective disorders, particularly impairments in memory, executive control, feedback sensitivity and affective processing (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Chamberlain and Sahakian2009). These cognitive symptoms are associated with pathophysiology across a distributed neural circuit, which is made up of various regions across the prefrontal cortex, as well as subcortical regions and also temporal lobe structures (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Chamberlain and Sahakian2009). Both affective disorders, such as depression and anxiety, and poor cognitive function are common in older adulthood (Rovner et al., Reference Rovner1989; Alexopoulos and Abrams, Reference Alexopoulos and Abrams1991). It is estimated that after age 70, the combination of low mood and poor cognition doubles with every 5 years. By age 85, around one in four of individual experience both these comorbid conditions (Arve et al., Reference Arve1999). Due to the high prevalence of these conditions in older adulthood, this is a research area of clinical relevance and importance.
Previous research has proposed that affective problems, such as depression and anxiety, may be associated with accelerated cognitive ageing (da Silva et al., Reference Da Silva2013; Gulpers et al., Reference Gulpers2016). However, there are significant gaps in our understanding of this link. For instance, the precise temporal order of the association between affective problems and decline in cognitive state is currently unclear. It is possible that affective problems may act as an early risk factor for decline in cognitive state, or alternatively that affective problems may be a prodromal symptom of oncoming cognitive impairment. Additionally, previous studies, including several meta-analyses, have been largely diagnosis driven, with a primary focus on dementia as an outcome (Jorm, Reference Jorm2001; Ownby et al., Reference Ownby2006; Byers and Yaffe, Reference Byers and Yaffe2011; da Silva et al., Reference Da Silva2013; Bennett and Thomas, Reference Bennett and Thomas2014; Cherbuin et al., Reference Cherbuin, Kim and Anstey2015). Less is known about the impact of affective problems on decline in cognitive state across the entire population spectrum. The focus on the transition to dementia as an outcome may be problematic, as it is now believed that there is a long pre-clinical period of several decades before cognitive impairment becomes evident (Morris, Reference Morris2005). It is possible that participants who transition to dementia at follow-up assessment may have already developed substantial cerebral pathology by the time of baseline assessment, even if they were not yet presented with any cognitive symptoms. In this case, associations between affective disorders and development of dementia may be the result of reverse causality. The present study focuses on the association between affective disorders and decline in cognitive state in healthy older adults in order to minimise effects of possible reverse causality.
Cognitive state refers to a composite measure of overall cognitive function. It has been studied extensively in previous research (Nordin et al., Reference Nordin, Rosendahl and Lundin-Olsson2006; Kavé et al., Reference Kavé2008; Sohrabi et al., Reference Sohrabi2008; Esslinger et al., Reference Esslinger2011), using assessments of overall cognitive status, such as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) or composite assessments of multiple cognitive domains (e.g. memory, information processing speed, executive function). Therefore, decline in cognitive state is defined in the present review as a decline in overall cognitive function, rather than decline in specific cognitive domains. There is evidence from longitudinal research that low scores on cognitive state tests may predict onset of functional impairment (Moritz et al., Reference Moritz, Kasl and Berkman1995; Gill et al., Reference Gill1996), and functional dependence over time (Agüero-Torres et al., Reference Agüero-Torres2002; Gill et al., Reference Gill, Hardy and Williams2002; Wang et al., Reference Wang2002). For this reason, it is important to investigate how affective problems influence decline in cognitive state over time.
There are large individual differences in the extent of cognitive decline experienced by healthy older adults; however, the decline in cognitive state occurs at a steady and gradual rate over time. On average there is a decline of around 1–2 standard deviations in fluid cognition from age 20 to 70, after which average decline increases to around 0.5 s.d. every 10 years (Anstey and Low, Reference Anstey and Low2004). This stable decline is often maintained over time until symptoms of dementia begin to manifest, at which point a sharper decline in cognitive state may be observed (Rubin et al., Reference Rubin1998). As such, studies in which substantial cognitive decline is apparent within a short time frame of under 1 year may be more indicative of pathological ageing (e.g. oncoming dementia), rather than healthy ageing. Since the present study aims to examine the longitudinal association between affective disorders and decline in cognitive state in cognitively healthy individuals, it includes only longitudinal studies with sufficient time between baseline and follow-up assessments (i.e., minimum 1 year) for a substantial decline to occur within these populations. There are several studies that have investigated the association between affective problems and decline in cognitive state (Bassuk et al., Reference Bassuk, Berkman and Wypij1998; Geerlings et al., Reference Geerlings2000; Paterniti et al., Reference Paterniti2002; Ganguli et al., Reference Ganguli2006; Reyes-Ortiz et al., Reference Reyes-Ortiz2008; Köhler et al., Reference Köhler2010; Bunce et al., Reference Bunce2012; Gale et al., Reference Gale, Allerhand and Deary2012; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Hall and O'Bryant2013; Neubauer et al., Reference Neubauer, Wahl and Bickel2013; Royall and Palmer, Reference Royall and Palmer2013; Rajan et al., Reference Rajan2014; Chang and Tsai, Reference Chang and Tsai2015, Chen and Chang, Reference Chen and Chang2016; Brailean et al., Reference Brailean2017). However, it is difficult to draw a straightforward conclusion from this work due to conflicting findings. Specifically, some studies report a significant association between affective problems and decline in cognitive state (Geerlings et al., Reference Geerlings2000; Paterniti et al., Reference Paterniti2002; Reyes-Ortiz et al., Reference Reyes-Ortiz2008; Köhler et al., Reference Köhler2010; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Hall and O'Bryant2013; Royall and Palmer, Reference Royall and Palmer2013; Rajan et al., Reference Rajan2014; Chang and Tsai, Reference Chang and Tsai2015, Chen and Chang, Reference Chen and Chang2016), while conversely others report that affective problems do not predict decline (Bassuk et al., Reference Bassuk, Berkman and Wypij1998; Ganguli et al., Reference Ganguli2006; Bunce et al., Reference Bunce2012; Gale et al., Reference Gale, Allerhand and Deary2012; Neubauer et al., Reference Neubauer, Wahl and Bickel2013; Brailean et al., Reference Brailean2017). These contradictory results are likely attributable to inconsistencies in methodologies and study design, such as length of follow-up, sampling, definitions used, differences in assessment tools and also the primary aim of each study (Bennett and Thomas, Reference Bennett and Thomas2014). To date, however, there have been no systematic reviews or meta-analyses addressing associations between affective problems and subsequent decline in cognitive state, prior to the onset of dementia. Due to inconsistencies in findings, as well as the lack of attempts to synthesise these data, it is still unclear whether affective problems across the life course are associated with a decline in cognitive state, prior to the onset of dementia and cognitive impairment. The primary aim of the present study therefore was to systematically review and synthesise current evidence regarding the longitudinal association between affective problems (depression and anxiety separately) and subsequent decline in cognitive state, with consideration of several potential moderators, including mean age of sample at baseline, length of follow-up, quality of study and publication year.
Method
Search strategy
This review was conducted in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Moher et al., Reference Moher2009). A systematic literature search was conducted using PubMed, PsycInfo, Cochrane, and ScienceDirect databases for studies investigating the association between affective problems and decline in cognitive state. All studies published up to November 2016 were included in the search. There was no restriction on the start date. Our search terms comprised three search blocks (Table 1). The first search block included keywords relating to affective problems. The second search block contained keywords describing the decline in cognitive state. To reduce the number of irrelevant hits, a third search block was added, which contained keywords related to methodology, to ensure all studies with cross-sectional designs were excluded from search results. In addition, reference lists of relevant papers were scanned for articles of interest.
Inclusion/exclusion criteria
Stringent inclusion/exclusion criteria were applied to articles identified through the initial search.
Design criteria
Original studies written in English up to November 2016 were included. Only studies using longitudinal, prospective designs with human participants were included in order to test for the association between affective disorders and decline in cognitive state over time. Cross-sectional, case-control, experimental, including intervention and treatment, studies were excluded. Studies with a follow-up period of 1 year or greater were included, as it is possible that substantial decline in cognitive state may not be observed over very short follow-up periods in a general population. Included studies used samples drawn from a general population, whereas studies using specific clinical populations only, for example, a sample of stroke patients, were excluded. This criterion was used because inclusion of clinical samples may increase the heterogeneity of data synthesis and reduce the comparability of studies. Studies with a sample size of 100 or less were also considered ineligible, due to insufficient statistical power.
Outcome-related criteria
Samples with cognitive impairment or dementia present at baseline were excluded. In addition to this, studies with any measure of change in cognitive state from baseline to follow-up were selected for inclusion. Other outcomes, such as the transition to dementia or cognitive performance at follow-up without consideration of change from a baseline measure were omitted. This was because the present study aimed to look at the association between affective disorders and cognitive decline within healthy ageing populations, rather than samples with dementia. Additionally, studies assessing specific cognitive domains, such as attention or visuospatial ability exclusively, rather than cognitive state, were also excluded to reduce heterogeneity.
Predictor-related criteria
Both diagnostic and dimensional measures of depression and anxiety at baseline assessment were judged as eligible. Studies with retrospective assessments of affective problems were excluded, as such assessments may be less reliable. Both binary indicators of affective problems, defined as either a diagnosis or as a score above a threshold level, or continuous symptoms scores, as assessed by a validated scale of affective problems were included in this review.
Screening procedure
All articles identified through our search strategy were screened for eligibility using a three-step process. All references were first reviewed by title. Next, the remaining references were screened by abstract. Finally, all remaining articles were read in full and final eligibility determinations were made on this basis. All articles were reviewed for inclusion by one rater, and 10% of all articles were additionally screened by an independent rater, in order to assess the consistency of screening. Any disagreements were resolved during consensus meetings.
Data extraction
Data from the relevant articles were extracted using a detailed coding form. Information extracted included: Study information (Authors, publication year, DOI); Sample information (Country, mean age at baseline, gender composition, ethnicity, year of data collection, number of follow-ups, time between lags, total length of follow-up, sample size at baseline, sample size at final follow-up); Instrument information (Type of affective problem, measure used to assess affective problem, measure used to assess decline in cognitive state); Statistical information (Statistical test used, effect sizes, covariates adjusted for in statistical model). Where results for more than one follow-up were reported, the longest follow-up was selected for our analysis, as longer follow-up times allow a greater period for the decline in cognitive state to occur. Similarly, where multiple models were reported with various adjustments made, the most conservative model (with the greatest amount of adjustments) was selected. In cases where insufficient statistical information was available, original authors were contacted directly via email. All studies were evaluated for methodological quality using STROBE (Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology) guidelines (Elm et al., Reference Elm2007).
Statistical analysis and data synthesis
All analyses were conducted in R Studio (Studio, Reference Studio2012), using the metafor package. Separate meta-analyses were run for studies in which affective problems were assessed as a binary predictor (using a defined threshold), and studies where affective problems were assessed as a continuous predictor (using a symptom score). In addition, separate analyses were conducted for studies that used depression or anxiety as predictors of decline in cognitive state.
Odds ratios (ORs) were used as a common effect size across studies with a binary measure of affective problems. When ORs were not reported in original studies, these were estimated from available data using standard computational techniques (Lipsey and Wilson, Reference Lipsey and Wilson2000; Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein2009; Field and Gillett, Reference Field and Gillett2010). Log ORs were then computed for subsequent analysis. Standardised regression coefficients were used as a common effect size across studies with a continuous measure of affective problems. If unstandardised effect sizes were reported, or measures were not standardised to a z score before analysis, coefficients were converted to standardised coefficients using standard computational methods (Kim and Ferree, Reference Kim and Ferree1981; Duncan, Reference Duncan2014). In cases where insufficient information was reported in the study to calculate the standardised coefficients, authors were contacted directly via email. We also converted the estimated regression coefficients into ORs to facilitate the comparison with the analyses using a binary predictor of depression.
Multi-level meta-analyses were conducted to account for multiple effect sizes within studies (Van Den Noortgate and Onghena, Reference Van Den Noortgate and Onghena2003). Heterogeneity across studies was assessed using the Q statistic, with p < 0.1 suggesting significant heterogeneity between studies, and the I 2 statistic, in which 25, 50 and 75% represent low, medium and high heterogeneity (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins2003).
Additional meta-regression analyses were also conducted to assess the effects of potential moderators, including the length of follow-up, age of sample at baseline, publication year, method of affective problem assessment (diagnosis or self-report) and quality of studies. All moderators were entered initially as continuous variables, except for the assessment method of affective problems which was coded as a binary variable. For significant moderators, binary variables were created using the average values and sub-group analyses were run using these variables.
Publication bias was assessed using Begg's funnel plot and Begg's rank correlation test (Richard and Pillemer, Reference Light and Pillemer1984; Song et al., Reference Song2000; Egger et al., Reference Egger, Davey-Smith and Altman2008).
Results
Literature search
Our search identified 25 844 references. After exclusion of duplicates, 20 954 unique citations remained. At stage 1, citations were screened by title and 981 were determined to be eligible (inter-rater reliability = 96%). In stage 2, remaining citations were screened by abstract and 185 were judged as relevant (inter-rater reliability = 91%). Finally, all 185 citations were selected for full-text screening, after which 84 references remained (inter-rater reliability = 94%). At this stage, a further 36 studies were excluded, as they were addressing the decline in specific cognitive outcomes, rather than cognitive state. Of the 48 studies remaining, initially there were 18 with insufficient information for calculation of effect sizes. Authors were contacted directly by email about this and four (22%) responded to provide the relevant information. This left a total of 34 studies with sufficient information to calculate effect sizes, with 32 studies investigating the link between depression and a decline in cognitive state (n = 68 793), and five studies investigating anxiety and decline in cognitive state (n = 4698; Fig. 1 and Table 2).
Depression studies
Of the depression studies, 17 used a binary measure of depression (k = 34) and 16 measured depression as a continuous variable (k = 36). Depression studies had a mean follow-up length of approximately 6.61 years (s.d. = 4.41). The mean age of participants was 72.15 at baseline (s.d. = 7.56) and the gender composition of the sample was approximately 59.48% female. The majority of studies took place in the USA (n = 14), followed by the Netherlands (n = 4) and Taiwan (n = 3). Studies also took place in Australia, Canada, France (n = 2 for each), Germany, England, Italy, Singapore and Japan, Hawaii and the mainland-USA (n = 1 for each). Overall, the majority of studies used the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) to assess depression present at baseline (n = 16), followed by the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) (n = 7), the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS) (n = 3), the Neuroticism scale from the NEO Personality Inventory, the Duke Depression Evaluation Schedule (DDES), the Goldberg Depression Scale, Neuropsychiatric Inventory, Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HDRS), the Symptom Checklist (n = 1 for each). Only one study reported separate effect sizes for more than one follow-up period (Bassuk et al., Reference Bassuk, Berkman and Wypij1998). This study reported effect sizes at 3 years after baseline (n = 2030), 6 years after baseline (n = 1447) and 12 years after baseline (n = 756). The effect size with the longest follow-up (12 years) was selected for inclusion in the meta-analysis.
Anxiety studies
Of the five anxiety studies, two used a binary indicator of anxiety (k = 2) and three used a continuous measure of anxiety (k = 3). Anxiety studies had a mean follow-up time of 5.9 years (s.d. = 4.36). On average, participants were 76.56 years old at baseline (s.d. = 4.23) and were predominantly female (60.14% female). The majority of studies took place in Australia (n = 2), followed by the USA, the Netherlands and Israel (n = 1 for each). Anxiety was assessed using Sinoff's Short Anxiety Screening Test (SAST), Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale-Anxiety (HADS-A), Neuropsychiatric Inventory, Goldberg Anxiety Scale and the Neuroticism scale from the NEO Personality Inventory (n = 1 for each). All anxiety studies had a score of 60% or greater on the STROBE checklist (maximum score = 81%, median score = 78%).
Depression and decline in cognitive state
Meta-analysis of studies with depression as a binary predictor
There were 34 relevant effect sizes across 17 studies with a binary measure of depression (Dufouil et al., Reference Dufouil1996; Bassuk et al., Reference Bassuk, Berkman and Wypij1998; Yaffe et al., Reference Yaffe1999; Geerlings et al., Reference Geerlings2000; Paterniti et al., Reference Paterniti2002; Rosenblatt et al., Reference Rosenblatt2003; Ganguli et al., Reference Ganguli2006; Raji et al., Reference Raji2007; Han et al., Reference Han2008; Reyes-Ortiz et al., Reference Reyes-Ortiz2008; Niti et al., Reference Niti2009; Köhler et al., Reference Köhler2010; Brodaty et al., Reference Brodaty2012; Sawyer et al., Reference Sawyer2012; Chang and Tsai, Reference Chang and Tsai2015; Downer et al., Reference Downer2016; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson2016). A multi-level meta-analysis of 34 effect sizes revealed that depression was associated with an increased risk of subsequent decline in cognitive state (OR 1.36, 95% CI 1.05–1.76, p = .02; Fig. 2).
Assessment of heterogeneity, meta-regression and sub-group analyses
Significant heterogeneity was observed across the studies with depression as a binary predictor (Q = 106.83, df = 33, p < 0.0001, I 2 = 69.08%). An omnibus meta-regression analysis including publication year, mean age at baseline, length of follow-up, method of depression assessment (diagnosis or self-report) and quality of study revealed that these variables together were able to explain a significant amount of heterogeneity in the model (QM = 13.32, df = 5, p = 0.02). However, even after accounting for these factors, significant heterogeneity remained in the model (QE = 93.51, df = 28, p < 0.0001). To further explore the effect of publication year, age at baseline, length of follow-up, depression assessment and quality on heterogeneity, individual meta-regressions were conducted for each of these potential modifiers. These analyses revealed that mean age at baseline (p = 0.13), publication year (p = 0.19), quality of the study (p = 0.09) and depression assessment (p = 0.91) did not significantly explain the between-study variability.
Meta-regression analyses including the length of follow-up showed significant between-study variability, whereby studies with shorter follow-up periods had significantly greater effect sizes than studies with longer follow-up periods (B = −0.03, s.e. = 0.009, p = 0.002). Additionally, meta-regression analyses including the method of cognitive assessment (MMSE v. neuropsychiatric batteries) showed significant between-study variability (B = −0.2, s.e. = 0.08, p = 0.01). To further explore precisely how these significant factors were involved in this association sub-group meta-analyses were conducted.
To explore how length of follow-up affected the association, effect sizes were divided by the mean follow-up length in years (M = 6.35 (s.d. = 4.25) years), resulting in two groups of longer (k = 7, M = 10.79 (s.d. = 2.45) years) and shorter follow-up periods (k = 10, M = 3.25 (s.d. = 1.48) years). Multi-level sub-group meta-analyses revealed that depression was significantly associated with decline in cognitive state in studies with shorter follow-up periods (OR 1.43, 95% CI 1.03–2.00, p = 0.03) and was approaching significance in studies with longer follow-up periods (OR 1.15, 95% CI 0.98–1.36, p = 0.08). However, the overall effect size was larger for studies with shorter follow-up periods than those with longer follow-up periods. Studies with longer follow-up periods did not differ significantly from studies with shorter follow-up periods on quality (t (14.29) = 1.08, p = 0.3), publication year (t (15.38) = 0.1, p = 0.92), mean age at baseline (t (10.67) = −0.41, p = 0.69), or depression assessment (t (14.07) = −0.5, p = 0.63).
The meta-regression analysis including the method of cognitive assessment suggested that effect sizes were significantly smaller for studies using the MMSE than studies using neuropsychiatric batteries (B = −0.2, s.e. = 0.08, p = 0.01). However, there were only four studies using neuropsychiatric battery assessments of cognitive state, so results need to be treated with caution.
Meta-analysis of studies with depression as a continuous predictor
A multi-level meta-analysis of the 36 effect sizes across 16 studies with a continuous measure of depression was conducted (Geerlings et al., Reference Geerlings2000; Vinkers et al., Reference Vinkers2004; Han et al., Reference Han2006; Dotson et al., Reference Dotson, Resnick and Zonderman2008; Panza et al., Reference Panza2009; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson2011; Bunce et al., Reference Bunce2012; Gale et al., Reference Gale, Allerhand and Deary2012; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Hall and O'Bryant2013; Neubauer et al., Reference Neubauer, Wahl and Bickel2013; Royall and Palmer, Reference Royall and Palmer2013; Van den Kommer et al., Reference Van den Kommer2013; Rajan et al., Reference Rajan2014; Turner et al., Reference Turner2015; Chen and Chang, Reference Chen and Chang2016; Chiao and Weng, Reference Chiao and Weng2016). This analysis revealed that depression was significantly associated with a decline in cognitive state (B = −0.008, 95% CI −0.015 to −0.002, p = 0.012; Fig. 3; OR 0.992, 95% CI 0.985–0.998).
Assessment of heterogeneity and meta-regression analyses
Significant heterogeneity was observed across studies with depression as a continuous predictor (Q = 93.86, df = 35, p < 0.0001, I 2 = 69.74%). In order to try and explain some of this heterogeneity, an omnibus meta-regression analysis was conducted, including mean age at baseline, length of follow-up, quality and publication year as potential moderators. This analysis revealed that together these variables were not able to explain a significant amount of the heterogeneity in the model (QM = 8.97, df = 4, p = 0.06), but even after accounting for these factors, significant heterogeneity remained within the model (QE = 84.9, df = 31, p < 0.0001). In order to explore the influence of age at baseline, follow-up length, quality and publication year in more depth, individual meta-regressions were conducted for each potential modifier. These analyses revealed that mean age at baseline (p = 0.27), length of follow-up (p = 0.1), publication year (p = 0.18), method of cognitive assessment (p = 0.47) and quality (p = 0.11) could not significantly explain between-study variability individually.
Publication bias
Publication bias is unlikely for meta-analyses of depression studies measured as a continuous variable, as Begg's rank correlation test was non-significant (p = 0.07). Begg's funnel plot also appears relatively symmetrical (online Supplementary Fig. S1). There may have been some publication bias present in the meta-analysis of studies with depression as a binary predictor, as although Begg's funnel plot appears symmetrical (online Supplementary Fig. S1), Begg's rank correlation test was significant (p = 0.02). Results should, therefore, be interpreted with caution.
Anxiety and decline in cognitive state
Due to the limited number of studies with anxiety which met our inclusion criteria, meta-analyses for these studies were not possible. Instead, these studies are described in the form of a narrative review. Of the five relevant anxiety studies, two used a binary indicator of anxiety (Sinoff and Werner, Reference Sinoff and Werner2003; Brodaty et al., Reference Brodaty2012) and three used a continuous measure of anxiety (Bierman et al., Reference Bierman2008; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson2011; Bunce et al., Reference Bunce2012).
Two of these studies reported that anxiety was a significant predictor of decline in cognitive state (Sinoff and Werner, Reference Sinoff and Werner2003; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson2011). Specifically, Sinoff and Werner, Reference Sinoff and Werner2003 reported that in a sample of 100 people, anxiety (assessed using Sinoff's Short Anxiety Screening Test – SAST) had a strong direct and indirect effect on predicting future decline in cognitive state over 3.2 years (B = 0.23, 95% CI −0.03 to –3.95, p < 0.05). Similarly, Wilson et al. (Reference Wilson2011) found that in 785 older adults, higher levels of anxiety symptoms (assessed using the anxiety sub-scale from the 48-item Neuroticism scale) were significantly associated with more rapid decline in cognitive state over a 3.4-year period (B = −0.003, s.e. = 0.001, p = 0.01).
Conversely, three of the eligible studies found no association between anxiety symptoms and a decline in cognitive state (Bierman et al., Reference Bierman2008; Brodaty et al., Reference Brodaty2012; Bunce et al., Reference Bunce2012). Bierman et al. (Reference Bierman2008) found no evidence that anxiety (assessed using the anxiety sub-scale from the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale – HADS-A) predicted a linear decline in cognitive state in a sample of 2351 people over a period of 9 years. Instead, a significant negative quadratic trend for cognition was reported. The authors state that this is suggestive of a curvilinear association between anxiety levels and cognitive performance. Specifically, milder anxiety symptoms may be associated with an improvement on the MMSE until it reaches an optimal level, beyond which the beneficial influence reduces, so more severe anxiety is related to poorer cognitive function. The authors posit that the Yerkes and Dodson law regarding the association between arousal and cognitive performance (Yerkes and Dodson, Reference Yerkes and Dodson1908; Mendl, Reference Mendl1999) may also apply to anxiety symptoms. Brodaty et al. (Reference Brodaty2012) found that in a sample of 480 non-impaired people, the odds of decline in global cognitive state over a period of 2 years were not significantly higher for participants with anxiety (assessed using Neuropsychiatric Inventory) at baseline than those without (OR 1.63, 95% CI 0.5–5.8, p = 0.45). They did, however, find a significant effect of anxiety at baseline on the decline in executive function (OR 3.54, 95% CI 1.3–9.9, p = 0.016). Finally, Bunce et al. (Reference Bunce2012) found no evidence that anxiety (assessed using the Goldberg Anxiety Scale) affected change in cognitive state over a period of 12 years in a sample of 836 community-dwelling individuals over the age of 70 (B = −0.14, s.e. = 0.19, p = 0.46).
Discussion
The aim of the current study was to systematically investigate associations between affective problems (depression and anxiety) present at baseline and subsequent decline in cognitive state. Our findings revealed that individuals with depression (measured as a binary or continuous predictor) were at an increased risk of a greater decline in cognitive state. These findings are consistent with previous reviews which have indicated an association between affective problems and development of dementia (Jorm, Reference Jorm2001; Ownby et al., Reference Ownby2006; Byers and Yaffe, Reference Byers and Yaffe2011; da Silva et al., Reference Da Silva2013; Bennett and Thomas, Reference Bennett and Thomas2014; Cherbuin et al., Reference Cherbuin, Kim and Anstey2015; Gulpers et al., Reference Gulpers2016). Our results extend these findings by linking affective problems to a greater decline in cognitive state in samples without dementia at baseline.
Strengths and limitations
Several limitations of the current study must be acknowledged. They are subject to the limitations of the included studies. Our analyses suggest that there are several key methodological differences between studies which significantly affect the results produced. For example, our results suggest that effects may differ based on length of follow-up. As shown, this is unrelated to differences in publication year, age at baseline, or method of depression assessment (self-report or diagnosis). It is possible that this is more likely attributable to additional unobserved heterogeneity.
This review only included studies of decline in cognitive state as an outcome. For this reason, it is not clear whether affective problems may differentially influence decline in different cognitive domains. Additionally, the majority of included studies assessed cognitive state using the MMSE. This measure has been criticised for lacking sensitivity to subtle changes in cognition and for ceiling and floor effects (Tombaugh and McIntyre, Reference Tombaugh and McIntyre1992). Consistent with this, the meta-analysis of studies using depression as a binary predictor revealed that effect sizes are significantly smaller for studies using the MMSE than studies using neuropsychiatric batteries. It is therefore possible that the widespread use of the MMSE may have resulted in an underestimation of the association between affective disorders and decline in cognitive state in healthy older adults over time. One further limitation is that excluding cognitive impairment and dementia at baseline does not completely rule out the possibility of reverse causality.
Beyond this, included studies used different approaches and instruments to assess affective problems. Research suggests that there is low overlap among different scales of depression and anxiety, with content analysis suggesting that different types of assessments may capture different symptoms (Fried, Reference Fried2017). It is therefore possible that studies included in this review are not entirely comparable on the basis that the methods of assessing depression are heterogeneous and may each be capturing different kinds of symptoms. Beyond this, there were also very few studies which met our inclusion criteria which examined the association between anxiety and decline in cognitive state, meaning that a quantitative meta-analysis was not possible. Moreover, an additional limitation is that as there were no studies investigating comorbidity between anxiety and depression. Finally, many of the studies did not report separate effect sizes for different types of symptoms of affective problems (e.g. negative affect symptoms, somatic symptoms, etc.), meaning we could not look at how different symptoms of affective problems may influence decline in cognitive state in the current study.
Plausible mechanisms
Three major hypotheses have been proposed to explain this observed association. The first states that affective problems may act as an aetiological risk factor for the subsequent decline in cognitive state, perhaps by lowering the threshold for manifesting decline (Butters et al., Reference Butters2008; Bennett and Thomas, Reference Bennett and Thomas2014). The second hypothesis proposes that affective problems may act as a prodromal feature of dementia. Specifically, affective problems may manifest as an early clinical presentation of this disorder. Affective problems and decline in cognitive state may therefore be different symptoms of the same underlying condition (Panza et al., Reference Panza2010; Bennett and Thomas, Reference Bennett and Thomas2014). The third hypothesis posits that affective problems and decline in cognitive state are separate processes but may share common risk factors and underlying neurobiological substrates (Djernes, Reference Djernes2006; Enache et al., Reference Enache, Winblad and Aarsland2011; Bennett and Thomas, Reference Bennett and Thomas2014). These hypotheses are not necessarily mutually exclusive and it is likely that multiple pathways and mechanisms underlie this relationship.
There are several biological and behavioural pathways which may be involved in the association between affective problems and decline in cognitive state. These include vascular disease, increased cortisol production leading to atrophy of the hippocampus (Geerlings and Gerritsen, Reference Geerlings and Gerritsen2017), increased deposition of β-amyloid plaques (Byers and Yaffe, Reference Byers and Yaffe2011), inflammatory changes (Byers and Yaffe, Reference Byers and Yaffe2011) and a decline in the levels and activities of neurotrophic factors (Royall et al., Reference Royall2017). A multiple pathways model has also been proposed by Butters et al. (Reference Butters2008), which posits that depression-associated cerebrovascular disease and glucocorticoid neurotoxicity may operate to decrease levels of brain and cognitive reserve, as well as interact with pathology of Alzheimer's disease, giving rise to the clinical manifestation of Alzheimer's disease and accelerated cognitive decline. Additional potential lifestyle and behavioural pathways associated with affective problems include educational attainment, social support, early life adversity and health behaviours such as exercise regime, alcohol consumption, smoking status and medication status. It is more likely that a complex interaction of biological and sociobehavioural mechanisms are involved in linking affective problems with cognitive decline, rather than one single aetiological determinant (da Silva et al., Reference Da Silva2013).
Implications and future directions
Future research should focus on investigating whether effective treatment and management of affective problems may reduce risk of decline in cognitive state. Additionally, future reviews could focus on how affective problems are associated with decline in specific cognitive domains, such as memory, executive function and information processing speed. This information can help to elucidate the pattern of decline characteristic of individuals with a history of affective problems. The present review could not address the issue of comorbidity between depression and anxiety and how comorbidity is associated with subsequent decline in the mental state. Indeed, comorbidity of depression and anxiety disorders is common. It is estimated that around 50–60% of individuals who have experienced depression also have a history of anxiety disorder (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler1996; Fava et al., Reference Fava2000). Additionally, it is believed that comorbidity of anxiety and depression may be related to higher symptom severity and persistence, as well as poorer functional outcomes (Angst et al., 1999; Roy-Byrne et al., 2000). For this reason, it is important for future research to address how comorbid depression and anxiety is associated with future cognitive decline, and whether comorbidity of these conditions may result in poorer cognitive outcomes than depression or anxiety in isolation. One additional question which remains unresolved is whether affective problems act as a risk factor for the accelerated decline in cognitive state or whether they are an early biomarker representing prodromal dementia. While we excluded studies where cognitive impairment was present at baseline, it is also known that dementia has a preclinical period of many decades (Sperling et al., Reference Sperling2011). It is therefore possible that participants in included studies may have already built up substantial dementia pathology at baseline, even if cognitive symptoms were not yet apparent. Associations could therefore be due to reverse causality from subtle cognitive changes short of dementia. Future research should focus on distinguishing more clearly between these possibilities.
As average life expectancy lengthens and rapid demographic ageing occurs in populations worldwide, there is a dramatic predicted increase in the number of older adults living in our society (Oeppen and Vaupel, Reference Oeppen and Vaupel2002; Lutz et al., Reference Lutz, Sanderson and Scherbov2008). By 2030, it is estimated that approximately one in five people in England will be over the age of 65 (House of Lords, 2013). Given the predicted increase in population size of adults over the age of 65, as well as the poor outcomes and economic costs associated with a decline in cognitive state and impairment, it is important to identify life course risk factors for poorer late-life cognitive outcomes, for potential early intervention. These findings may have value in identifying individuals who may be at a greater risk of deterioration in cognitive function over time. It is possible that effective management and treatment of depression may reduce risk and improve cognitive outcomes within these individuals. However, there has also been some evidence to suggest there may be persisting neurocognitive disturbances even after remission of depression (Frasch et al., Reference Frasch2000; Weiland-Fiedler et al., Reference Weiland-Fiedler2004; Paelecke-Habermann et al., Reference Paelecke-Habermann, Pohl and Leplow2005). Additionally, cognition may be an important treatment target for depression (Kaser et al., Reference Kaser, Zaman and Sahakian2017). Due to the high prevalence of depression in the population, these results are of great public health importance.
In conclusion, demographic ageing is occurring rapidly worldwide and the number of people living with dementia is expected to grow substantially in prevalence over the next thirty years. As such, focussing research on potentially modifiable life-course risk factors, such as affective problems, is of increasing importance. This review highlights the importance of affective problems, particularly depression, in this context.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718001137
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank all the authors of the work included in the current review, and all the participants who took part in these studies.
Financial support
This project was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC). Grant Number: ES/J500173/1