Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T06:09:08.314Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Beyond IQ: executive function deficits and their relation to functional, clinical, and neuroimaging outcomes in 3q29 deletion syndrome

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 October 2024

Rebecca M. Pollak
Affiliation:
Center for Advanced Biotechnology and Medicine, Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, USA
Esra Sefik
Affiliation:
Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA
Katrina Aberizk
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
Kuaikuai Duan
Affiliation:
Marcus Autism Center, Children's Healthcare of Atlanta & Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
Roberto Espana
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
Ryan M. Guest
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
Adam E. Goldman-Yassen
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology and Imaging Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA Department of Radiology, Children's Healthcare of Atlanta, Atlanta, GA, USA
Katrina Goines
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
Derek M. Novacek
Affiliation:
Desert Pacific Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, VA Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System, Los Angeles, CA, USA Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Celine A. Saulnier
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA Neurodevelopmental Assessment & Consulting Services, Decatur, GA, USA
Cheryl Klaiman
Affiliation:
Marcus Autism Center, Children's Healthcare of Atlanta & Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA Department of Pediatrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
Stormi Pulver
Affiliation:
Marcus Autism Center, Children's Healthcare of Atlanta & Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA Department of Pediatrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
Joseph F. Cubells
Affiliation:
Department of Human Genetics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
T. Lindsey Burrell
Affiliation:
Atlanta Children's Center, Atlanta, GA, USA
Sarah Shultz
Affiliation:
Marcus Autism Center, Children's Healthcare of Atlanta & Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA Department of Pediatrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
Elaine F. Walker
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
Melissa M. Murphy
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
Jennifer G. Mulle*
Affiliation:
Center for Advanced Biotechnology and Medicine, Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, USA Department of Psychiatry, Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, USA
*
Corresponding author: Jennifer G. Mulle; Email: jennifer.mulle@rutgers.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

3q29 deletion syndrome (3q29del) is a rare (~1:30 000) genomic disorder associated with a wide array of neurodevelopmental and psychiatric phenotypes. Prior work by our team identified clinically significant executive function (EF) deficits in 47% of individuals with 3q29del; however, the nuances of EF in this population have not been described.

Methods

We used the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) to perform the first in-depth assessment of real-world EF in a cohort of 32 individuals with 3q29del (62.5% male, mean age = 14.5 ± 8.3 years). All participants were also evaluated with gold-standard neuropsychiatric and cognitive assessments. High-resolution structural magnetic resonance imaging was performed on a subset of participants (n = 24).

Results

We found global deficits in EF; individuals with 3q29del scored higher than the population mean on the BRIEF global executive composite (GEC) and all subscales. In total, 81.3% of study subjects (n = 26) scored in the clinical range on at least one BRIEF subscale. BRIEF GEC T scores were higher among 3q29del participants with a diagnosis of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and BRIEF GEC T scores were associated with schizophrenia spectrum symptoms as measured by the Structured Interview for Psychosis-Risk Syndromes. BRIEF GEC T scores were not associated with cognitive ability. The BRIEF-2 ADHD form accurately (sensitivity = 86.7%) classified individuals with 3q29del based on ADHD diagnosis status. BRIEF GEC T scores were correlated with cerebellar white matter and subregional cerebellar cortex volumes.

Conclusions

Together, these data expand our understanding of the phenotypic spectrum of 3q29del and identify EF as a core feature linked to both psychiatric and neuroanatomical features of the syndrome.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is used to distribute the re-used or adapted article and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

3q29 deletion syndrome (3q29del) is a rare (1:30 000) (Kendall et al., Reference Kendall, Rees, Escott-Price, Einon, Thomas, Hewitt and Kirov2017; Stefansson et al., Reference Stefansson, Meyer-Lindenberg, Steinberg, Magnusdottir, Morgen, Arnarsdottir and Stefansson2014) genomic disorder caused by the 1.6 Mb recurrent, typically de novo 3q29 deletion (hg19, chr3:195725000–197350000) (Ballif et al., Reference Ballif, Theisen, Coppinger, Gowans, Hersh, Madan-Khetarpal and Shaffer2008; Glassford, Rosenfeld, Freedman, Zwick, & Mulle, Reference Glassford, Rosenfeld, Freedman, Zwick and Mulle2016; Willatt et al., Reference Willatt, Cox, Barber, Cabanas, Collins, Donnai and Raymond2005). The clinical phenotype of 3q29del is heterogeneous, ranging from mild to moderate intellectual disability (ID) (Ballif et al., Reference Ballif, Theisen, Coppinger, Gowans, Hersh, Madan-Khetarpal and Shaffer2008; Cox & Butler, Reference Cox and Butler2015; Glassford et al., Reference Glassford, Rosenfeld, Freedman, Zwick and Mulle2016; Klaiman et al., Reference Klaiman, White, Saulnier, Murphy, Burrell, Cubells and Mulle2022; Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021; Willatt et al., Reference Willatt, Cox, Barber, Cabanas, Collins, Donnai and Raymond2005) to a 19-fold increased risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Itsara et al., Reference Itsara, Cooper, Baker, Girirajan, Li, Absher and Eichler2009; Pollak et al., Reference Pollak, Murphy, Epstein, Zwick, Klaiman, Saulnier and Mulle2019; Sanders et al., Reference Sanders, He, Willsey, Ercan-Sencicek, Samocha, Cicek and State2015) and a greater than 40-fold increased risk for schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SZ) (Kirov et al., Reference Kirov, Pocklington, Holmans, Ivanov, Ikeda, Ruderfer and Owen2012; Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Howrigan, Merico, Thiruvahindrapuram, Wu and Greer2017; Mulle, Reference Mulle2015; Mulle et al., Reference Mulle, Dodd, McGrath, Wolyniec, Mitchell, Shetty and Warren2010; Szatkiewicz et al., Reference Szatkiewicz, O'Dushlaine, Chen, Chambert, Moran, Neale and Sullivan2014). Recent work by our team has uncovered additional phenotypes associated with 3q29del, including attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and graphomotor weakness (Klaiman et al., Reference Klaiman, White, Saulnier, Murphy, Burrell, Cubells and Mulle2022; Pollak et al., Reference Pollak, Burrell, Cubells, Klaiman, Murphy, Saulnier and Mulle2023b; Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021). Further, 47% of study participants were found to have clinically significant executive function (EF) deficits (Klaiman et al., Reference Klaiman, White, Saulnier, Murphy, Burrell, Cubells and Mulle2022; Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021). However, our understanding of the nuances of these phenotypes, including EF, is still evolving.

EF refers to the constellation of higher-order cognitive processes that control and coordinate purposeful goal-directed behaviors (Baggetta & Alexander, Reference Baggetta and Alexander2016; Best & Miller, Reference Best and Miller2010; Miller & Wallis, Reference Miller and Wallis2009). It is generally agreed that EF is comprised of three core components: inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility (Baggetta & Alexander, Reference Baggetta and Alexander2016; Best & Miller, Reference Best and Miller2010; Miller & Wallis, Reference Miller and Wallis2009). These foundational functions are moderately inter-correlated yet separable, they can be differentially affected in different patient populations, and they serve as the basis for more complex cognitive constructs, such as planning, problem solving, and abstract reasoning (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013; Miyake et al., Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000). EF allows an individual to integrate information about their goals with sensory input to guide actions in an adaptive and dynamic manner, in accordance with the demands of the present context. EF deficits can have an adverse impact on academic and occupational achievement, mood regulation, and social function (Baggetta & Alexander, Reference Baggetta and Alexander2016; Best & Miller, Reference Best and Miller2010; Miller & Wallis, Reference Miller and Wallis2009). Studies have identified EF deficits in individuals with ADHD (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Monuteaux, Doyle, Seidman, Wilens, Ferrero and Faraone2004; Brown, Reference Brown2009; Marije Boonstra, Oosterlaan, Sergeant, & Buitelaar, Reference Marije Boonstra, Oosterlaan, Sergeant and Buitelaar2005; Martel, Nikolas, & Nigg, Reference Martel, Nikolas and Nigg2007) and SZ (Kraepelin, Reference Kraepelin1913; Lysaker et al., Reference Lysaker, Warman, Dimaggio, Procacci, LaRocco, Clark and Nicolò2008; Orellana & Slachevsky, Reference Orellana and Slachevsky2013; Pickup, Reference Pickup2008; Velligan & Bow-Thomas, Reference Velligan and Bow-Thomas1999; Wobrock et al., Reference Wobrock, Ecker, Scherk, Schneider-Axmann, Falkai and Gruber2009), as well as genomic disorders with phenotypic similarities to 3q29del, including 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (Albert, Abu-Ramadan, Kates, Fremont, & Antshel, Reference Albert, Abu-Ramadan, Kates, Fremont and Antshel2018; Everaert et al., Reference Everaert, Vorstman, Selten, Slieker, Wijnen, Boerma and Houben2023; Gur et al., Reference Gur, McDonald-McGinn, Moore, Gallagher, McClellan, White and McGinn2023; O'Hora et al., Reference O'Hora, Kushan-Wells, Hoftman, Jalbrzikowski, Gur, Gur and Bearden2023). Notably, EF ability has been shown to associate with later-onset phenotypes in children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (Albert et al., Reference Albert, Abu-Ramadan, Kates, Fremont and Antshel2018), highlighting the importance of understanding EF and its links to neurodevelopmental and psychiatric phenotypes, as well as its potential as an early treatment target with promising benefits for both childhood and adult outcomes.

The prefrontal cortex has canonically been associated with EF; however, it is now clear that these processes rely on distributed neural networks and emerging evidence points to a pivotal role for the cerebellum. Convergent evidence from neuropsychological testing, neuroimaging, humans with focal brain damage, and non-human animal studies have identified the cerebellum as a critical brain region for higher-order cognitive processing, including EF (Bellebaum & Daum, Reference Bellebaum and Daum2007; Deverett, Koay, Oostland, & Wang, Reference Deverett, Koay, Oostland and Wang2018; Koziol, Budding, & Chidekel, Reference Koziol, Budding and Chidekel2012; Schmahmann, Reference Schmahmann2019; Schmahmann & Sherman, Reference Schmahmann and Sherman1998). Notably, neuroimaging studies by our team have identified a particularly high frequency of structural anomalies in the posterior fossa of individuals with 3q29del, surpassing the combined occurrence of radiological anomalies found in all other brain regions; for example, more than 60% of 3q29del study participants have cerebellar hypoplasia and/or cystic or cyst-like malformations around the cerebellum, and both cerebellar cortex and white matter show significant volumetric differences compared to typically developing controls (Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021; Sefik et al., Reference Sefik, Li, Sholar, Evans, Pincus, Ammar and Mulle2024). These cerebellar abnormalities, coupled with the increased rate of EF deficits identified in individuals with 3q29del, highlight the importance of understanding this complex cognitive phenotype and its relationship with neurodevelopmental and psychiatric morbidity in 3q29del.

The present study is the first detailed description of EF abilities assessed in individuals with 3q29del. We define the profile of EF and we explore the relationship between EF and general cognitive ability, neurodevelopmental and psychiatric phenotypes, and cerebellar volume. This study is an important contribution to our evolving understanding of 3q29del; EF sits at the nexus of multiple neurodevelopmental and psychiatric phenotypes associated with 3q29del and may help to explain some of the underlying mechanisms contributing to the substantial psychiatric multimorbidity experienced by this population. The results from this study will help to guide future research to further explore EF in this population, as well as targeted interventions to improve EF abilities in individuals with 3q29del.

Methods

See online Supplementary information for detailed methods.

Study participants

Individuals with 3q29del were recruited from the online 3q29 registry (3q29deletion.org) for 2 days of in-person deep phenotyping, as previously described (Klaiman et al., Reference Klaiman, White, Saulnier, Murphy, Burrell, Cubells and Mulle2022; Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Lindsey Burrell, Cubells, Espana, Gambello, Goines and Mulle2018; Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021). Informed consent was provided by all participants over 18 years of age; for participants under 18 years of age, a parent or guardian provided informed consent and the study participant provided informed assent. In total, 32 individuals with 3q29del (62.5% male) were included in the present study. Study participants ranged in age from 4.85 to 39.12 years (mean = 14.5 ± 8.3 years). See Table 1 for a description of the study sample. This study was approved by Emory University's Institutional Review Board (IRB00064133) and Rutgers University's Institutional Review Board (PRO2021001360).

Table 1. Demographic information for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32)

Measures

The measures used in this study were as previously described (Klaiman et al., Reference Klaiman, White, Saulnier, Murphy, Burrell, Cubells and Mulle2022; Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Lindsey Burrell, Cubells, Espana, Gambello, Goines and Mulle2018; Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021). Briefly, EF was assessed using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Second Edition (BRIEF-2), for participants 18 years of age and younger (n = 26) or the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function for Adults (BRIEF-A) for participants over 18 years of age (n = 6) (Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2015; Roth & Gioia, Reference Roth and Gioia2005). General cognitive ability was evaluated using the Differential Ability Scales, Second Edition (Elliott, Murray, & Pearson, Reference Elliott, Murray and Pearson1990), for individuals under 18 years of age (n = 24) or the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, Second Edition (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999), for individuals 18 years of age and older (n = 8). Adaptive behavior was assessed using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition, Comprehensive Parent/Caregiver Form (Vineland-3) (Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Saulnier, Reference Sparrow, Cicchetti and Saulnier2016). Psychosis symptoms were assessed using the Structured Interview for Psychosis-Risk Syndromes (SIPS) by trained personnel for individuals 8 years of age and older (n = 23) (Miller et al., Reference Miller, McGlashan, Rosen, Cadenhead, Cannon, Ventura and Woods2003). Individual SIPS items are grouped within major symptom domains (positive, negative, and disorganization), and each item is rated on a scale from 0 (Absent) to 6 (Severe and Psychotic for positive symptoms, Extreme for others), with a rating of 3 (Moderate) indicating clinical significance. Item ratings were summed to produce a total score for each domain. Individuals exhibiting clinically significant attenuated positive symptoms (i.e. at least one positive symptom rated ⩾3) were considered to meet criteria for psychosis prodrome. Graphomotor abilities were assessed using the Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (VMI-6) (Beery & Beery, Reference Beery and Beery2010). Clinically significant graphomotor weakness was defined as a standard score >2 s.d. below the expected mean. Diagnoses of neurodevelopmental and psychiatric phenotypes, including SZ and other psychotic disorders, were reached using gold-standard evaluations and clinician best estimate diagnosis (American Psychiatric Association & American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Our prior investigations of 3q29del have revealed only minor morbidity associated with somatic conditions; the majority of disability in this population is due to neurodevelopmental and psychiatric illness (Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021). We have previously defined a set of neurodevelopmental and psychiatric conditions with increased prevalence in 3q29del (online Supplementary Table S1). Multimorbidity was therefore defined as the total number of these neurodevelopmental and psychiatric conditions, with a current diagnosis as established by our expert clinicians at the time of the study visit.

Neuroimaging

High-resolution structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data were collected and processed as previously described (Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021; Sefik et al., Reference Sefik, Li, Sholar, Evans, Pincus, Ammar and Mulle2024).

Analysis

All analyses were performed in R version 4.0.4 (R Core Team, 2008). Due to the small sample size, most analyses were considered exploratory and unadjusted p values were reported. Statistical analysis was performed using simple linear models and one-sided, one-sample Student's t-tests implemented using the stats R package (R Core Team, 2008). p Values for simple linear models were calculated using robust standard errors via the sandwich and lmtest R packages (Hothorn et al., Reference Hothorn, Zeileis, Farebrother, Cummins, Millo, Mitchell and Zeileis2015; Zeileis, Lumley, Berger, Graham, & Zeileis, Reference Zeileis, Lumley, Berger, Graham and Zeileis2019). All models were adjusted for age and sex. For neuroimaging data, multiple linear regression models were constructed adjusting for age and sex; for models where there was a significant relationship with the absolute brain region volume, models were re-run adjusting for age, sex, and estimated total intracranial volume (eICV) to test for regional specificity beyond the influence of global variability in head size. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves were constructed using the ROCit R package (Khan & Brandenburger, Reference Khan and Brandenburger2020). Data visualization was performed using the plotly R package (Sievert et al., Reference Sievert, Parmer, Hocking, Chamberlain, Ram, Corvellec and Despouy2017).

Results

Global EF deficits in 3q29del

Higher scores on the BRIEF indicate worse EF; a T score of 70 or higher indicates a clinically significant deficit. To account for developmentally appropriate changes in EF abilities over the lifespan (Anderson, Reference Anderson2002), BRIEF T scores are age-normed. On average, study participants with 3q29del scored nominally higher than the population mean T score of 50 on the Global Executive Composite (GEC) (mean = 67.8 ± 10.7, p < 0.001) and across all subscales (all p < 0.001, Fig. 1a, online Supplementary Table S3). There were no differences between males and females with 3q29del across all domains of the BRIEF (online Supplementary Fig. S1). In total, 15 study participants with 3q29del (46.9%) scored above the clinical threshold of 70 on the GEC, indicating clinically significant EF deficits. Of the nine subscales, study participants with 3q29del showed the most impairment on the Shift subscale (mean = 69.2 ± 12.5), and the least impairment on the Organization of Materials subscale (mean = 60.1 ± 11.6, Fig. 1a, online Supplementary Table S3). Study participants with 3q29del demonstrated a range of impairments across BRIEF subscales; six participants (18.8%) did not score above the clinical threshold on any subscales, while two participants (6.3%) scored above the clinical threshold on all nine subscales (Fig. 1b). A majority of study participants with 3q29del (n = 22, 68.8%) scored above the clinical threshold on two or more subscales and 81.3% of study participants with 3q29del (n = 26) scored above the clinical threshold on at least one subscale (Fig. 1b), further emphasizing the substantial burden of executive dysfunction in this population. Together, these data demonstrate significant adverse impacts to EF abilities in individuals with 3q29del.

Figure 1. (a) Distribution of T scores on the BRIEF GEC and BRIEF subscales for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32). The black dashed line indicates the population mean; the red dashed line indicates the clinical cutoff. Subscales are ordered left to right by decreasing mean severity. (b) Pie chart showing the proportion of study participants with 3q29del (n = 32) scoring in the clinical range (T scores ⩾ 70) on one or more BRIEF scales, expanded to show the proportion of participants scoring in the clinical range on 1 to 9 BRIEF scales. 3q29del, 3q29 deletion syndrome; BRIEF, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; GEC, global executive composite.

EF is orthogonal to cognitive ability in 3q29del

The 3q29 deletion is commonly associated with mild-to-moderate ID (Ballif et al., Reference Ballif, Theisen, Coppinger, Gowans, Hersh, Madan-Khetarpal and Shaffer2008; Cox & Butler, Reference Cox and Butler2015; Glassford et al., Reference Glassford, Rosenfeld, Freedman, Zwick and Mulle2016; Willatt et al., Reference Willatt, Cox, Barber, Cabanas, Collins, Donnai and Raymond2005); in the present study, the mean composite IQ in study participants with 3q29del was 73.03 ± 14.18, as previously reported (Klaiman et al., Reference Klaiman, White, Saulnier, Murphy, Burrell, Cubells and Mulle2022; Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021). However, there was substantial variability in IQ across study participants, ranging from moderate ID to normal cognitive ability (range = 40–99) (Klaiman et al., Reference Klaiman, White, Saulnier, Murphy, Burrell, Cubells and Mulle2022; Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021). We sought to determine whether variation in EF is correlated with variability in cognitive ability in our study participants. There was no relationship between BRIEF GEC T scores and composite (r 2 = −0.01, p = 0.4), non-verbal (r 2 = 0.04, p = 0.15), or verbal IQ (r 2 = −0.03, p = 0.9; Fig. 2a–c). Together, these data demonstrate that EF is orthogonal to cognitive ability in individuals with 3q29del, and that poor performance on the BRIEF is not an artifact of diminished cognitive ability in our study population.

Figure 2. (a) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and composite IQ for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32). (b) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and non-verbal IQ for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32). (c) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and verbal IQ for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32). (d) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and SIPS Positive Symptom Ratings for study participants with 3q29del (n = 23). (e) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and SIPS Negative Symptom Ratings for study participants with 3q29del (n = 23). (f) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and SIPS Disorganization Symptom Ratings for study participants with 3q29del (n = 23). 3q29del, 3q29 deletion syndrome; BRIEF, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; GEC, global executive composite; SIPS, Structured Interview for Psychosis-Risk Syndromes.

EF is correlated with psychosis spectrum symptoms

The 3q29 deletion is the largest known genetic risk factor for SZ (Mulle, Reference Mulle2015; Mulle et al., Reference Mulle, Dodd, McGrath, Wolyniec, Mitchell, Shetty and Warren2010; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Poterba, Curtis, Akil, Al Eissa, Barchas and Daly2022); EF deficits have a well-established association with SZ (Kraepelin, Reference Kraepelin1913; Lysaker et al., Reference Lysaker, Warman, Dimaggio, Procacci, LaRocco, Clark and Nicolò2008; Orellana & Slachevsky, Reference Orellana and Slachevsky2013; Pickup, Reference Pickup2008; Velligan & Bow-Thomas, Reference Velligan and Bow-Thomas1999; Wobrock et al., Reference Wobrock, Ecker, Scherk, Schneider-Axmann, Falkai and Gruber2009). We sought to define the relationship between EF and psychosis spectrum symptoms in our study population. We found nominally significant positive correlations between the BRIEF GEC and positive and disorganization symptoms endorsed on the SIPS (Fig. 2d–f), indicating that individuals with 3q29del and poorer EF experience more severe psychosis spectrum symptoms on average. The strongest correlation was between the BRIEF GEC and the SIPS disorganization symptom dimension (r 2 = 0.18, p = 0.02; Fig. 2f). There was a weak relationship between the BRIEF GEC and the SIPS positive symptom dimension (r 2 = 0.06, p = 0.03; Fig. 2d). There was no relationship between the BRIEF GEC and the SIPS negative symptom dimension (r 2 = −0.008, p = 0.4; Fig. 2e). These data show that EF deficits are nominally significantly associated with the severity of two major dimensions of psychosis spectrum symptoms in individuals with 3q29del.

Psychiatric and neurodevelopmental multimorbidity is associated with EF deficits

Individuals with 3q29del are at increased liability for a wide range of neurodevelopmental and psychiatric phenotypes, including ASD, anxiety disorder, ADHD, and SZ (Ballif et al., Reference Ballif, Theisen, Coppinger, Gowans, Hersh, Madan-Khetarpal and Shaffer2008; Cox & Butler, Reference Cox and Butler2015; Glassford et al., Reference Glassford, Rosenfeld, Freedman, Zwick and Mulle2016; Itsara et al., Reference Itsara, Cooper, Baker, Girirajan, Li, Absher and Eichler2009; Kirov et al., Reference Kirov, Pocklington, Holmans, Ivanov, Ikeda, Ruderfer and Owen2012; Klaiman et al., Reference Klaiman, White, Saulnier, Murphy, Burrell, Cubells and Mulle2022; Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Howrigan, Merico, Thiruvahindrapuram, Wu and Greer2017; Mulle, Reference Mulle2015; Mulle et al., Reference Mulle, Dodd, McGrath, Wolyniec, Mitchell, Shetty and Warren2010; Pollak et al., Reference Pollak, Murphy, Epstein, Zwick, Klaiman, Saulnier and Mulle2019; Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021; Sanders et al., Reference Sanders, He, Willsey, Ercan-Sencicek, Samocha, Cicek and State2015; Szatkiewicz et al., Reference Szatkiewicz, O'Dushlaine, Chen, Chambert, Moran, Neale and Sullivan2014; Willatt et al., Reference Willatt, Cox, Barber, Cabanas, Collins, Donnai and Raymond2005). In the general population, ADHD and SZ are both associated with poorer EF (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Monuteaux, Doyle, Seidman, Wilens, Ferrero and Faraone2004; Brown, Reference Brown2009; Kraepelin, Reference Kraepelin1913; Lysaker et al., Reference Lysaker, Warman, Dimaggio, Procacci, LaRocco, Clark and Nicolò2008; Marije Boonstra et al., Reference Marije Boonstra, Oosterlaan, Sergeant and Buitelaar2005; Martel et al., Reference Martel, Nikolas and Nigg2007; Orellana & Slachevsky, Reference Orellana and Slachevsky2013; Pickup, Reference Pickup2008; Velligan & Bow-Thomas, Reference Velligan and Bow-Thomas1999; Wobrock et al., Reference Wobrock, Ecker, Scherk, Schneider-Axmann, Falkai and Gruber2009). To determine whether any individual neurodevelopmental or psychiatric diagnosis was associated with EF in our study population, we compared the BRIEF GEC T scores for individuals with 3q29del with and without ASD, SZ prodrome/psychosis, ADHD, anxiety disorders, graphomotor weakness, ID, and enuresis (Fig. 3a). We found that individuals with 3q29del and a diagnosis of ADHD have nominally significantly worse EF relative to individuals with 3q29del without ADHD (ADHD mean = 72.8 ± 8.06, no ADHD mean = 59.5 ± 9.4, p = 3.08 × 10−8; Fig. 3a); there were no other relationships. These data demonstrate that the relationship between EF and ADHD phenotypes is present in individuals with 3q29del.

Figure 3. (a) Distribution of BRIEF GEC T scores for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32) with and without specific neurodevelopmental or psychiatric diagnoses. (b) Distribution of BRIEF GEC T scores for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32) with an increasing number of multimorbid neurodevelopmental and psychiatric diagnoses. (c) Receiver operating characteristic curve showing the ability of the BRIEF-2 ADHD form to correctly classify study participants with 3q29del (n = 26) with and without a diagnosis of ADHD. 3q29del, 3q29 deletion syndrome; BRIEF, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; GEC, global executive composite; ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

Prior work by our team has identified neurodevelopmental and psychiatric multimorbidity as a hallmark feature of 3q29del (Pollak et al., Reference Pollak, Burrell, Cubells, Klaiman, Murphy, Saulnier and Mulle2023a; Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021); we found that increasing degrees of multimorbidity, rather than individual neurodevelopmental or psychiatric diagnoses, is significantly associated with poorer adaptive function in this population (Pollak et al., Reference Pollak, Burrell, Cubells, Klaiman, Murphy, Saulnier and Mulle2023a). We sought to determine whether there is a similar relationship between EF and multimorbidity in our study population. We found that BRIEF GEC T scores are correlated with multimorbidity, where increasing multimorbidity is nominally significantly associated with poorer EF (p = 0.04; Fig. 3b). Together, these data emphasize the central nature of neurodevelopmental and psychiatric multimorbidity to the 3q29del phenotype, and show that multimorbidity, rather than individual neurodevelopmental or psychiatric diagnoses, has a stronger relationship with EF in individuals with 3q29del.

BRIEF-2 is an accurate screener for ADHD in individuals with 3q29del

Screening tools are valuable instruments that can be used to prioritize individuals for diagnostic evaluations or to identify sub-populations for future studies. In the case of 3q29del, identifying effective screening tools for specific neurodevelopmental or psychiatric phenotypes will help to ensure that the highest-risk individuals will receive critical diagnostic evaluations as early as possible, while simultaneously reducing the burden of multi-disorder diagnostic batteries on caregivers and the healthcare system. Our team has identified ADHD as one of the most common psychiatric diagnoses in individuals with 3q29del, with 63% of individuals with 3q29del qualifying for a diagnosis of ADHD in a recent study (Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021). ADHD is intimately linked to EF (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Monuteaux, Doyle, Seidman, Wilens, Ferrero and Faraone2004; Brown, Reference Brown2009; Marije Boonstra et al., Reference Marije Boonstra, Oosterlaan, Sergeant and Buitelaar2005; Martel et al., Reference Martel, Nikolas and Nigg2007); here, we sought to determine whether the BRIEF-2 ADHD form is an accurate screening tool for ADHD in children with 3q29del (n = 26). We found that the BRIEF-2 ADHD form had a sensitivity rate of 86.7%, indicating that 13.3% of individuals with ADHD did not screen positive on the ADHD form (n = 2), and a specificity rate of 60.0%, indicating that 40.0% of individuals that screened positive did not have an ADHD diagnosis (n = 4) (Fig. 3c). The BRIEF-2 ADHD form is a more sensitive screener for ADHD in individuals with 3q29del than the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist DSM-keyed Attention-deficit/hyperactivity problems scale (Pollak, Mortillo, Murphy, & Mulle, Reference Pollak, Mortillo, Murphy and Mulle2024), which had a sensitivity of 72.2% and a specificity of 81.8%. Together, these data show that the BRIEF-2 ADHD form can be a useful screening tool for ADHD in children with 3q29del.

EF ability is associated with cerebellar volumetric changes

While the prefrontal cortex has long been considered the locus of EF, emerging evidence highlights the cerebellum's contribution to higher-order cognition (Bellebaum & Daum, Reference Bellebaum and Daum2007; Koziol et al., Reference Koziol, Budding and Chidekel2012; Schmahmann, Reference Schmahmann2019; Schmahmann & Sherman, Reference Schmahmann and Sherman1998). Work by our team has identified significant cerebellar abnormalities associated with 3q29del; for example, over 60% of 3q29del individuals show cerebellar hypoplasia (Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021; Sefik et al., Reference Sefik, Li, Sholar, Evans, Pincus, Ammar and Mulle2024). We sought to determine whether cerebellar anomalies are associated with EF abilities in individuals with 3q29del. There was no relationship between BRIEF GEC T scores and total cerebellar volume (r 2 = −0.05, p = 0.82; Fig. 4b). There was a nominally significant relationship between BRIEF GEC T scores and cerebellar white matter volume, where worse EF is associated with increased cerebellar white matter volume (r 2 = 0.35, p = 8.11 × 10−4; Fig. 4c); this relationship persisted after adjusting for eICV (r 2 = 0.35, p = 7.72 × 10−4). There was a nominally significant relationship between BRIEF GEC T scores and cerebellar cortical volume, where worse EF is associated with decreased cerebellar cortical volume (r 2 = 0.05, p = 0.04; Fig. 4d); however, this relationship did not persist after adjusting for eICV (r 2 = 0.02, p = 0.1). In a prior study our team identified an increased prevalence of posterior fossa arachnoid cysts and mega cisterna magna (PFAC/MCM) in individuals with 3q29del (Sanchez Russo et al., Reference Sanchez Russo, Gambello, Murphy, Aberizk, Black, Burrell and Mulle2021; Sefik et al., Reference Sefik, Li, Sholar, Evans, Pincus, Ammar and Mulle2024); however, BRIEF GEC T scores were not significantly different between individuals with 3q29del with and without PFAC/MCM (PFAC/MCM mean = 66.23 ± 9.19, no PFAC/MCM mean = 67.09 ± 11.77, p = 0.65; online Supplementary Fig. S2A).

Figure 4. (a) Diagram illustrating the cerebellum with a representative coronal image from a T1-weighted MRI showing cerebellar white matter and cerebellar cortex. (b) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and total cerebellar volume for study participants with 3q29del (n = 23). (c) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and cerebellar white matter volume for study participants with 3q29del (n = 23). (d) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and cerebellar cortical (grey matter) volume for study participants with 3q29del (n = 23). 3q29del, 3q29 deletion syndrome; BRIEF, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; GEC, global executive composite.

To further explore the putative relationship between EF and cerebellar cortical volume in study subjects with 3q29del, we analyzed the relationship between BRIEF GEC T scores and cerebellar cortex subregional volumes (online Supplementary Fig. S2B–R). There were no significant relationships between BRIEF GEC T scores and any subregions of the anterior lobe of the cerebellar cortex (online Supplementary Fig. S2C and D). Within the vermis, posterior lobe, and flocculonodular lobe of the cerebellar cortex, specific subregions were nominally significantly associated with BRIEF GEC T scores. In general, increased volume of the subregion was associated with worse EF. The following regions showed nominally significant relationships with BRIEF GEC T scores that persisted after adjusting for eICV: left cerebellar hemisphere lobule VI volume (unadjusted r 2 = 0.072, p = 0.037; adjusted r 2 = 0.13, p = 0.04; online Supplementary Fig. S2F); right cerebellar hemisphere lobule VIII volume (unadjusted r 2 = 0.13, p = 0.04; adjusted r 2 = 0.15, p = 0.04; online Supplementary Fig. S2N); and left cerebellar hemisphere lobule IX volume (unadjusted r 2 = 0.12, p = 0.03; adjusted r 2 = 0.15, p = 0.03; online Supplementary Fig. S2O). There were two regions that had a nominally significant relationship with BRIEF GEC T scores that did not persist after adjusting for eICV: cerebellar vermis VI–VII volume (unadjusted r 2 = 0.12, p = 0.05; adjusted r 2 = 0.15, p = 0.06; online Supplementary Fig. S2E); and right cerebellar hemisphere lobule X volume (unadjusted r 2 = 0.17, p = 0.05; adjusted r 2 = 0.19, p = 0.06; online Supplementary Fig. S2R). Together, these data demonstrate a relationship between EF and cerebellar white matter volume in individuals with 3q29del and reveal subregion-specific relationships between EF and cerebellar cortical volume localized mostly to the posterior lobe of the cerebellar cortex, consistent with the presence of a functional topography within the cerebellum (Stoodley & Schmahmann, Reference Stoodley and Schmahmann2010; Stoodley, Desmond, Guell, & Schmahmann, Reference Stoodley, Desmond, Guell, Schmahmann, Manto, Gruol, Schmahmann, Koibuchi and Sillitoe2020).

Discussion

The present study is the first detailed description of EF abilities in individuals with 3q29del. We identified global deficits in EF, with elevated mean scores on the BRIEF GEC as well as across all nine BRIEF subscales. The Shift subscale, a measure of cognitive flexibility, showed the highest mean impairment in 3q29del. Individuals with 3q29del and a diagnosis of ADHD had substantially poorer EF relative to individuals with 3q29del without ADHD; poorer EF was also associated with more severe SZ spectrum symptoms as measured by the SIPS. Additionally, the BRIEF-2 ADHD form accurately discriminated between individuals with 3q29del with and without a diagnosis of ADHD, highlighting its potential role as a time-efficient screening tool in this population. Neurodevelopmental and psychiatric multimorbidity was associated with EF, with an increasing number of diagnoses corresponding to poorer EF. Furthermore, BRIEF GEC scores showed no correlation with cognitive ability, indicating that mechanisms underlying cognitive ability and everyday EF may be orthogonal in 3q29del, while both factors contribute to adaptive behavior as reported previously (Pollak et al., Reference Pollak, Burrell, Cubells, Klaiman, Murphy, Saulnier and Mulle2023a). Finally, EF was correlated with volumetric measures of the cerebellum, potentially identifying neuroanatomical changes contributing to EF deficits in this population.

While this is the first description of EF in 3q29del, deficits in EF in this population are not without precedent. Genomic disorders with phenotypic similarities to 3q29del, including 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (Albert et al., Reference Albert, Abu-Ramadan, Kates, Fremont and Antshel2018; Everaert et al., Reference Everaert, Vorstman, Selten, Slieker, Wijnen, Boerma and Houben2023; Gur et al., Reference Gur, McDonald-McGinn, Moore, Gallagher, McClellan, White and McGinn2023; O'Hora et al., Reference O'Hora, Kushan-Wells, Hoftman, Jalbrzikowski, Gur, Gur and Bearden2023), have documented evidence of significant EF deficits. There is a large body of literature surrounding EF abilities in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome specifically; EF deficits in this population have been identified across the lifespan, from preschool-aged children to adults (Albert et al., Reference Albert, Abu-Ramadan, Kates, Fremont and Antshel2018; Everaert et al., Reference Everaert, Vorstman, Selten, Slieker, Wijnen, Boerma and Houben2023; Gur et al., Reference Gur, McDonald-McGinn, Moore, Gallagher, McClellan, White and McGinn2023; O'Hora et al., Reference O'Hora, Kushan-Wells, Hoftman, Jalbrzikowski, Gur, Gur and Bearden2023). Worsening EF is associated with psychosis spectrum symptoms in children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (Gur et al., Reference Gur, McDonald-McGinn, Moore, Gallagher, McClellan, White and McGinn2023), and measures of childhood EF can predict young adult outcomes in this population, including symptoms of psychosis (Albert et al., Reference Albert, Abu-Ramadan, Kates, Fremont and Antshel2018). These data are similar to our findings in the present study of individuals with 3q29del and suggest that EF may be a core feature central to understanding the wide neurodevelopmental and psychiatric phenotypic spectrum associated with the 3q29 deletion, including the substantial risk for conversion to psychosis.

In addition to the links between EF and genomic disorders phenotypically similar to 3q29del, EF deficits have also been long associated with idiopathic SZ and ADHD. Indeed, concepts related to EF have been associated with the SZ phenotype since the early 20th century (Kraepelin, Reference Kraepelin1913); EF deficits are the most common cognitive phenotype in individuals with SZ (Orellana & Slachevsky, Reference Orellana and Slachevsky2013; Velligan & Bow-Thomas, Reference Velligan and Bow-Thomas1999; Wobrock et al., Reference Wobrock, Ecker, Scherk, Schneider-Axmann, Falkai and Gruber2009). EF deficits are also a core feature of ADHD (Brown, Reference Brown2009; Silverstein et al., Reference Silverstein, Faraone, Leon, Biederman, Spencer and Adler2020) and are present across the lifespan (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Monuteaux, Doyle, Seidman, Wilens, Ferrero and Faraone2004; Marije Boonstra et al., Reference Marije Boonstra, Oosterlaan, Sergeant and Buitelaar2005; Martel et al., Reference Martel, Nikolas and Nigg2007). Previous research indicates that behavioral scales like the BRIEF can more accurately predict ADHD status and assess different components of EF than performance-based measures (Mahone & Hoffman, Reference Mahone and Hoffman2007; Tan, Delgaty, Steward, & Bunner, Reference Tan, Delgaty, Steward and Bunner2018; Toplak, Bucciarelli, Jain, & Tannock, Reference Toplak, Bucciarelli, Jain and Tannock2008). It is important to recognize that both the BRIEF and the diagnostic assessment of ADHD rely on caregiver reports of everyday behaviors, which may lead to overlapping evaluations that could inflate their correlation. However, while there is an intersection between the constructs assessed by the BRIEF and ADHD diagnosis, EF weaknesses are neither necessary nor sufficient to reach a DSM-5 diagnosis in the clinic. Additional performance-based assessments or reports from other sources will be used in the future to provide further evaluation of the relationship between EF difficulties and clinical diagnoses in larger cohorts. We identified a relationship between EF and diagnoses of ADHD, as well as SZ spectrum symptom severity, in our study sample of individuals with 3q29del. EF was nominally significantly associated with both Positive and Disorganized SZ spectrum symptom severity. Some items included in the Disorganized domain of the SIPS are similar to features assessed by the BRIEF, including Trouble with Focus and Attention, and this similarity may be contributing to the nominal association between the BRIEF and the SIPS Disorganized symptom domain. Other Disorganized symptom domain items like Odd Behavior or Appearance and Bizarre Thinking are less entangled with the BRIEF. All items were rated based on their presence and severity and without regard to other comorbidities. Notably, the distinct features measured by the BRIEF and the SIPS Positive symptom domain indicate that the nominal association between the BRIEF and SZ spectrum symptom severity in individuals with 3q29del is not merely due to confounding. The concordance between our findings and results from studies of idiopathic cases of SZ and ADHD suggest that understanding EF deficits in the context of the 3q29 deletion may provide generalizable insights to the study of SZ and ADHD at large.

Cognitive ability, specifically composite IQ, has canonically been used as a proxy for an individual's level of everyday functioning. The results of the present study, notably the astonishingly high rate of clinically significant EF deficits alongside the lack of a relationship between cognitive ability and EF, suggest that IQ alone may not be a sufficient measure to understand real-world function for individuals with 3q29del. Traditional cognitive testing may overlook impairments in crucial aspects of higher-order cognitive functioning that are relevant to everyday behaviors in this population. This finding is aligned with prior reports that have shown that certain EFs, including cognitive flexibility, are either uncorrelated with or are relatively weakly related to IQ (Ardila, Pineda, & Rosselli, Reference Ardila, Pineda and Rosselli2000; Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Corley, Young, Defries and Hewitt2006), suggesting that traditional intelligence tests are insufficient in gauging the full spectrum of fundamental executive control abilities essential for various behaviors. This is of particular concern for individuals with 3q29del who have IQ scores well within the normal range alongside compromised EF; IQ scores in the normal range may create a perception of academic ability that is not accurate, given the co-occurring EF deficits. Together, these data suggest that individuals with 3q29del should be clinically evaluated using measures of both cognitive ability and EF as a standard of care. Furthermore, plans for treatment, management, and educational and occupational support should explicitly address and support EF deficits.

We identified a nominally significant relationship between cerebellar volumetric measures and EF in the present study. There is emerging evidence linking the cerebellum to a range of cognitive processes, including EF (Bellebaum & Daum, Reference Bellebaum and Daum2007; Koziol et al., Reference Koziol, Budding and Chidekel2012; Schmahmann, Reference Schmahmann2019; Schmahmann & Sherman, Reference Schmahmann and Sherman1998). Changes in cerebellar structure and function have been identified in children with ADHD (Bechtel et al., Reference Bechtel, Kobel, Penner, Klarhöfer, Scheffler, Opwis and Weber2009; Tomasi & Volkow, Reference Tomasi and Volkow2012), suggesting that cerebellar anomalies may contribute to the pathogenesis of ADHD. The cerebellum is also thought to have a role in SZ, due to its role in cognitive processes and the increased incidence of cerebellar anomalies in individuals with SZ (Andreasen & Pierson, Reference Andreasen and Pierson2008; Picard, Amado, Mouchet-Mages, Olié, & Krebs, Reference Picard, Amado, Mouchet-Mages, Olié and Krebs2008; Yeganeh-Doost, Gruber, Falkai, & Schmitt, Reference Yeganeh-Doost, Gruber, Falkai and Schmitt2011). Consistent with the findings of the present study, increased cerebellar white matter volume has been reported in idiopathic SZ, possibly indicating abnormal cerebellar connectivity (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Farrow, Parks, Newton, Mir, Egleston and Woodruff2007). Our findings suggest that this is another point of convergence between 3q29del and idiopathic SZ; further exploration of cerebellar connectivity and EF in 3q29del may shed light on the neuroanatomical underpinnings of these complex phenotypes. In an exploratory analysis, we also identified nominally significant relationships between EF and cerebellar cortical subregional volumes, where increased subregional volume was associated with poorer EF. Future studies are required to replicate these exciting preliminary findings and may yield additional insight into altered cerebellar connectivity in 3q29del.

The genes affected by the 3q29 deletion may provide some insight into the mechanisms contributing to EF deficits in this population. The multi-domain scaffolding protein DLG1 and the serine/threonine kinase PAK2, encoded by two genes in the 3q29 interval, are important in synapse maintenance, cytoskeletal dynamics, long-term potentiation, and synaptic transmission (Howard, Elias, Elias, Swat, & Nicoll, Reference Howard, Elias, Elias, Swat and Nicoll2010; Kreis & Barnier, Reference Kreis and Barnier2009; Nakagawa et al., Reference Nakagawa, Futai, Lashuel, Lo, Okamoto, Walz and Sheng2004; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Zeng, Li, Zhou, Ju, Xia and Sun2018). Another 3q29 interval gene, UBXN7, participates in the ubiquitin–proteasome system, which is increasingly recognized as a crucial regulator of synaptic development and learning-dependent synaptic plasticity (Patrick, Omar, Werner, Mitra, & Jarome, Reference Patrick, Omar, Werner, Mitra and Jarome2023). UBXN7 is the only gene in the interval that is implicated as a ‘hub gene’, with a large number of predicted connections to other genes expressed in human cortex (Sefik, Purcell, Walker, Bassell, & Mulle, Reference Sefik, Purcell, Walker, Bassell and Mulle2021). Studies have demonstrated the importance of synaptic strength, synaptic remodeling, and brain connectivity for the development of EF abilities (Fiske & Holmboe, Reference Fiske and Holmboe2019), and members of the DLG gene family are intimately associated with the development of complex cognitive processes, including EF (Nithianantharajah et al., Reference Nithianantharajah, Komiyama, McKechanie, Johnstone, Blackwood, Clair and Grant2013). Studies have linked variation in SZ risk genes to specific, predictable changes in EF based on gene expression and functional connectivity measures (Eisenberg & Berman, Reference Eisenberg and Berman2010). Together, these data suggest that synaptic dysfunction, potentially mediated by DLG1, PAK2, and UBXN7, may contribute to the EF deficits in individuals with 3q29del. Other interval genes may also play a role in these deficits through various mechanisms yet to be fully understood.

EF deficits can have a major impact on day-to-day function, but there are techniques and interventions that can improve an individual's EF abilities. Targeted interventions to improve EF can be applied across the lifespan; studies have shown efficacy in improving EF in children as young as 3–4 years of age (Dowsett & Livesey, Reference Dowsett and Livesey2000; Rueda, Rothbart, McCandliss, Saccomanno, & Posner, Reference Rueda, Rothbart, McCandliss, Saccomanno and Posner2005; Tang, Yang, Leve, & Harold, Reference Tang, Yang, Leve and Harold2012) through adulthood (Franklin & Franklin, Reference Franklin and Franklin2012; Goudreau & Knight, Reference Goudreau and Knight2018; Kramer, Larish, & Strayer, Reference Kramer, Larish and Strayer1995; Parker & Boutelle, Reference Parker and Boutelle2009). Specific interventions have also been designed for clinical populations, such as the Unstuck and On Target Program for children on the autism spectrum (Kenworthy et al., Reference Kenworthy, Anthony, Naiman, Cannon, Wills, Luong-Tran and Wallace2014) and training programs for children and adults with ADHD (Klingberg et al., Reference Klingberg, Fernell, Olesen, Johnson, Gustafsson, Dahlström and Westerberg2005; White & Shah, Reference White and Shah2006). Together, these studies emphasize the malleable nature of EF, and suggest that individuals with 3q29del would benefit from targeted EF interventions from an early age.

While the present study is the first detailed description of EF in individuals with 3q29del, it is not without limitations. The average age of our study subjects is young (mean = 14.50 ± 8.26 years); as such, a majority of individuals have not reached the age at onset for SZ and psychotic disorders. Longitudinal follow up of study participants is needed to determine if EF abilities predict later-onset phenotypes. Additionally, the small sample size of the present study rendered our analyses exploratory, and we were likely underpowered for some comparisons, particularly in the neuroimaging analysis. Ongoing work by our team includes EF measurements in a larger sample of individuals with 3q29del; we will aim to replicate the results of the present study in that cohort. Finally, we were unable to assess the relative effect of race and ethnicity on EF in the current study, as our sample was overwhelmingly white and non-Hispanic. Current and future recruitment efforts will aim to include more underrepresented minorities so that future studies ideally have a more representative study sample.

The present study is the first to describe details of EF in individuals with 3q29del. We identified global deficits in EF, which were consistent between males and females. We found that EF was not correlated with IQ, but was associated with SZ spectrum symptom severity, ADHD diagnosis, and neuropsychiatric and neurodevelopmental multimorbidity; the BRIEF-2 ADHD form accurately discriminated between individuals with 3q29del with and without ADHD. This study, coupled with previous work by our team, emphasizes the central nature of EF to the 3q29del phenotype beyond general intellectual ability, and highlights the need for EF evaluation and interventions for all individuals with 3q29del. The malleable nature of EF means that early intervention in this population will likely yield substantial gains in abilities and improvements in long-term outcomes and ability to function independently.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724002320.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge our study population, the 3q29 deletion community, for their participation and commitment to research.

Author contributions

R. M. P. performed analysis, created figures and tables, and wrote the manuscript. E. S. performed analysis, created figures and tables, wrote the manuscript, and collected data. K. D. and A. E. G.-Y. performed analysis of neuroimaging data. K. A., R. E., R. M. G., K. G., and D. M. N. collected data. C. A. S., C. K., S. P., J. F. C., and T. L. B. evaluated study participants. S. S. led the collection of neuroimaging data and supervised analysis of neuroimaging data. E. F. W. supervised data collection efforts. M. M. M. planned and executed the study. J. G. M. planned and executed the study and was the principal investigator responsible for study coordination.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health grant R01 MH110701 (PI Mulle) and R01 MH118534 (MPI Schultz/Mulle).

Competing interests

C. A. S. reports receiving royalties from Pearson Assessments for the Vineland-3. The remaining authors have no competing interests to disclose.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

*

These authors contributed equally this work.

References

Albert, A. B., Abu-Ramadan, T., Kates, W. R., Fremont, W., & Antshel, K. M. (2018). Childhood executive functioning predicts young adult outcomes in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 24(9), 905916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association, D., & American Psychiatric Association, D. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (Vol. 5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8(2), 7182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, N. C., & Pierson, R. (2008). The role of the cerebellum in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 64(2), 8188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ardila, A., Pineda, D., & Rosselli, M. (2000). Correlation between intelligence test scores and executive function measures. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15(1), 3136. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(98)00159-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baggetta, P., & Alexander, P. A. (2016). Conceptualization and operationalization of executive function. Mind, Brain, and Education, 10(1), 1033.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ballif, B. C., Theisen, A., Coppinger, J., Gowans, G. C., Hersh, J. H., Madan-Khetarpal, S., … Shaffer, L. G. (2008). Expanding the clinical phenotype of the 3q29 microdeletion syndrome and characterization of the reciprocal microduplication. Molecular Cytogenetics, 1, 8. doi: 10.1186/1755-8166-1-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechtel, N., Kobel, M., Penner, I.-K., Klarhöfer, M., Scheffler, K., Opwis, K., & Weber, P. (2009). Decreased fractional anisotropy in the middle cerebellar peduncle in children with epilepsy and/or attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary study. Epilepsy & Behavior, 15(3), 294298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beery, K. E., & Beery, N. A. (2010). The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration (Beery VMI): With supplemental developmental tests of visual perception and motor coordination and stepping stones age norms from birth to age six: Administration, scoring, and teaching manual. Pearson.Google Scholar
Bellebaum, C., & Daum, I. (2007). Cerebellar involvement in executive control. The Cerebellum, 6, 184192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 16411660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Doyle, A. E., Seidman, L. J., Wilens, T. E., Ferrero, F., … Faraone, S. V. (2004). Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72(5), 757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, T. E. (2009). ADD/ADHD and impaired executive function in clinical practice. Current Attention Disorders Reports, 1(1), 3741. doi: 10.1007/s12618-009-0006-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, D. M., & Butler, M. G. (2015). A clinical case report and literature review of the 3q29 microdeletion syndrome. Clinical Dysmorphology, 24(3), 8994. doi: 10.1097/MCD.0000000000000077CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deverett, B., Koay, S. A., Oostland, M., & Wang, S. S. H. (2018). Cerebellar involvement in an evidence-accumulation decision-making task. eLife, 7, e36781. doi: 10.7554/eLife.36781CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dowsett, S. M., & Livesey, D. J. (2000). The development of inhibitory control in preschool children: Effects of ‘executive skills’ training. Developmental Psychobiology, 36(2), 161174. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-2302(200003)36:2<161::aid-dev7>3.0.co;2-03.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, D. P., & Berman, K. F. (2010). Executive function, neural circuitry, and genetic mechanisms in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 258277. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, C. D., Murray, G., & Pearson, L. (1990). Differential ability scales. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Everaert, E., Vorstman, J. A., Selten, I. S., Slieker, M. G., Wijnen, F., Boerma, T. D., & Houben, M. L. (2023). Executive functioning in preschoolers with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome and the impact of congenital heart defects. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 15(1), 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, A., & Holmboe, K. (2019). Neural substrates of early executive function development. Developmental Review, 52, 4262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2019.100866CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franklin, J., & Franklin, A. (2012). The long-term independently assessed benefits of coaching: A controlled 18-month follow-up study of two methods. International Coaching Psychology Review, 7(1), 3338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., Defries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17(2), 172179. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01681.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gioia, G., Isquith, P., Guy, S., & Kenworthy, L. (2015). Behavior rating inventory of executive function, second edition (BRIEF2). Lutz, FL: PAR Inc.Google Scholar
Glassford, M. R., Rosenfeld, J. A., Freedman, A. A., Zwick, M. E., & Mulle, J. G., & Unique Rare Chromosome Disorder Support, G. (2016). Novel features of 3q29 deletion syndrome: Results from the 3q29 registry. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A, 170A(4), 9991006. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.37537CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goudreau, S. B., & Knight, M. (2018). Executive function coaching: Assisting with transitioning from secondary to postsecondary education. Journal of Attention Disorders, 22(4), 379387. doi: 10.1177/1087054715583355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gur, R. E., McDonald-McGinn, D. M., Moore, T. M., Gallagher, R. S., McClellan, E., White, L., … McGinn, D. E. (2023). Psychosis spectrum features, neurocognition and functioning in a longitudinal study of youth with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Psychological Medicine, 53(14), 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hothorn, T., Zeileis, A., Farebrother, R. W., Cummins, C., Millo, G., Mitchell, D., & Zeileis, M. A. (2015). Package ‘lmtest’. Testing linear regression models. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lmtest/lmtest.pdf. Accessed, 6.Google Scholar
Howard, M. A., Elias, G. M., Elias, L. A., Swat, W., & Nicoll, R. A. (2010). The role of SAP97 in synaptic glutamate receptor dynamics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(8), 38053810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Itsara, A., Cooper, G. M., Baker, C., Girirajan, S., Li, J., Absher, D., … Eichler, E. E. (2009). Population analysis of large copy number variants and hotspots of human genetic disease. American Journal of Human Genetics, 84(2), 148161. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2008.12.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendall, K. M., Rees, E., Escott-Price, V., Einon, M., Thomas, R., Hewitt, J., … Kirov, G. (2017). Cognitive performance among carriers of pathogenic copy number variants: Analysis of 152000 UK Biobank subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 82(2), 103110. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.08.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenworthy, L., Anthony, L. G., Naiman, D. Q., Cannon, L., Wills, M. C., Luong-Tran, C., … Wallace, G. L. (2014). Randomized controlled effectiveness trial of executive function intervention for children on the autism spectrum. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(4), 374383. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12161CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khan, M., & Brandenburger, T. (2020). ROCit: Performance Assessment of Binary Classifier with Visualization. R package version 2.1.1. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ROCitGoogle Scholar
Kirov, G., Pocklington, A. J., Holmans, P., Ivanov, D., Ikeda, M., Ruderfer, D., … Owen, M. J. (2012). De novo CNV analysis implicates specific abnormalities of postsynaptic signalling complexes in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry, 17(2), 142153. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.154CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klaiman, C., White, S. P., Saulnier, C., Murphy, M., Burrell, L., Cubells, J., … Mulle, J. G. (2022). A distinct cognitive profile in individuals with 3q29 deletion syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 67(3), 216227. doi: 10.1111/jir.12919.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlström, K., … Westerberg, H. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD – A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(2), 177186. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200502000-00010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koziol, L. F., Budding, D. E., & Chidekel, D. (2012). From movement to thought: Executive function, embodied cognition, and the cerebellum. The Cerebellum, 11(2), 505525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraepelin, E. (1913). Section IX, die endogenen verblödungen. Die dementia praecox. Psychiatrie. Ein Lehrbuch für Studierende und Ärzte, 746749.Google Scholar
Kramer, A. F., Larish, J. F., & Strayer, D. L. (1995). Training for attentional control in dual task settings: A comparison of young and old adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 1(1), 50.Google Scholar
Kreis, P., & Barnier, J.-V. (2009). PAK signalling in neuronal physiology. Cellular Signalling, 21(3), 384393. doi: 10.1016/j.cellsig.2008.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, K.-H., Farrow, T. F., Parks, R. W., Newton, L. D., Mir, N. U., Egleston, P. N., … Woodruff, P. W. (2007). Increased cerebellar vermis white-matter volume in men with schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 41(8), 645651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lysaker, P. H., Warman, D. M., Dimaggio, G., Procacci, M., LaRocco, V. A., Clark, L. K., … Nicolò, G. (2008). Metacognition in schizophrenia: Associations with multiple assessments of executive function. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 196(5), 384389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahone, E. M., & Hoffman, J. (2007). Behavior ratings of executive function among preschoolers with ADHD. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21(4), 569586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marije Boonstra, A., Oosterlaan, J., Sergeant, J. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2005). Executive functioning in adult ADHD: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Medicine, 35(8), 10971108. doi: 10.1017/S003329170500499XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martel, M., Nikolas, M., & Nigg, J. T. (2007). Executive function in adolescents with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(11), 14371444. https://doi.org/10.1097/chi.0b013e31814cf953CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, E., & Wallis, J. (2009). Executive function and higher-order cognition: Definition and neural substrates. Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, 4(99–104).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, T. J., McGlashan, T. H., Rosen, J. L., Cadenhead, K., Cannon, T., Ventura, J., … Woods, S. W. (2003). Prodromal assessment with the structured interview for prodromal syndromes and the scale of prodromal symptoms: Predictive validity, interrater reliability, and training to reliability. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 29(4), 703715. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulle, J. (2015). The 3q29 deletion confers >40-fold increase in risk for schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry, 20(9), 10281029. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.76CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulle, J., Dodd, A., McGrath, J., Wolyniec, P., Mitchell, A., Shetty, A., … Warren, S. (2010). Microdeletions of 3q29 confer high risk for schizophrenia. American Journal of Human Genetics, 87(2), 229236. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.07.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, M. M., Lindsey Burrell, T., Cubells, J. F., Espana, R. A., Gambello, M. J., Goines, K. C. B., … Mulle, J. G. (2018). Study protocol for the Emory 3q29 project: Evaluation of neurodevelopmental, psychiatric, and medical symptoms in 3q29 deletion syndrome. BMC Psychiatry, 18(1), 183. doi: 10.1186/s12888-018-1760-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakagawa, T., Futai, K., Lashuel, H. A., Lo, I., Okamoto, K., Walz, T., … Sheng, M. (2004). Quaternary structure, protein dynamics, and synaptic function of SAP97 controlled by L27 domain interactions. Neuron, 44(3), 453467. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nithianantharajah, J., Komiyama, N. H., McKechanie, A., Johnstone, M., Blackwood, D. H., Clair, D. S., … Grant, S. G. N. (2013). Synaptic scaffold evolution generated components of vertebrate cognitive complexity. Nature Neuroscience, 16(1), 1624. doi: 10.1038/nn.3276CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Hora, K. P., Kushan-Wells, L., Hoftman, G. D., Jalbrzikowski, M., Gur, R. C., Gur, R., … Bearden, C. E. (2023). Distinct neurocognitive profiles and clinical phenotypes associated with copy number variation at the 22q11.2 locus. Autism Research, 16(12), 22472262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orellana, G., & Slachevsky, A. (2013). Executive functioning in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4, 35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, D. R., & Boutelle, K. (2009). Executive function coaching for college students with learning disabilities and ADHD: A new approach for fostering self-determination. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 24(4), 204215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patrick, M. B., Omar, N., Werner, C. T., Mitra, S., & Jarome, T. J. (2023). The ubiquitin–proteasome system and learning-dependent synaptic plasticity – A 10 year update. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 152, 105280. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105280CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Picard, H., Amado, I., Mouchet-Mages, S., Olié, J.-P., & Krebs, M.-O. (2008). The role of the cerebellum in schizophrenia: An update of clinical, cognitive, and functional evidences. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(1), 155172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickup, G. J. (2008). Relationship between theory of mind and executive function in schizophrenia: A systematic review. Psychopathology, 41(4), 206213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollak, R., Murphy, M., Epstein, M., Zwick, M., Klaiman, C., Saulnier, C., … Mulle, J. (2019). Neuropsychiatric phenotypes and a distinct constellation of ASD features in 3q29 deletion syndrome: Results from the 3q29 registry. Molecular Autism, 10(1), 30. doi: 10.1186/s13229-019-0281-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollak, R., Burrell, T., Cubells, J., Klaiman, C., Murphy, M., Saulnier, C., … Mulle, J. (2023a). Adaptive behaviour deficits in individuals with 3q29 deletion syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 68(2), 113127. doi: 10.1111/jir.13094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollak, R., Burrell, T., Cubells, J., Klaiman, C., Murphy, M., Saulnier, C., … Mulle, J. (2023b). Visual-motor integration deficits in 3q29 deletion syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 113. doi: 10.1007/s10803-023-06034-2.Google ScholarPubMed
Pollak, R., Mortillo, M., Murphy, M., & Mulle, J. (2024). Behavioral phenotypes and comorbidity in 3q29 deletion syndrome: Results from the 3q29 registry. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 111. doi: 10.1007/s10803-023-06218-w.Google ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2008). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.r-project.org/Google Scholar
Roth, R. M., & Gioia, G. A. (2005). Behavior rating inventory of executive function-adult version. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 102(41), 1493114936. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506897102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanchez Russo, R., Gambello, M. J., Murphy, M. M., Aberizk, K., Black, E., Burrell, T. L., … Mulle, J. G. (2021). Deep phenotyping in 3q29 deletion syndrome: Recommendations for clinical care. Genetics in Medicine, 23(5), 872880. doi: 10.1038/s41436-020-01053-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanders, S. J., He, X., Willsey, A. J., Ercan-Sencicek, A. G., Samocha, K. E., Cicek, A. E., … State, M. W. (2015). Insights into autism spectrum disorder genomic architecture and biology from 71 risk loci. Neuron, 87(6), 12151233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, C. R., Howrigan, D. P., Merico, D., Thiruvahindrapuram, B., Wu, W., & Greer, D. S., … Schizophrenia Working Groups of the Psychiatric Genomics, C. (2017). Contribution of copy number variants to schizophrenia from a genome-wide study of 41321 subjects. Nature Genetics, 49(1), 2735. doi:10.1038/ng.3725CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmahmann, J. D. (2019). The cerebellum and cognition. Neuroscience letters, 688, 6275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmahmann, J. D., & Sherman, J. C. (1998). The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 121(4), 561579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sefik, E., Purcell, R. H., Walker, E. F., Bassell, G. J., & Mulle, J. G. (2021). Convergent and distributed effects of the 3q29 deletion on the human neural transcriptome. Translational Psychiatry, 11(1), 357. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01435-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sefik, E., Li, Y., Sholar, B., Evans, L., Pincus, J., Ammar, Z., … Mulle, J. G. (2024). Structural deviations of the posterior fossa and the cerebellum and their cognitive links in a neurodevelopmental deletion syndrome. Molecular psychiatry, 117.Google Scholar
Sievert, C., Parmer, C., Hocking, T., Chamberlain, S., Ram, K., Corvellec, M., & Despouy, P. (2017). plotly: Create interactive web graphics via ‘plotly.js’. R package version 4.6.0. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=plotlyGoogle Scholar
Silverstein, M. J., Faraone, S. V., Leon, T. L., Biederman, J., Spencer, T. J., & Adler, L. A. (2020). The relationship between executive function deficits and DSM-5-defined ADHD symptoms. Journal of Attention Disorders, 24(1), 4151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, T., Poterba, T., Curtis, D., Akil, H., Al Eissa, M., Barchas, J. D., … Daly, M. J. (2022). Rare coding variants in ten genes confer substantial risk for schizophrenia. Nature, 604(7906), 509516. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04556-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sparrow, S., Cicchetti, D., & Saulnier, C. (2016). Vineland adaptive behavior scales – Third edition (Vineland-3). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Stefansson, H., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Steinberg, S., Magnusdottir, B., Morgen, K., Arnarsdottir, S., … Stefansson, K. (2014). CNVs conferring risk of autism or schizophrenia affect cognition in controls. Nature, 505(7483), 361366. doi: 10.1038/nature12818CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoodley, C. J., & Schmahmann, J. D. (2010). Evidence for topographic organization in the cerebellum of motor control versus cognitive and affective processing. Cortex, 46(7), 831844. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoodley, C. J., Desmond, J. E., Guell, X., & Schmahmann, J. D. (2020). Functional topography of the human cerebellum revealed by functional neuroimaging studies. In Manto, M., Gruol, D., Schmahmann, J., Koibuchi, N., & Sillitoe, R. (Eds.), Handbook of the cerebellum and cerebellar disorders (pp. 137). Cham: Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
Szatkiewicz, J. P., O'Dushlaine, C., Chen, G., Chambert, K., Moran, J. L., Neale, B. M., … Sullivan, P. F. (2014). Copy number variation in schizophrenia in Sweden. Molecular Psychiatry, 19, 762. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.40CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, A., Delgaty, L., Steward, K., & Bunner, M. (2018). Performance-based measures and behavioral ratings of executive function in diagnosing attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 10, 309316. doi: 10.1007/s12402-018-0256-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, Y. Y., Yang, L., Leve, L. D., & Harold, G. T. (2012). Improving executive function and its neurobiological mechanisms through a mindfulness-based intervention: Advances within the field of developmental neuroscience. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 361366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasi, D., & Volkow, N. D. (2012). Abnormal functional connectivity in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 71(5), 443450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toplak, M. E., Bucciarelli, S. M., Jain, U., & Tannock, R. (2008). Executive functions: Performance-based measures and the behavior rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF) in adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 15(1), 5372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velligan, D., & Bow-Thomas, C. (1999). Executive function in schizophrenia. Paper presented at the Seminars in clinical neuropsychiatry.Google Scholar
Wang, Y., Zeng, C., Li, J., Zhou, Z., Ju, X., Xia, S., … Sun, Z. S. (2018). PAK2 haploinsufficiency results in synaptic cytoskeleton impairment and autism-related behavior. Cell Reports, 24(8), 20292041. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.07.061CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. Bloomington: Pearson.Google Scholar
White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2006). Training attention-switching ability in adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10(1), 4453. doi: 10.1177/1087054705286063CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willatt, L., Cox, J., Barber, J., Cabanas, E. D., Collins, A., Donnai, D., … Raymond, F. L. (2005). 3q29 microdeletion syndrome: Clinical and molecular characterization of a new syndrome. American Journal of Human Genetics, 77(1), 154160. doi: 10.1086/431653CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wobrock, T., Ecker, U. K., Scherk, H., Schneider-Axmann, T., Falkai, P., & Gruber, O. (2009). Cognitive impairment of executive function as a core symptom of schizophrenia. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 10(4–2), 442451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeganeh-Doost, P., Gruber, O., Falkai, P., & Schmitt, A. (2011). The role of the cerebellum in schizophrenia: From cognition to molecular pathways. Clinics, 66, 7177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeileis, A., Lumley, T., Berger, S., Graham, N., & Zeileis, M. A. (2019). Package ‘sandwich’. R package version, 2.5-1.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic information for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32)

Figure 1

Figure 1. (a) Distribution of T scores on the BRIEF GEC and BRIEF subscales for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32). The black dashed line indicates the population mean; the red dashed line indicates the clinical cutoff. Subscales are ordered left to right by decreasing mean severity. (b) Pie chart showing the proportion of study participants with 3q29del (n = 32) scoring in the clinical range (T scores ⩾ 70) on one or more BRIEF scales, expanded to show the proportion of participants scoring in the clinical range on 1 to 9 BRIEF scales. 3q29del, 3q29 deletion syndrome; BRIEF, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; GEC, global executive composite.

Figure 2

Figure 2. (a) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and composite IQ for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32). (b) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and non-verbal IQ for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32). (c) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and verbal IQ for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32). (d) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and SIPS Positive Symptom Ratings for study participants with 3q29del (n = 23). (e) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and SIPS Negative Symptom Ratings for study participants with 3q29del (n = 23). (f) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and SIPS Disorganization Symptom Ratings for study participants with 3q29del (n = 23). 3q29del, 3q29 deletion syndrome; BRIEF, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; GEC, global executive composite; SIPS, Structured Interview for Psychosis-Risk Syndromes.

Figure 3

Figure 3. (a) Distribution of BRIEF GEC T scores for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32) with and without specific neurodevelopmental or psychiatric diagnoses. (b) Distribution of BRIEF GEC T scores for study participants with 3q29del (n = 32) with an increasing number of multimorbid neurodevelopmental and psychiatric diagnoses. (c) Receiver operating characteristic curve showing the ability of the BRIEF-2 ADHD form to correctly classify study participants with 3q29del (n = 26) with and without a diagnosis of ADHD. 3q29del, 3q29 deletion syndrome; BRIEF, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; GEC, global executive composite; ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

Figure 4

Figure 4. (a) Diagram illustrating the cerebellum with a representative coronal image from a T1-weighted MRI showing cerebellar white matter and cerebellar cortex. (b) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and total cerebellar volume for study participants with 3q29del (n = 23). (c) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and cerebellar white matter volume for study participants with 3q29del (n = 23). (d) Correlation between BRIEF GEC T scores and cerebellar cortical (grey matter) volume for study participants with 3q29del (n = 23). 3q29del, 3q29 deletion syndrome; BRIEF, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; GEC, global executive composite.

Supplementary material: File

Pollak et al. supplementary material 1

Pollak et al. supplementary material
Download Pollak et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 3.7 MB
Supplementary material: File

Pollak et al. supplementary material 2

Pollak et al. supplementary material
Download Pollak et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 14.1 KB